Article published in:
Visually Situated Language Comprehension
Edited by Pia Knoeferle, Pirita Pyykkönen-Klauck and Matthew W. Crocker
[Advances in Consciousness Research 93] 2016
► pp. 151184
Cited by

Cited by 1 other publications

Gračanin-Yuksek, Martina, Sol Lago, Duygu Fatma Şafak, Orhan Demir & Bilal Kırkıcı
2017. The Interaction of Contextual and Syntactic Information in the Processing of Turkish Anaphors. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 46:6  pp. 1397 ff. Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 18 october 2021. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.

References

References

Allopenna, P.D., Magnuson, J.S., & Tanenhaus, M.K.
(1998) Tracking the time course of spoken word recognition: Evidence for continuous mapping models. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 419-439. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Almor, A.
(1999) Noun-phrase anaphora and focus: The informational load hypothesis. Psychological Review, 106(4), 748-765. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Altmann, G.T.M. & Kamide, Y.
1999Incremental interpretation at verbs: Restricting the domain of subsequent reference. Cognition, 73, 247-264. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Altmann, G.T.M.
(2004) Language-mediated eye movements in the absence of a visual world: The ‘blank screen paradigm’. Cognition, 93, 79-87 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ariel, M.
(1990) Accessing NP antecedents. London: Routledge, Croom Helm.Google Scholar
(2001) Accessibility theory: An overview. In T. Sanders, J. Schilperoord, & W. Spooren (Eds.), Text representation, linguistic and psycholinguistic aspects (pp. 29-87). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Arnold, J.E., & Tanenhaus, M.K.
(2011) Disfluency effects in comprehension: How new information can become accessible. In E. Gibson & N. Perlmutter (Eds.), The processing and acquisition of reference. MIT Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Arnold, J.E., Eisenband, J.G., Brown-Schmidt, S., & Trueswell, J.C.
(2000) The immediate use of gender information: Eyetracking evidence of the time-course of pronoun resolution. Cognition, 76, B13-B26. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Arnold, J.E., Wasow, T., Losongco, A., & Ginstrom, R.
(2000) Heaviness vs. newness: The effects of structural complexity and discourse status on constituent ordering. Language, 76, 28-55 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Arnold, J.E., Brown-Schmidt, S., & Trueswell, J.C.
(2007) Children's use of gender and order-of-mention during pronoun comprehension. Language and Cognitive Processes, 22(4), 527-565. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Beckman, M.E.
(1996) The parsing of prosody. Language and Cognitive Processes, 11, 17-67. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Beckman, M.E., & Ayers, G.M.
(1997) Guidelines for ToBI labelling, vers 3.0. Ohio State University.Google Scholar
Birch, S.L., Albrecht, J.E., & Myers, J.L.
(2000) Syntactic focusing structures influence discourse processing. Discourse Processes, 30, 285-304. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Birner, B., & Ward, G.
(1998) Information status and noncanonical word order in English. Amsterdam: John Benjamins CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2009) Information structure and syntactic structure. Language and Linguistics Compass, 3/4, 1167-1187. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bolinger, D.
(1986) Intonation and its parts: Melody in spoken English. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Brennan, S.E., Friedman, M.A., & Pollard, C.J.
(1987) A centering approach to pronouns. In Proceedings of the 25th annual meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics (pp. 155-162). Stanford, CA: Association for Computational Linguistics. Crossref
Brown-Schmidt, S.
(2005) Language processing in conversation. Ph.D. Dissertation, University of Rochester.Google Scholar
Brown-Schmidt, S., Byron, D.K., & Tanenhaus, M.
(2005) Beyond salience: Interpretation of personal and demonstrative pronouns. Journal of Memory and Language, 53, 292-313. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Büring, D.
(1997) The meaning of topic and focus – The 59th Street Bridge accent. London: Routledge. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chafe, W.L.
(1974) Language and consciousness. Language, 50, 111-133. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1976) Givenness, contrastiveness, definiteness, subjects, topics, and point of view. In C. Li (Ed.), Subject and topic (pp. 25-55). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Chen, A., Den Os, E., & De Ruiter, J.P.
(2007) Pitch accent type matters for online processing of information status: Evidence from natural and synthetic speech. The Linguistic Review, 24(2), 317-344. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N.
(1971) Deep structure, surface structure, and semantic interpretation. In D. Steinberg & L. Jacobovits (Eds.), Semantics (pp. 183-216). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, H.H., & Clark, E.V.
(1977) Psychology and language. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Google Scholar
Clark, H.H., & Haviland, S.
(1977) Comprehension and the given-new contract. In R. Freedle (Ed.), Discourse production and comprehension (pp. 1-40). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Clifton, C., & Frazier, L.
(2004) Should given information come before new? Yes and no. Memory and Cognition, 32(6), 886-895. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Colonna, S., Schimke, S., & Hemforth, B.
(2010) Le rôle de la structure informationnelle dans l’interprétation d’une anaphore pronominale inter-phrastique. In F. Neveu at al. (Eds.), Congrès Mondial de Linguistique Française, 1489-1499.Google Scholar
Cooper, R.M.
(1974) The control of eye fixation by the meaning of spoken language: A new methodology for the real-time investigation of speech perception, memory, and language processing. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 84-107. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cowles, H.W.
(2003) Processing information structure: Evidence from comprehension and production. Ph.D. dissertation, UCSD.Google Scholar
Cowles, H.W., Walenski, M., & Kluender, R.
(2007) Linguistic and cognitive prominence in anaphor resolution: Topic, constrastive focus and pronouns. Topoi, 26, 3-18. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Crawley, R.J., & Stevenson, R.J.
(1990) Reference in single sentences and in texts. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 19(3), 191-210. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cutler, A., & Fodor, J.
(1979) Semantic focus and sentence comprehension. Cognition, 7, 49-59 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dahan, D., Tanenhaus, M.K., & Chambers, C.G.
(2002) Accent and reference resolution in spoken-language comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 47, 292-314. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Delin, J., & Oberlander, J.
(1995) Syntactic constraints on discourse structure: The case of it-clefts. Linguistics, 33, 3. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dufter, A.
(2009) Clefting and discourse organization: Comparing Germanic and Romance. In A. Dufter & D. Jacob (Eds.), Focus and background in romance languages (Studies in Language Companion Series 112). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ellert, M., Järvikivi, J., & Roberts, L.
(2009) Information structure guides gaze behavior: Processing the German subject pronouns er and der in spoken discourse. Poster presented at 15th Annual Conference on Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing , Barcelona, Spain.
Engelhardt, P.E., Ferreira, F., & Patsenko, E.G.
(2010) Pupillometry reveals processing load during spoken language comprehension. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 639-645. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Féry, C. Skopeteas, S., & Hörnig, R.
(2010) Cross-linguistic comparison of prosody, syntax and information structure in a production experiment on localizing expressions. Transactions of the Philological Society, 108(3), 329-351 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Foraker, S., & McElree, B.
(2007) The role of prominence in pronoun resolution: Active versus passive representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 56(3), 357-383 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Garnham, A.
(2001) Mental models and the interpretation of anaphora. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Garvey, C., & Caramazza, A.
(1974) Implicit causality in verbs. Linguistic Inquiry, 5, 459-464.Google Scholar
Givón, T.
(1983) Topic continuity in discourse: A quantitative cross-language study. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gleitman, L., January, D., Nappa, R. & Trueswell, J.
(2007) On the give and take between event apprehension and utterance formulation. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 544-569. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gordon, P.C., Grosz, B.J., & Gilliom, L.A.
(1993) Pronouns, names, and the centering o attention in discourse. Cognitive Science, 17, 311-347. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Green, M. & Jaggar, P.
(2003) ‘Ex-situ and In-situ Focus in Hausa: Syntax, semantics and discourse.’ In Lecarme, J (Ed.), Research in afroasiatic grammar II. [CILT 241]. (pp. 187-213). Amsterdam: John Benjamins: CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Griffin, Z.M., & Bock, J.K.
(2000) What the eyes say about speaking. Psychological Science, 11, 274-279. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gundel, J.K.
(1974) The role of topic and comment in linguistic theory. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Texas at Austin.Google Scholar
(1988) Universals of topic-comment structure. In M. Hammond, E. Moravczik, & J. Wirth (Eds.), Studies in syntactic typology (pp. 209-239). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gundel, J.K., & Fretheim, T.
(2004) Topic and focus. In G. Ward & L. Horn (Eds.), Handbook of pragmatics (Blackwell Handbooks in Linguistics). (pp.175-196). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Gundel, J.K., Hedberg, N., & Zacharski, R.
(1993) Cognitive status and the form of referring expressions in discourse. Language, 69, 274-307. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gussenhoven, C.
(1983) Focus, mode, and nucleus. Journal of Linguistics, 19, 377-417. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Halliday, M.
(1967) Notes on transitivity and theme in English. Part 1 and 2. Journal of Linguistics, 3, 37-81; 199-244. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hartshorne, Joshua K., Nappa, R., & Snedeker, J.
in press). Development of the first-mention bias. Journal of Child Language.
Haviland, S.E., & Clark, H.H.
(1974) What's new? Acquiring new information as a process in comprehension. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 512-521. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hedberg, N.
(1990) Discourse pragmatics and cleft sentences in English. Ph.D dissertation, Universitiy of Minnesota.Google Scholar
(2000) The referential status of clefts. Language, 76, 891-920. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hornby, P.A.
(1974) Surface structure and presupposition. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 530-538. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Huang, Y., & Snedeker, J.
(2009) Online interpretation of scalar quantifiers: Insight into the semantics–pragmatics interface. Cognitive Psychology, 58(3), 376-415. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hwang, Heeju, & Kaiser, Elsi
(2014) The role of the verb in grammatical function assignment in English and Korean. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 1363-1376. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jackendoff, R.
(1972) Semantic interpretation in generative grammar. Cambridge, MA: MIT PressGoogle Scholar
Järvikivi, J., Van Gompel, R.P.G., Bertram, R., & Hyönä, J.
(2005) Ambiguous pronoun resolution: Contrasting the first-mention and subject preference accounts. Psychological Science, 16, 260-264. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kaiser, E., & Trueswell, J.C.
(2008) Interpreting pronouns and demonstratives in Finnish: Evidence for a form-specific approach to reference resolution. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23(5), 709-748. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2004) The role of discourse context in the processing of a flexible word-order language. Cognition, 94(2), 113-147. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kaiser, E.
(2009) Effects of anaphoric dependencies and semantic representations on pronoun interpretation. In S.L. Devi, A. Branco, & R. Mitkov (Eds.), Anaphora processing and applications (pp.121-130). Heidelberg: Springer. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kaiser, E., Runner, J.T., Sussman, R.S., & Tanenhaus. M.K.
(2009) Structural and semantic constraints on the resolution of pronouns and reflexives. Cognition, 112, 55-80. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kaiser, Elsi
(2011a) Focusing on pronouns: Consequences of subjecthood, pronominalisation, and contrastive focus. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26, 1625-1666. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2011b) Salience and contrast effects in reference resolution: The interpretation of Dutch pronouns and demonstratives, Language and Cognitive Processes, 26, 1587-1624. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kamide, Y., Altmann, G.T.M., & Haywood, S.
(2003) The time-course of prediction in incremental sentence processing: Evidence from anticipatory eye-movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 133-59. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kehler, A.
(2002) Coherence, reference, and the theory of grammar. Stanford: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Kehler, A., Kertz, L., Rohde, H., & Elman, J.
(2008) Coherence and coreference revisited. Journal of Semantics (Special Issue on Processing Meaning), 25(1), 1-44.Google Scholar
Kiss, K.E.
(1998) Identificational focus versus information focus. Language, 74, 245-273. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Knoeferle, P., Crocker, M.W., Scheepers, C., & Pickering, M.J.
(2005) The influence of the immediate visual context on incremental thematic role assignment: Evidence from eye-movements in depicted events. Cognition, 95, 95-127. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Koornneef, A.W., & Van Berkum, J.J.A.
(2006) On the use of verb-based implicit causality in sentence comprehension: Evidence from self-paced reading and eye tracking. Journal of Memory and Language, 54, 445-465. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ladd, D.R.
(1996) Intonational phonology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lambrecht, K.
(1994) Information structure and sentence form: Topic, focus, and the mental representation of discourse referents. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2001) A framework for the analysis of cleft constructions. Linguistics, 39, 463-516. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Magnuson, J.S., Dixon, J.A., Tanenhaus, M.K., & Aslin, R.N.
(2007) The dynamics of lexical competition during spoken word recognition. Cognitive Science, 31, 133-156. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Myhill, J.
(1992) Typological discourse analysis. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Pierrehumbert, J., & Hirschberg, J.
(1990) The meaning of intonational contours in the interpretation of discourse. In P.R. Cohen, J. Morgan, & M.E. Pollack (Eds.), Intentions in communication (pp. 271-311). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Prince, E.F.
(1978) A comparison of WH-clefts and IT-clefts in discourse. Language, 54, 883-906. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1992) The ZPG letter: Subjects, definiteness, and information status. In S. Thompson & W. Mann (Eds.), Discourse description: Diverse analyses of a fund-raising text (pp. 295-325). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pyykkönen, P., & Järvikivi, J.
(2010) Activation and persistence of implicit causality information in spoken language comprehension. Experimental Psychology, 57 (1), 5-16. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pyykkönen, P., Matthews, D., & Järvikivi, J.
(2010) Three-year-olds are sensitive to semantic prominence during online language comprehension: A visual world study of pronoun resolution. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25, 115-129. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Reinhart, T.
(1982) Pragmatics and linguistics: An analysis of sentence topics. University of Indiana Linguistics Club. (also Philosophica 1981, 27, 53-94).Google Scholar
Rochemont, M.
(1986) Focus in generative grammar. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rooth, M.
(1992) A Theory of focus interpretation. Natural Language Semantics, 1, 75-116. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schwarzschild, R.
(1999) GIVENness, Avoid F and other constraints on the placement of focus. Natural Language Semantics, 7, 141-177. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sedivy, J., Tanenhaus, M., Chambers, C., & Carlson, G.
(1999) Achieving incremental semantic interpretation through contextual representation. Cognition, 71, 109-147. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sekerina, I.E. & Trueswell, J.C.
(2012) Interactive processing of contrastive expressions by Russian children. First Language 32: 63-87. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Selkirk, E.O.
(1995) Sentence prosody: Intonation, stress, and phrasing. In J.A. Goldsmith (Ed.), The handbook of phonological theory (pp. 550-569). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Sgall, P., & Hajicova, W.E.
(1977) Focus on focus. The Prague Bulletin of Mathematical Linguistics, 28: 5-54.Google Scholar
Song, H., & Fisher, C.
(2005) Who’s ‘she’? Discourse prominence influences preschoolers comprehension of pronouns. Journal of Memory and Language, 52, 29-57. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Spivey, M.J., Richardson, D.C., & Fitneva, S.A.
(2004) Thinking outside the brain: Spatial indices to visual and linguistic Information. In J. Henderson & F. Ferreira (Eds.), Interfacing language, vision, and action (pp. 161-190). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Steedman, M.
(2000) Information structure and the syntax–phonology interface. Linguistic Inquiry, 31, 649-689. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Strube, M., & Hahn, U.
(1996) Functional centering. In Proceedings of the 34th annual meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics (pp. 270-277), Santa Cruz, CA. Crossref
(1999) Functional centering: Grounding referential coherence in information structure. Computational Linguistics, 25(3), 309-344.Google Scholar
Sturt, P., Sanford, A.J., Stewart, A., & Dawydiak, E.
(2004) Linguistic focus and good-enough representations: An application of the change-detection paradigm. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 11, 882-888. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tanenhaus, M.K.
(2007) Spoken language comprehension: insights from eye movements. In G. Gaskell (Ed.), Oxford handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 309-326). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tanenhaus, M.K., Spivey-Knowlton, M., Eberhard, K.M., & Sedivy, J.C.
(1995) Integration of visual and linguistic information in spoken language comprehension. Science, 268, 1632-1634. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tavano, E., & Kaiser, E.
(2008) Effects of stress and coherence on pronoun interpretation. Poster presented at the 21st Annual CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing , University of North Carolina-Chapel Hill.
Vallduví, E., & Vilkuna, M.
(1998) On rheme and kontrast. In P. Culicover & M. Louise (Eds.), The limits of syntax. Syntax and semantics 29 (pp. 79-108). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Vallduvi. E.
(1990) The information component. Ph.D dissertation, University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Van de Velde, M., Meyer, A.S., & Konopka, A.E.
(2014) Message formulation and structural assembly: Describing "easy" and "hard" events with preferred and dispreferred syntactic structures. Journal of Memory and Language, 71(1), 124-144. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Venditti, J.J., Stone, M., Nanda, P., & Tepper, P.
(2001) Discourse constraints on the interpretation of nuclear-accented pronouns. In Proceedings of the 2002 International Conference on Speech Prosody , Aix-en-Provence, France.
Vilkuna, M.
(1989) Free word order in finnish: Its syntax and discourse functions. Helsinki: Finnish Literature Society.Google Scholar
Ward, G.
(1985) The semantics and pragmatics of preposing. Ph.D dissertation, University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Ward, P., & Sturt, P.
(2007) Linguistic focus and memory: An eye-movement study. Memory and Cognition, 35, 73-86. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Weber, A., Braun, B., & Crocker, M.W.
(2006) Finding referents in time: Eye-tracking evidence for the role of contrastive accents. Language and Speech, 49, 367-392. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Weber, A., Grice, M., & Crocker, M.W.
(2006) The role of prosody in the interpretation of structural ambiguities: A study of anticipatory eyemovements. Cognition, 99, B63-B72. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wilson. F.
(2009) Processing at the syntax-discourse interface in second language acquisition. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Edinburgh.Google Scholar
Yee, E., Heller, D., & Sedivy, J.C.
(2009) On the relationship between eye-movements and activation: Active vs. passive tasks during ambiguous pronoun resolution. Poster presented at the 22nd Annual CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing .
Zimmer, H.D., & Engelkamp, J.
(1981) The given-new structure of cleft sentences and their influence on picture viewing. Psychological Research, 43, 375-389. CrossrefGoogle Scholar