Chapter published in:
How the Brain Got Language – Towards a New Road Map
Edited by Michael A. Arbib
[Benjamins Current Topics 112] 2020
► pp. 352369
References

References

Alme, C. B., Miao, C., Jezek, K., Treves, A., Moser, E. I. & Moser, M. -B.
(2014) Place cells in the hippocampus: Eleven maps for eleven rooms. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 111, 18428–18435. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Babb, S. J. & Crystal, J. D.
(2005) Discrimination of what, when, and where: Implications for episodic-like memory in rats. Learning & Motivation, 36, 177–189. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Barnard, A.
(2013) Cognitive and social aspects of language origins. In Lefebvre, C., Comrie, B. &. Cohen, H. (Eds.), New perspectives on the origins of language (pp. 53–71). Amesterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Beran, M. J.
(2015) Animal memory: chimpanzees anticipate what comes next in short movies. Current Biology, 25, R827–R844 Crossref[ p. 365 ]Google Scholar
Beran, M. J., Perdue, B. M., Bramlett, J. L., Menzel, C. R. & Evans, T. A.
(2012) Prospective memory in a language-trained chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). Learning & Motivation, 43, 192–199. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bickerton, D.
(1990) Language and species. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2014) More than nature needs: Language, mind, and evolution. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bischof-Kohler, D.
(1985) Zur phyogenese menschlicher motivation [On the phylogeny of human motivation]. In L. H. Eckensberger & E. D. Lantermann (Eds.), Emotion und reflexivitut (pp. 3–47). Vienna: Urban & Schwarzenberg.Google Scholar
Bonnici, H. M., Chadwick, M. J., Lutti, A., Hassabis, D., Weiskopf, N. & Maguire, E. A.
(2012) Detecting representations of recent and remote autobiographical memories in vmPFC and hippocampus. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 16, 982–16, 991.Google Scholar
Buzsáki, G.
(1989) Two-stage model of memory trace formation: A role for “noisy” brain states. Neuroscience, 31, 551–570. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Call, J. & Tomasello, M.
(2008) Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends in Cognitive. Sciences, 12, 187–192. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N.
(2010) Some simple evo devo theses: How true might they be for language? In R. K. Larson, Déprez, V. & Yamakido, H. (Eds), The evolution of human language (pp. 45–62). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Clayton, N. S., Bussey, T. J. & Dickinson, A.
(2003) Can animals recall the past and plan for the future? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 685–691.Google Scholar
Corballis, M. C.
(2011) The recursive mind: The origins of human language, thought, and civilization. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
(2013) Mental time travel: A case for evolutionary continuity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17, 5–6. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2017) Language evolution: A changing perspective. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 27, 229–236. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Corkin, S.
(2013) Permanent present tense: The man with no memory, and what he taught the world. London: Allen Lane.Google Scholar
Covington, N. V. & Duff, M. C.
(2016) Expanding the language network: contributions from the hippocampus. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 20, 869–870. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Darwin, C.
(1871) The descent of man and selection in relation to sex, 2nd edition. London: John Murray. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dediu, D. & Levinson, S. C.
(2013) On the antiquity of language: The reinterpretation of Neandertal linguistic capacities and its consequences. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, article 397. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Derdikman, D. & Moser, M. B.
(2010) A dual role for hippocampal replay. Neuron, 65, 582–584. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Deuker, L., Bellmund, J. L. S., Schröder, T. N. & Doeller, C. F.
(2016) An event map of memory space in the hippocampus. eLife, 5, e16534. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
De Waal, F. B. M.
(2012) The antiquity of empathy. Science, 336, 874–876. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Donald, M.
(1991) Origins of the modern mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Dor, D.
(2015) The instruction of imagination: Language as a social communication technology. New York: Oxford University Press. Crossref[ p. 366 ]Google Scholar
Duff, M. C. & Brown-Schmidt, S.
(2012) The hippocampus and the flexible use and processing of language. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, article 69. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Emery, N. & Clayton, N.
(2004) The mentality of crows: convergent evolution of intelligence in corvids and apes. Science, 306, 1903–1907. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fauconnier, G.
(2003) Cognitive linguistics. In Nadel, L. (Ed.) Encyclopedia of cognitive science (pp. 539–543). London: Nature Publishing Group.Google Scholar
Ferkin, M. H., Combs, A., del Barco-Trillo, J., Pierce, A. A. & Franklin, S.
(2008) Meadow voles, Microtus pennsylvanicus, have the capacity to recall the “what”, “where”, and “when” of a single past event. Animal Cognition, 11, 147–159. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Foster, D. J. & Wilson, M. A.
(2006) Reverse replay of behavioural sequences in hippocampal place cells during the awake state. Nature, 440, 680–683. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gärdenförs, P.
(2014) The evolution of sentential structure. Humana.Mente Journal of Philosophical Studies, 27, 79–97.Google Scholar
Gärdenförs, P. & Osvath, M.
(2010) Prospection as a cognitive precursor to symbolic communication. In R. Larson, V. Déprez, & H. Yamakido (Eds.), Evolution of language: Biolinguistic approaches (pp. 103–114). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gibson, B., Wilkinson, M. & Kelly, D.
(2012) Let the pigeon drive the bus: pigeons can plan future routes in a room. Animal Cognition, 15, 379–391. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gordon, B. A., Shelton, J. T., Bugg, J. M., McDaniel, M. A. & Head, D.
(2011) Structural correlates of prospective memory. Neuropsychologia, 49, 3795–3800. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grice, H. P.
(1989) Studies in the ways of words. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Guazzelli, A., Corbacho, F. J., Bota, M., & Arbib, M. A.
(1998) Affordances, motivation, and the world graph theory. Adaptive Behavior, 6, 435–471. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hafting, T., Fyhn, M., Molden, S., Moser, M. -B., & Moser, E. I.
(2005) Microstructure of a spatial map in the entorhinal cortex. Nature, 436, 801–806. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hassabis, D., Kumaran, D. & Maguire, E. A.
(2007) Using imagination to understand the neural basis of episodic memory. Journal of Neuroscience. 27, 14365–1437. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hasselmo, M. E.
(2009) A model of episodic memory: Mental time travel along encoded trajectories using grid cells. Neurobiology of Learning & Memory, 92, 559–573. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hockett, C. F.
(1960) The origins of speech. Scientific American, 203(3), 88–96. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hobaiter, C. & Byrne, R. W.
(2011) Serial gesturing by wild chimpanzees: Its nature and function for communication. Animal Cognition, 14, 827–838. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, J. K., Haywood, A., Mithen, S. J., Sellwood, B. W. & Valdes, P. J.
(2007) Investigating early hominin dispersal patterns: developing a framework for climate data integration. Journal of Human Evolution, 53, 465–474. Crossref[ p. 367 ]Google Scholar
Intraub, H. & Richardson, M.
(1989) Wide-angle memories of close-up scenes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 15, 179–187.Google Scholar
Jacobs, J., Weidemann, C. T., Miller, J. F., Solway, A., Burke, J. F., Wei, X. -X.Kahana, M. J.
(2013) Direct recordings of grid-like neuronal activity in human spatial navigation. Nature Neuroscience, 16, 1188–1190. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Janmaat, K. R. L., Polansky, L., Ban, S. D. & Boesch, C.
(2014) Wild chimpanzees plan their breakfast time, type, and location. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA,. 111, 16343–16348. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Johansson, S.
(2013) The talking Neandertals: What do fossils, genetics, and archeology say? Biolinguistics, 7, 35–74.Google Scholar
Kabadayi, C. & Osvath, M.
(2017) Ravens parallel great apes in flexible planning for tool-use and bartering. Science, 357, 202-204. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kaminski, J., Call, J. & Fischer, J.
(2004) Word learning in the domestic dog: evidence for ‘fast mapping’. Science 304, 1682–1683. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kano, F., & Hirata, S.
(2015) Great apes make anticipatory looks based on long-term memory of single events. Current Biology, 25, 2513–2517, CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Klein, S. B., Robertson, T. E., & Delton, A. W.
(2010) Facing the future: Memory as an evolved system for planning future acts. Memory & Cognition, 38, 13–22. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Köhler, W.
(1925) The mentality of apes. New York: Routledge & Kegan Paul. (Originally published in German in 1917).Google Scholar
Kundera, M.
(2002) Ignorance. New York: HarperCollins.Google Scholar
Lieblich, I., & Arbib, M. A.
(1982) Multiple representations of space underlying behavior. The Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 5, 627–659. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Loftus, E. & Ketcham, K.
(1994) The myth of repressed memory. New York: St. Martin’s Press.Google Scholar
Maguire, E. A., Intraub, H. & Mullally, S. L.
(2016) Scenes, spaces, and memory traces: What does the hippocampus do? The Neuroscientist, 22, 432–439. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Martin, V. C., Schacter, D. L., Corballis, M. C. & Addis, D. R.
(2011) A role for the hippocampus in encoding simulations of future events. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 13858–13863. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Menzel, C. R., Savage-Rumbaugh, S. & Menzel, E. W., Jr.
(2002) Bonobo (Pan paniscus) spatial memory and communication in a 20-hectare forest. International Journal of Primatology, 23, 601–619. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Miller, J. F., Neufang, M., Solway, A., Brandt, A., Trippel, M., Mader, I.Schulze-Bonhage, A.
(2013) Neural activity in human hippocampal formation reveals the spatial context of retrieved memories. Science, 342, 1111–1114. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Moser, M. B., Rowland, D. C. & Moser, E. I.
(2015) Place cells, grid cells, and memory. Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology. 7, a021808. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
O’Keefe, J. & Nadel, N.
(1978) The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Osvath, M. & Karvonen, E.
(2012) Spontaneous innovation for future deception in a male chimpanzee. PLoS ONE 7, e36782. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Patterson, F. G. P. & Gordon, W.
(2001) Twenty-seven years of Project Koko and Michael.” In Galdikas, B., Briggs, N. E., Sheeran, L. K. & Goodall, J. (Eds.), All apes great and small, Vol. 1: African Apes (pp. 165–176). New York: Kluver.[ p. 368 ]Google Scholar
Penn, D. C., Holyoak, K. J. & Povinelli, D. J.
(2008) Darwin’s mistake: Explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds. Behavioral & Brain Sciences. 31, 108–178. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pfeiffer, B. E. & Foster, D. J.
(2013) Hippocampal place-cell sequences depict future paths to remembered goals. Nature, 497, 74–79. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Piai, V., Anderson, K. L., Lin, J. J., Dewar, C., Parvizi, J., Dronkers, N. F., & Knight, R. T.
(2016) Direct brain recordings reveal hippocampal rhythm underpinnings of language processing. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113, 11366–11371. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pilley, J. W. & Reid, A. K.
(2011) Border collie comprehends object names as verbal referents. Behavioral Processes, 86, 184–195. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S. & Jackendoff, R.
(2005) The faculty of language: What’s special about it? Cognition, 95, 201–236. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Premack, D.
(2007) Human and animal cognition: Continuity and discontinuity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 104, 13861–13867. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Premack, D. & Woodruff, G.
(1978) Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 4, 515–526. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Raffaele, P.
(2006) Speaking bonobo. Smithsonian Magazine, November 2006. Online at: http://​www​.smithsonianmag​.com​/science​-nature​/speakingbonobo​.html.
Roberts, W. A.
(2002) Are animals stuck in time? Psychological Bulletin, 128, 473–489. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Roediger, H. L. & McDermott, K. B.
(1995) Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 24(4), 803–814Google Scholar
Rugg, M. D. & Vilberg, K. L.
(2013) Brain networks underlying episodic memory retrieval. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23, 255–260. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Russon, A. & Andrews, K.
(2001) Orangutan pantomime: Elaborating the message. Biology Letters, 7, 627–630. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schacter, D. L.
(2012) Adaptive constructive processes and the future of memory. American Psychologist, 67, 603–613. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Scott-Phillips, T.
(2015) Speaking our minds: Why human communication is different, and how language evolved to make it special. Basingstoke, UK: Palgrave Macmillan. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sebeok, T. A. & Rosenthal, R.
(Eds.) (1981) The Clever Hans phenomenon: Communication with horses, whales, apes, and people. New York: New York Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
Seyfarth, R. M. & Cheney, D. L.
(2017) Precursors to language: Social cognition and pragmatic inference in primates. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24, 79–84. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Squire, L. R.
(2004) Memory systems of the brain: A brief history and current perspective. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 82. 171–177. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stout, D. & Chaminade, T.
(2012) Stone tools, language and the brain in human evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 367, 75–87. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stout, D. & Hecht, E. E.
(2017) Evolutionary neuroscience of cumulative culture. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114, 7861–7868 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Suddendorf, T.
(2013) Mental time travel: continuities and discontinuities. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17, 151–152. Crossref[ p. 369 ]Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T. & Corballis, M. C.
(1997) Mental time travel and the evolution of the human mind. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 123, 133–167.Google Scholar
(2007) The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 299–351. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2010) Behavioural evidence for mental time travel in nonhuman animals. Behavioural Brain Research, 215, 292–298. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Timmermann, A. & Friedrich, T.
(2016) Late Pleistocene climate drivers of early human migration. Nature, 538, 92–95. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, A. H., Hunt, G. R., Holzhaider, J. C. & Gray, R. D.
(2007) Spontaneous metatool use by New Caledonian crows. Current Biology, 17, 1504–1507 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tooby, J., and DeVore, I.
(1987) The reconstruction of hominid evolution through strategic modeling. In W. G. Kinzey (Ed.), The evolution of human behavior: Primate models (pp. 183–227). Albany, NY: SUNY Press.Google Scholar
Tulving, E.
(2002) Episodic memory: From mind to brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 1–15. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Venkataraman, V. V., Kraft, T. S., Dominy, N. J., Endicott, K. M.
(2017) Hunter-gatherer residential mobility and the marginal value of rainforest patches. Proceedings of the National academy of Sciences, 114, 3097–3102. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wearing, D.
(2005) Forever today. New York: Doubleday.Google Scholar
Wilson, A. G., Pizzo, M. J. & Crystal, J. D.
(2013) Event-based prospective memory in the rat. Current Biology. 23, 1089–1093. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Zinkivskay, A., Nazir, F. & Smulders, T. V.
(2009) What–Where–When memory in magpies (Pica pica). Animal Cognition, 12, 119–125 CrossrefGoogle Scholar