Part of
Second Language Acquisition Theory: The legacy of Professor Michael H. Long
Edited by Alessandro G. Benati and John W. Schwieter
[Bilingual Processing and Acquisition 14] 2022
► pp. 101112
References
Ajemian, R., D’Ausilio, A., Moormand, E., & Bizzi, A.
(2013) A theory for how sensorimotor skills are learned and retained in noisy and nonstationary neural circuits. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (PNAS), 110(52) E5078–E5087. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bizzi, E., & Ajemian, R.
(2015) A hard scientific quest: Understanding voluntary movements. Daedalus, 144(1), 83–95. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bley-Vroman, R.
(2002) Frequency in production, comprehension and acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 24(2), 209–213. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chater, N., & Christiansen, M. H.
(2010) Language acquisition meets language evolution. Cognitive Science, 34(7): 1131–1157. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N.
(1995) The minimalist program. The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Dekydspotter, L.
(2009) Second language epistemology. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 31(2), 291–321. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N.
(2005) At the interface: Dynamic interactions of explicit and implicit language knowledge. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 305–352. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gigerenzer, G.
(2007) Gut feelings. Short cuts to better decision making. Penguin Books.Google Scholar
(2020) How to explain behavior. Topics in Cognitive science, 12(4), 1363–1381. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, A.
(2008) Universal grammar? Or prerequisites for natural languages. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31(5), 522–523. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gregg, K., Long, M. H., Jordan, G., & Beretta, A.
(1997) Rationality and its discontents in SLA. Applied Linguistics, 18(4), 538–558. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hafenbrädl, S., Waeger, D., Marewski, J. N., & Gigerenzer, G.
(2016) Applied decision making with fast-and-frugal heuristics. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 5, 215–231. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hulstijn, J. H.
(2013) Is the second language acquisition discipline disintegrating? Language Teaching, 46(4), 511–517. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jordan, G.
Lakshmanan, U., & Selinker, L.
(2001) Analysing interlanguage: How do we know what learners know? Second Language Research, 17, 393–420. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lardiere, D.
(2003) The comparative fallacy revisited: A reply to Lakshmanan and Selinker (2001). Second Language Research, 19, 129–143. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Long, M. H.
(1993) Assessment strategies for SLA theories. Applied Linguistics, 14, 225–249. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015) First person singular: Building the road as we travel. Language Teaching, 48(4), 561–574. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016) Stefano Rastelli: Discontinuity in second language acquisition. The switch between statistical and grammatical learning. Book review. Applied Linguistics, 37(4), 590–597. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mehler, J., Peña, M., Nespor, M., & Bonatti, L.
(2006) The soul of the language does not use statistics: Reflections on vowels and consonants. Cortex, 42(6), 846–854. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Osterhout, L., McLaughlin, J., Pitkänen, I., Frenck-Mestre, C., & Molinaro, N.
(2006) Novice learners, longitudinal designs and event-related potentials: A means for exploring the neurocognition of second language processing. In P. Indefrey & M. Gullberg (Eds.), The cognitive neuroscience of second language acquisition (pp. 199–230). Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pallotti, G.
(2021) Cratylus’ silence. On the philosophy and methodology of Complex Dynamic System Theory in SLA. Second Language Research. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Raab, M., & Gigerenzer, G.
(2015) The power of simplicity: A fast-and-frugal heuristics approach to performance science. Frontiers in Psychology. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rastelli, S.
(2014) Discontinuity in second language acquisition: The switch between statistical and grammatical learning. Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2019) The Discontinuity Model: Statistical and grammatical learning in adult second language acquisition. Language Acquisition, 26(4), 387–415. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2020) Contingency learning and perfective morpheme productivity in L2 Italian: A study on lexeme–morpheme associations with ΔP. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory, 16(3), 459–486. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2021) Intra-language: The study of L2 morpheme productivity as within-item variance. International Review of Applied Linguistics IRAL. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
forthcoming). Statistical and grammatical processing of auxiliaries in L2 Italian: An eye-tracking study.
Rastelli, S., & Murakami, A.
(2022) Apparently identical verbs can be represented differently. Comparing L1-L2 inflection with contingency-based measure ΔP. Corpora, 17/1, 97–121. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ullman, M. T.
(2020) The Declarative-Procedural Model. In B. VanPatten, G. D. Keating, & S. Wullf (Eds.), Theories in second language acquisition. An introduction (3rd ed., pp. 128–161). Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar