References

References

Adobe Audition CC [Computer Program]
Baddeley, A. D.
(1966) Short-term memory for word sequences as a function of acoustic, semantic and formal similarity. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 362–365. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J.
(1994) Developments in the concept of working memory. Neuropsychology, 8, 485–493. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. D., Thomson, N., & Buchanan, M.
(1975) Word length and the structure of short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 575–589. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Caplan, D., Rochon, E., & Waters, G. S.
(1992) Articulatory and phonological determinants of word length effects in span tasks. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45A, 177–192. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Caplan, D., & Waters, G. S.
(1994) Articulatory length and phonological similarity in span tasks: A reply to Baddeley and Andrade. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47A, 1055–1062. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1995) On the nature of the phonological output planning processes involved in verbal rehearsal: Evidence from aphasia. Brain and Language, 48, 191–220. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Conrad, R., & Hull, A. J.
(1964) Information, acoustic confusion and memory span. British Journal of Psychology, 55, 429–432. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, L., & Shaw, J. A.
(2012) Sources of illusion in consonant cluster perception. Journal of Phonetics, 40, 234–248. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Dupoux, E., & Gout, A.
(2000) Electrophysiological correlates of phonological processing: A cross-linguistic study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 635–647. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dupoux, E., Kakehi, K., Hirose, Y., Pallier, C., & Mehler, J.
(1999) Epenthetic vowels in Japanese: A perceptual illusion? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1568–1578. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dupoux, E., Pallier, C., Kakehi, K., & Mehler, J.
(2001) New evidence for prelexical phonological processing in word recognition. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16, 491–505. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fais, L., Kajikawa, S., Amano, S., & Werker, J. F.
(2010) Now you hear it, now you don’t: Vowel devoicing in Japanese infant-directed speech. Journal of Child Language, 37, 319–340. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. E.
(1989) Chinese subjects’ perception of the word-final English /t/-/d/ contrast: Performance before and after training. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 86, 1684–1697. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. E., & Fletcher, K. L.
(1992) Talker and listener effects on degree of perceived foreign accent. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 91, 370–389. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. E., Frieda, E. M., & Nozawa, T.
(1997) Amount of native-language (L1) use affects the pronunciation of an L2. Journal of Phonetics, 25, 169–186. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. E., & MacKay, I. R. A.
(2004) Perceiving vowels in a second language. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 26, 1–34. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. E., Munro, M. J., & MacKay, I. R. A.
(1995) Factors affecting strength of perceived foreign accent in a second language. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 97, 3125–3134. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. E., Yeni-Komshian, G. H., & Liu, S.
(1999) Age constraints on second-language acquisition. Journal of Memory and Language, 41, 78–104. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Forster, J. C.
(2016) DMDX (Version 5) [Computer Program]. Retrieved from http://​www​.u​.arizona​.edu​/~kforster​/dmdx​/download​.htm .
Gussenhoven, C., & Jacobs, H.
(2011) Understanding phonology (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Hulme, C., Surprenant, A. M., Bireta, T. J., Stuart, G., & Neath, I.
(2004) Abolishing the word-length effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 98–106. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kabak, B., & Idsardi, W. J.
(2007) Perceptual distortions in the adaptation of English consonant clusters: Syllable structure or consonantal contact constraints? Language and Speech, 50, 23–52. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kim, J., Taft, M., & Davis, C.
(2004) Orthographic-phonological links in the lexicon: When lexical and sublexical information conflict. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 17, 187–218. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
LaCharite, D., & Paradis, C.
(2005) Category preservation and proximity versus phonetic approximation in loanword adaptation. Linguistic Inquiry, 36, 223–258. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Long, M. H.
(1990) Maturational constraints on language development. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 12, 251–285. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lovatt, P., Avons, S. E., & Masterson, J.
(2000) The word-length effect and disyllabic words. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53A, 1–22. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Martinez-Garcia, M. T., & Tremblay, A.
(2015) Syllable structure affects second-language spoken word recognition and production. In The Scottish Consortium for ICPhS 2015 (Ed.), Proceedings of the 18th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences. Glasgow: The University of Glasgow. Retrieved from http://​www​.icphs2015​.info​/pdfs​/Papers​/ICPHS0824​.pdf
Masuda, H. & Arai, T.
(2010) Processing of consonant clusters by Japanese native speakers: Influence of English learning backgrounds. Acoustical Science and Technology, 31, 320–327. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mueller, S. T., Seymour, T. L., Kieras, D. E., & Meyer, D. E.
(2003) Theoretical implications of articulatory duration, phonological similarity, and phonological complexity in verbal working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 1353–1380. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Neath, I., Bireta, T. J., & Surprenant, A. M.
(2003) The time-based word length effect and stimulus set specificity. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 430–434. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Neath, I. & Nairne, J. S.
(1995) Word-length effects in immediate memory: Overwriting trace decay theory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 429–441. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nguyen-Hoan, M., & Taft, M.
(2010) The impact of a subordinate L1 on L2 auditory processing in adult bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13, 217–230. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Oyama, S.
(1976) A sensitive period for the acquisition of a nonnative phonological system. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 5, 261–283. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Peperkamp, S. & Dupoux, E.
(2003) Reinterpreting loanword adaptations: The role of perception. In M. J. Sole, D. Recasens, & J. Romero (Eds.), Proceedings of the 15th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (pp. 367–370). Barcelona: Causal Productions. Retrieved from https://​www​.internationalphoneticassociation​.org​/icphs​-proceedings​/ICPhS2003​/papers​/p15​_0367​.pdf
Piske, T., MacKay, I. R. A., & Flege, J. E.
(2001) Factors affecting degree of foreign accent in an L2: A review. Journal of Phonetics, 29, 191–215. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rochet, B. L. & Putnam Rochet, A.
(1999) Effects of L1 phonotactic constraints on L2 speech perception. In J. J. Ohala, Y. Hasegawa, M. Ohala, D. Granville, & A. C. Baily (Eds.), Proceedings of the 14th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (pp. 1443–1446). Berkeley, CA: University of California. Retrieved from <https://​www​.internationalphoneticassociation​.org​/icphs​-proceedings​/ICPhS1999​/papers​/p14​_1443​.pdf
Service, E.
(1998) The effect of word length on immediate serial recall depends on phonological complexity, not articulatory duration. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51A, 283–304. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Shibuya, Y. & Erickson, D.
(2010, September). Consonant cluster production in Japanese learners of English. Paper presented at the Interspeech 2010 Satellite Workshop on Second Language Studies, Tokyo, Japan.
Smith, J. L.
(2006) Loan phonology is not all perception: Evidence from Japanese loan doublets. In T. J. Vance & K. A. Jones (Eds.), Japanese/Korean Linguistics (Vol. 14, pp. 63–74). Stanford, CA: CSLI.Google Scholar
Taft, M.
(2006) Orthographically influenced abstract phonological representation: Evidence from non-rhotic speakers. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 35, 67–78. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Verdonschot, R. G., Kiyama, S., Tamaoka, K., Kinoshita, S., La Heij, W., & Schiller, N. O.
(2011) The functional unit of Japanese word naming: Evidence from masked priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 1458–1473. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Vokic, G.
(2008) The role of structural position in L2 phonological acquisition: Evidence from English learners of Spanish as L2. Foreign Language Annals, 41, 347–363. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Weber, A. & Cutler, A.
(2006) First-language phonotactics in second-language listening. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 119, 597–607. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Yeni-Komshian, G. H., Flege, J. E., & Liu, S.
(2000) Pronunciation proficiency in the first and second languages of Korean-English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 3, 131–149. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 2 other publications

Bae, Sungbong, Hye K. Pae & Kwangoh Yi
2018.  In Writing Systems, Reading Processes, and Cross-Linguistic Influences [Bilingual Processing and Acquisition, 7],  pp. 373 ff. Crossref logo
Pae, Hye K.
2018.  In Writing Systems, Reading Processes, and Cross-Linguistic Influences [Bilingual Processing and Acquisition, 7],  pp. 1 ff. Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 08 may 2021. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.