Review article published In:
Evolution of Communication
Vol. 1:1 (1997) ► pp.5999
References (50)
References
Armstrong, D. F., W. C. Stokoe and S. E. Wilcox. 1995. Gesture and the nature of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bickerton, D. 1990. Language and species. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Binford, L. 1992. Hard evidence. Discover February 1992: 44–51.Google Scholar
Boesch, C. and H. Boesch. 1984. Mental map in wild chimpanzees. Primates 251: 160–70. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. 1983. Child’s talk. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. W. 1995. The thinking ape. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cartmill, M. 1990. Human uniqueness and theoretical content in paleoanthropology. International Journal of Primatology 11(3): 173–192. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cheney, D. L. and R. M. Seyfarth. 1988. Assessment of meaning and the detection of unreliable signals by vervet monkeys. Animal Behaviour 361: 477–486. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Descartes, R. 1637. Discourse in method. In J. Cottingham, R. Stoothoff, and D. Murdoch (Trans.), The philosophical writings of Descartes (Vol. 1). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1986.Google Scholar
Frye, N. 1982. The great code: The Bible and literature. Toronto: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Gibson, K. R. 1993. Animal minds, human minds. In K. R. Gibson and T. Ingold (Eds.), Tools, language and cognition in human evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
1996. The biocultural human brain, seasonal migration, and the emergence of the Upper Palaeolithic. In P. Mellars and K. Gibson (Eds.), Modelling the early human mind. Cambridge: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research.Google Scholar
Golding, W. 1955. The inheritors. London: Faber and Faber.Google Scholar
Gouzoules, H., S. Gouzoules and J. Ashley. 1995. Representational signaling in non-human primate vocal communication. In E. Zimmerman, J. D. Newman, and U. Jurgens (Eds.), Primate vocal communication. New York: Plenum Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gouzoules, S., H. Gouzoules and P. Marler. 1984. Rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta) screams: Representational signalling in the recruitment of agonistic aid. Animal Behaviour 321: 182–193. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Harris, R. 1990. On defining linguistics. In H. G. Davis and T. J. Taylor (Eds.), Redefining linguistics. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
1996. The language connection. Bristol: Thoemmes Press.Google Scholar
Hauser, M. 1996. The evolution of communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hinde, R. M. 1987. Individuals, relationships, and culture. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Huffmann, M. A. and R. W. Wrangham. 1994. Diversity of medicinal plant use by chimpanzees in the wild. In R. W. Wrangham, W. C. McGrew, F. B. M. deWaal, and P. G. Heltne (Eds.), Chimpanzee cultures. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Kane, J. 1995. Savages. Toronto: Douglas & Mcintrye.Google Scholar
King, B. J. 1994a. The information continuum: Evolution of social information transfer in monkeys, apes, and hominids. Santa Fe, NM.: School of American Research Press.Google Scholar
1994b. Evolutionism, essentialism, and an evolutionary perspective on language: Moving beyond a human standard. Language & Communication 14(1): 1–13. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1996. Syntax and the origins of language. Language & Communication 16 (2): 193–203. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kummer, H. 1968. Social organization of hamadryas baboons: A field study. Biblioteca Primatologica 61: 1–189. Basel: S. Karger.Google Scholar
Lovejoy, A. O. J. 1960. The great chain of being: A study in the history of an idea. New York: Harper.Google Scholar
Maestripieri, D. n.d. Primate social organization, gestural repertoire size, and communication dynamics: A comparative study of macaques. Submitted.
Mithen, S. J. 1996. The prehistory of the mind. London: Thames and Hudson Ltd.Google Scholar
Noble, W. and I. Davidson. 1996. Human evolution, language, and mind: A psychological and archaeological inquiry. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Quiatt, D. and V. Reynolds. 1993. Primate behaviour: Information, social knowledge, and the evolution of culture. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Saussure, F. 1972. Course in general linguistics. London: Duckworth.Google Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., J. Murphy, R. A. Sevcik, K. E. Brakke, S. L. Williams, D. M. Rumbaugh. 1993. Language comprehension in ape and child. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, Serial No. 233, Vol. 581, Nos. 3–4.Google Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., S. G. Shanker, and T. J. Taylor. In press. Apes, language and the human mind. New York: Oxford University Press.
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., S. L. Williams, T. Furuichi, and T. Kano. 1996. Language perceived: Pan paniscus branches out. In W. C. McGrew, L. R. Marchant, and T. Nishida (Eds.), Great ape societies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schore, A. 1994. Affect regulation and the origin of self. Hillsdale: Lawrence Er-lbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Seyfarth, R. L. and D. L. Cheney. In press. Some general features of vocal development in nonhuman primates. In C. T. Snowdon and M. Hausberger (Eds.), Social influences on vocal development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logo
Shanker, S. G. In press. Reassessing Bruner’s role in the cognitive revolution. In D. Bakhurst and S. G. Shanker (Eds.), Jerome Bruner’s philosophical psychology. New York: Sage.
Shanker, S. G. and T. J. Taylor. Forthcoming. The house that Bruner built. In D. Bakhurst and S. G. Shanker (Eds.), Jerome Bruner’s philosophical psychology. New York: Sage. DOI logo
Snowdon, C. n.d. An empiricist view of language evolution and development. Submitted.
Stringer, C. and C. Gamble. 1993. In search of the Neanderthals. New York: Thames and Hudson.Google Scholar
Taylor, T. J. 1992. Mutual misunderstanding. Durham, NC: Duke University Press and London: Routledge Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1997. The origin of language: Why it never happened. Language Sciences 19(1), 67–78. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tobias, P. V. 1991. Olduvai gorge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. and J. Call. 1994. Social cognition of monkeys and apes. Year-book of Physical Anthropology 371: 273–305. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Trevarthan, C. 1993. The self born in intersubjectivity: The psychology of an infant communicating. In U. Neisser (Ed.), The perceived self. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
deWaal, F. 1996a. Good-natured: The origins of right and wrong in humans and other animals. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
deWaal, F. B. M. 1996b. Conflict as negotiation. In W. C. McGrew, L. F. Marchant, and T. Nishida (Eds.), Great ape societies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Walker, A. and P. Shipman. 1996. The wisdom of the bones: In search of human origins. New York: Alfred A. Knopf.Google Scholar
Whiten, A. and R. W. Byrne. 1988. Tactical deception in primates. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 111: 233–273. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. 1953. Philosophical investigations. G. E. M. Anscombe (Trans.). Oxford: Basil Blackwell, 1973.Google Scholar
Cited by (1)

Cited by one other publication

Shanker, Stuart
2001. What Children Know When They Know What a Name Is. Current Anthropology 42:4  pp. 481 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 1 august 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.