Article published In:
Gesture
Vol. 19:2/3 (2020) ► pp.269298
References
Abercrombie, David
(1954) Gesture. English Language Teaching, 9 (1), 3–12. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Acredolo, Linda P., Goodwyn, Susan W., Horobin, Karen D., & Emmons, Yvonne D.
(1999) The signs and sounds of early language development. In Lawrence Balter & Catherine Susan Tamis-Lemonada. (Eds.), Child psychology: A handbook of contemporary issues (pp. 116–142). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Anderson, Diane. E. & Reilly, Judy S.
(2002) The MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory: Normative data for American Sign Language. The Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 7 (2), 83–106. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Anderson, Kate, Devitt, Jeannie, Cunningham, Joan, Preece, Cilla, & Cass, Alan
(2008) “All they said was my kidneys were dead”: Indigenous Australian patients’ understanding of their chronic kidney disease. Medical Journal of Australia, 189 (9), 499–503. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Andrén, Mats
Andrew, Richard John
(1963) Evolution of facial expression. Science, 142 (3595), 1034–1041. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Antzakas, Klimis
(2006) The use of negative head movements in Greek Sign Language. In Ulrike Zeshan. (Ed.), Interrogative and negative constructions in sign languages (pp. 258–269). Nijmegen: Ishara Press.Google Scholar
Baker-Shenk, Charlotte L.
(1983) A microanalysis of the nonmanual components of questions in American Sign Language. PhD Dissertation, University of California.Google Scholar
Barakat, Robert A.
(1973) Arabic gestures. The Journal of Popular Culture 6 (4), 749–793. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, Elizabeth, Bretherton, Inge, Camaioni, Luigia, & Volterra, Virginia
(Eds.) (1979) The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Benazzo, Sandra & Morgenstern, Aliyah
(2014) A bilingual child’s multimodal path into negation. Gesture, 14 (2), 171–202. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bickel, Balthasar
(2011) Absolute and statistical universals. In Patrick Colm Hogan. (Ed.), The Cambridge encyclopedia of the language sciences (pp. 77–79). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bloom, Lois
(1970) Language development: Form and function in emerging grammars. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bruce, Susan M.
(2005) The impact of congenital deafblindness on the struggle to symbolism. International Journal of Disability, Development, and Education, 521, 233–251. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bulgarian Ministry of Education and Science
(Ed.) (2017) Teoretichno opisanie na gramatikata na bulgarskiya zhestov ezik [Theoretical Description of Bulgarian Sign Language Grammar]. Sofia: Nauka i obrazovanie.Google Scholar
Cameron, A. L. P.
(1881) Letter to A. W. Howitt. In Howitt Papers. Box 2, Folder 5, Australian Institute of Aboriginal Studies. Mimeo.Google Scholar
Cartmill, Erica A. & Byrne, Richard W.
(2010) Semantics of gestures: Intentional meanings of orangutan gestures. Animal Cognition, 13 (6), 793–804. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chang, Ling-Hui & Wang, Jye
(2009) A tough-love pedagogy in rehabilitation: Integration of rehabilitation ideology with local cultures. International Journal of Rehabilitation Research, 32 (3), 219–227. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Choi, Soonja
(1988) The semantic development of negation: A cross-linguistic longitudinal study. Journal of Child Language, 151, 517–531. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, William Philo
(1885) The Indian sign language with brief explanatory notes of the gestures taught deaf-mutes in our institutions for their instruction and a description of some of the peculiar laws, customs, myths, superstitions, ways of living, code of peace and war signals of our Aborigines. Philadelphia: Hamersly & Co.Google Scholar
Collett, Peter & Chilton, Josephine
(1998) Laterality in negation: Are Jakobson and Vavra right? Semiotica, 35 (1/2), 57–70. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Creider, Chet A.
(1977) Towards a description of East African gestures. Sign Language Studies, 141, 1–20. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Darwin, Charles
(1872) The expression of the emotions in man and animals. London: John Murray. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Deasy, Kathleen & Fiona Lyddy
(2009) Exploring language and communication in an individual with congenital deafblindness: A case study. Report to the National Council for Special Education Research Initiative. Online: [URL], received February 6, 2016.
De Vries, Johanna I. P., Visser, Gerald H. A., & Prechtl, Heinz Friedrich Rudolf.
(1982) The emergence of fetal behaviour. I. Qualitative aspects. Early Human Development, 7 (4), 301–322. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dimroth, Christine
(2010) The acquisition of negation. In Laurence R. Horn. (Ed.), The expression of negation (pp. 39–71). Berlin & New York: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, Irenäus
(1973) The expressive behaviour of the deaf-and-blind born. In Mario von Cranach & Ian Vine. (Eds.), Social communication and movement (pp. 163–194). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Emmorey, Karen
(1999) Do signers gesture? In Lynn Messing & Ruth Campbell. (Eds.), Gesture, speech, and sign (pp. 133–159). New York: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eriksson, Marten & Berglund, Eva
(1999) Swedish early communicative development inventories: Words and gestures. First Language, 19 (55), 55–90. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fenson, Larry, Dale, Philip S., Reznick, J. Steven, Bates, Elizabeth, Thal, Donna J., Stephen J. Pethick, Stephen J., Tomasello, Michael, Mervis, Carolyn B., & Stiles, Joan
(1994) Variability in early communicative development. Ann Arbor: Society for Research in Child Development. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fischer, Susan D.
(2006) Questions and negation in American Sign Language. In Ulrike Zeshan. (Ed.), Interrogative and negative constructions in sign languages (pp. 167–197). Nijmegen: Ishara Press.Google Scholar
Fusaro, Maria, Harris, Paul L., & Pan, Barbara A.
(2012) Head nodding and head shaking gestures in children’s early communication. First Language, 321, 439–458. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Genty, Emilie, Breuer, Thomas, Hobaiter, Catherine, & Byrne, Richard W.
(2009) Gestural communication of the gorilla (Gorilla gorilla): repertoire, intentionality and possible origins. Animal Cognition, 121, 527–546. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, Susan & Mylander, Carolyn
(1984) Gestural communication in deaf children: The effects and none effects of parental input on early language development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 49 (3/4), 1–121. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goodenough, Florence L.
(1932) Expressions of the emotions in a blind-deaf child. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 27 (3), 328–333. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Guidetti, Michele
(2005) Yes or no? How do young children combine gestures and words to agree and refuse? Journal of Child Language, 321, 911–924. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hamiru-aqui
(2008) 70 Japanese gestures: No language communication. Berkeley, CA: Stone Bridge Press.Google Scholar
Harrison, Simon
(2009) Grammar, gesture, and cognition. The case of negation in English. PhD Dissertation, University of Bordeaux.Google Scholar
Heinen, H. Dieter
(1972) Economic factors in marriage alliance and kinship system among the Winikia-Warao. Anthropologica, 321, 28–67.Google Scholar
Hendriks, Bernadet
(2007) Negation in Jordanian Sign Language: A cross-linguistic perspective. In Pamerla M. Perniss, Roland Pfau, & Markus Steinbach. (Eds.), Visible variation. Comparative studies on sign language structure (pp. 103–128). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hentschel, Elke
(1998) Negation und Interrogation: Studien zur Universalität ihrer Funktionen. Tübingen: Niemeyer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hobaiter, Catherine & Byrne, Richard W.
(2011) The gestural repertoire of the wild chimpanzee. Animal Cognition, 14 (5), 745–767. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hummer, Peter, Wimmer, Heinz, & Antes, Gertraud
(1993) On the origins of denial negation. Journal of Child Language, 201, 607–618. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ingram, David
(1991) An historical observation on “Why ‘mama’ and ‘papa’?” Journal of Child Language, 181, 711–713. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Iverson, M. Jana, Tencer, Heather L., Lany, Jill, & Goldin-Meadow, Susan
(2000) The relation between gesture and speech in congenitally blind and sighted language-learners. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24 (2), 105–130. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jakobson, Roman
(1962) Why mama and papa? In Roman Jakobson, Selected writings. Volume I1: Phonological studies (pp. 538–545). The Hague: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
(1970) Da i nyet v mimike. In Vladimir Andreyevich Zvegintsev (Ed.), Yazyk i chelovek. Sbornik statej pamyati professora Petra Savvicha Kuznecova (1899–1968) (pp. 284–289). Moscow: Moscow State University Press.Google Scholar
(1972) Motor signs for ‘yes’ and ‘no’. Language in Society, 11, 91–96. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kendon, Adam
(1988) Sign languages of Aboriginal Australia. Cultural, semiotic and communicative perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
(2002) Some uses of the head shake. Gesture, 2 (2), 147–182. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2004) Gesture: Visible action as utterance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kettner, Viktoria A. & Carpendale, Jeremy I. M.
Kita, Sotaro
(2009) Cross-cultural variation of speech-accompanying gesture: A review. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24 (2), 145–167. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kortlandt, Adriaan
(1969) Chimpansees. In Bernhard Grzimek. (Ed.), Het Leven der Dieren (Vol. XI1, pp. 14–49). Utrecht: Het Spectrum.Google Scholar
Kulovesi, Yrjo
(1939) Die Ausdrucksbewegung der Bejahung und der Verneinung. Internationale Zeitschrift für Ärztliche Psychoanalyse, 241, 446–447.Google Scholar
Liebal, Katja, Pika, Simone, & Tomaseollo, Michael
(2006) Gestural communication of orangutans. Gesture, 6 (1), 1–38. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Li, Xiaoting
(2019) Multimodal turn construction in Mandarin conversation: Verbal, vocal, and visual practices in the construction of syntactically incomplete turns. In Xiaoting Li & Tsuyoshi Ono. (Eds.), Multimodality in Chinese interaction (pp. 181–212). Berlin & Boston: De Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Luef, Eva Maria & Liebal, Katja
(2012) Infant-directed communication in Lowland Gorillas (Gorilla gorilla): Do older animals scaffold communicative competence in infants? American Journal of Primatology, 74 (9), 841–852. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Masur, Elise Frank
(1980) The development of communicative gestures in mother-infant interactions. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 191, 121–128.Google Scholar
Matsumoto, David & Hwang, Hyisung C.
(2012) Cultural similarities and differences in emblematic gestures. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 37 (1), 1–27. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McClave, Evelyn
(2000) Linguistic functions of head movements in the context of speech. Journal of Pragmatics, 321, 855–878. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McClave, Evelyn, Kim, Helen, Tamer, Rita, & Mile, Milo
Meek, Joan Younger
(2017) The American Academy of Pediatrics new mother’s guide to breastfeeding. New York: Bantam.Google Scholar
Meier, Richard P.
(2002) Why different, why the same? Explaining effects and non-effects of modality upon linguistic structure in sign and speech. In Richard P. Meier, Kearsy Cormier, & David Quinto-Pozos. (Ed.), Modality and structure in signed and spoken languages (pp. 1–25). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morris, Desmond, Collett, Peter, Marsh, Peter, & O’Shaughnessy, Marie
(1979) Gestures: Their origins and distribution. London: Stein and Day.Google Scholar
Murdock, G.
(1959) Cross-language parallels in parental kin terms. Anthropological Linguistics, 1 (9), 1–5.Google Scholar
Pea, Roy D.
(1980) The development of negation in early child language. In David R. Olson. (Ed.), The social foundations of language and thought: Essays in honor of Jerome S. Bruner (pp. 156–186). New York: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
Pereira, Michael Eric, Seeligson, Martha L., & Macedonia, Joseph
(1988) The behavioral repertoire of the Black-and-White Ruffed Lemur, Varecia variegata variegata (Primates: Lemuridae), Folia Primatologica, 51 (1), 1–32. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pfau, Roland
(2012) Manual communication systems: Evolution and variation. In Roland Pfau, Markus Steinbach, & Bencie Woll. (Eds.), Sign language: An international handbook (pp. 513–551). Berlin & Boston: Walter de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015) The grammaticalization of headshakes: From head movement to negative head. In Andrew D. M. Smith, Graeme Trousdale, & Richard Waltereit. (Eds.), New directions in grammaticalization research (pp. 9–50). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pika, Simone
(2007) Gestures in subadult bonobos (Pan paniscus). In Joseph Call & Michael Tomasello. (Eds.), The gestural communication of apes and monkeys (pp. 41–68). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Pika, Simone, Liebal, Katja, & Tomasello, Michael
(2003) Gestural communication in young gorillas (Gorilla gorilla): Gestural repertoire, learning, and use. American Journal of Primatology, 601, 95–111. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinfield, Tom
(2013) Gestural head movements in captive bonobos (Pan paniscus): Use, function and evolutionary implications. Canopy, 14 (1), 6–10.Google Scholar
Porac, Clare & Martin, William Lee Berdel
(2007) A cross-cultural comparison of pressures to switch left-hand writing: Brazil versus Canada. Laterality, 12 (3), 273–291. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Prechtl, Heinz Friedrich Rudolf
(1989) Development of postural control in infancy. In Curt von Euler, Hans Forssberg, & Hugo Lagercrantz. (Eds.), Neurobiology of early infant behaviour (pp. 50–68). London: Macmillan. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Premavardhena, Neelakshi Chandrasena
(2007) How diplomatic can a language be? The unwritten rules in a language: An analysis of spoken Sinhala. In Marion Grein & Edda Weigand. (Eds.), Dialog and culture (pp. 213–226). Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Quer, Josep
(2012) Negation. In Roland Pfau, Markus Steinbach, & Bencie Woll. (Eds.), Sign language: An international handbook (pp. 316–339). Berlin & Boston: Walter de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Reilly, Judy & Anderson, Diane E.
(2002) FACES. The acquisition of non-manual morphology in ASL. In Gary Morgan & Bencie Woll. (Eds.), Directions in sign language acquisition (pp. 159–181). Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rose, H. A.
(1919) The language of gesture. Folklore, 30 (4), 312–315. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Saitz, Robert L. & Cervenka, Edward J.
(1962) Colombian and North American gestures: An experimental study. Bogota: Centro Colombo-Americano.Google Scholar
Sandler, Wendy
(2009) Sign languages. In Keith Brown & Sarah Ogilvie. (Eds.), Concise encyclopedia of languages of the world (pp. 939–948). Oxford: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Sandler, Wendy & Lillo-Martin, Diane
(2006) Sign language and linguistic universals. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Saussure, Ferdinand de
(1916) Cours de linguistique générale. Lausanne: Payot.Google Scholar
Schneider, Christel, Call, Joseph, & Liebal, Katja
(2010) Do bonobos say NO by shaking their head? Primates, 51 (3), 199–202. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smith, Wilbur L., Erenberg, Allen, Nowak, Arthur, & Franken, Edmund A.
(1985) Physiology of sucking in the normal term infant using real-time US. Radiology, 1561, 379–381. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Solanki, G. S., Kumar, Awadhesh, & Sharma, B. K.
(2007) Reproductive strategies of trachypithecus pileatus in Arunachal Pradesh, India. International Journal of Primatology, 28 (5), 1075–1083. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Spitz, Ren A.
(1957) No and yes: On the genesis of human communication. New York: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
Stern, Clara & Stern, William
(1907) Die Kindersprache. Eine psychologische und sprachtheoretische Untersuchung. Leipzig: Johann Ambrosius Barth.Google Scholar
Sugar, Nikola
(1941) Zur Frage der mimischen Bejahung und Verneinung. Internationale Zeitschrift für Psychoanalyse und Imago, 261, 81–83.Google Scholar
Tanner, Joanne E., Patterson, Francine G., & Byrne, Richard W.
(2006) The development of spontaneous gesture in zoo-living gorillas and sign-taught gorillas: From action and location to object representation. Journal of Developmental Processes, 11, 69–103.Google Scholar
Tanner, Joanne E. & Perlman, Marcus
(2017) Moving beyond ‘meaning’: Gorillas combine gestures into sequences for creative display. Language & Communication, 541, 56–72. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, Michael & Call, Josep
(1997) Primate cognition. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tuttle, Russell H.
(2014) Apes and human evolution. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vaidyanathan, Raghunathan
(1991) Development of forms and functions of negation in the early stages of language acquisition: A study in Tamil. Journal of Child Language, 181, 51–66. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van Hooff, Jan A. R. A. M.
(1973) A structural analysis of the social behaviour of a semi-captive group of chimpanzees. In Mario von Cranach & Ian Vine. (Eds.), Social communication and expression in man and chimpanzee (pp. 75–162). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Vavra, Vlastimil
(1976) Is Jakobson right? Semiotica, 17 (2), 95–110. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Waal, Frans de
(1982) Chimpanzee politics: Power and sex among apes. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Wagner, Petra, Malisz, Zofia, & Kopp, Stefan
(2014) Gesture and speech in interaction: An overview. Speech Communication, 571, 209–232. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilbur, Ronnie B.
(2003) Modality and the structure of language: Sign languages versus signed systems. In Marc Maschark & Patricia E. Spencer. (Eds.), Oxford handbook of deaf studies, language, and education (pp. 332–346). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Wurtsburg, Susan & Campbell, Lyle
(1995) North American Indian Sign Language: Evidence for its existence before European contact. Journal of American Linguistics, 61 (2) 153–167. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zeshan, Ulrike
(2004) Hand, head, and face: Negative constructions in sign languages. Linguistic Typology, 8 (1), 1–58. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(Ed.) (2006) Interrogative and negative constructions in sign languages. Nijmegen: Ishara Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zlatev, Jordan & Andrén, Mats
(2009) Stages and transitions in children’s semiotic development. In Jordan Zlatev, Mats Andrén, Marlene Johannson Falck, & Carita Lundmark. (Eds.), Studies in language and cognition (pp. 380–401). Newcastle: Cambridge Scholars.Google Scholar