References
Best, C.T. & Strange, W
1992Effects of language-specific phonological and phonetic factors on cross-language perception of approximants. Journal of Phonetics 20: 305-30.Google Scholar
Best, C.T., Traill, A., Carter, A., Harrison, K.D. & Faber, A
2003!Xóõ click perception in English, Isizulu and Sesotho listeners. In Proceedings of the 15th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, M.J. Solé, D. Recasens & J. Romero (eds), 853-56. Barcelona: Causal Productions.Google Scholar
Best, C.T., & Tyler, M.D
2007Non-native and second-language speech perception. Commonalities and complementarities. In Second Language Speech Learning: The Role of Language Experience in Speech Perception and Production [Language Learning & Language Teaching 17], M.J. Munro & O.-S. Bohn (eds), 13-34. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bradlow, A.R., Pisoni, D.B., Akahane-Yamada, R.A. & Tohkura, Y
1997Training Japanese listeners to identify English /r/ and /l/: IV. Some effects of perceptual learning on speech production. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 101(4): 2299-2310. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brent, M.R., & Cartwright, T.A
1996aDistributional regularity and lexical access. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 26: 363-375. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brent, M.R. & Cartwright, T.A
1996bDistributional regularity and phonotactic constraints are useful for segmentation. Cognition 61: 93-125. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carroll, S.E
2012Segmentation on first exposure to an L2: Evidence for knowledge-driven, top-down processing. In Multilingual Individuals and Multilingual Societies [Hamburg Studies on Multilingualism 13], K. Braunmüller, C. Gabriel & B. Hänel-Faulhaber (eds), 23-45. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2014Processing ‘words’ in early-stage SLA: A comparison of first exposure and low proficiency learners. In Input Processing in Second Language Acquisition, Z.H. Han & R. Rast (eds). Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
Chambers, J.K
1975Canadian raising. In Canadian English. Origins and Structures, J.K. Chambers, 83-100. Toronto: Methuen.Google Scholar
1989Canadian raising: Blocking, fronting, etc. American Speech 64(1): 75-88. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chambers, K.E., Onishi, K.H. & Fisher, C
2003Infants learn phonotactic regularities from brief auditory experience. Cognition 87: B69-B77. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cholin, J., Schiller, N.O. & Levelt, W.J.M
2004The preparation of syllables in speech production. Journal of Memory & Language 50(1): 47-61. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Colantoni, L., & Steele, J
2007Acquiring /ʁ/ in context. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 29: 381-406. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Caramazza, A. & Sebastian-Galles, N
2000The cognate facilitation effect: Implications for models of lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 26(5): 1283-96. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Curtin, S
2002Representational Richness in Phonological Development. PhD dissertation, University of Southern California.
2009Twelve-month-olds learn word-object associations differing only in stress patterns. Journal of Child Language 36: 1157-1165. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Curtin, S., Mintz, T.H. & Christiansen, M.H
2005Stress changes the representational landscape: Evidence from word segmentation. Cognition 96: 233-62. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cutler, A
2012Native listening. Language Experience and the Recognition of Spoken Words. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cutler, A. & Norris, D.G
1988The role of strong syllables in segmentation for lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 14: 113-21. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Jong, K.J., Silbert, N.H. & Park, H
2009Generalisation across segments in second language consonant identification. Language Learning 59(1): 1-31. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dijkstra, A., Grainger, J. & van Heuven, W.J.B
1999Recognizing cognates and interlingual homographs: The neglected role of phonology. Journal of Memory & Language 41: 496-518. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dupoux, E., Pallier, C., Sebastián-Gallés, N. & Mehler, J
1997A destressing 'deafness' in French? Journal of Memory & Language 36: 406-21. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eisenberg, P
2004Grundriß der Deutschen Grammatik. Das Wort, 2nd edn. Stuttgart: J.B. Metzler.Google Scholar
Escudero, P
2005Linguistic Perception and Second Language Acquisition: Explaining the Attainment of Optimal Phonological Categorisation. PhD dissertation, University of Utrecht.
2006Second language phonology: The role of perception. In Phonology in Context, M.C. Pennington (ed.), 109-34. Houndmills: Palgrave MacMillan.Google Scholar
Finn, A. & Hudson Kam, C.L
2008The curse of knowledge: First language knowledge impairs adult learners' use of novel statistics for word segmentation. Cognition 108: 477-99. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Flege, J.E
2003Assessing constraints on second-language segmental production and perception. In Phonetics and Phonology in Language Comprehension and Production: Differences and Similarities, N.O. Schiller & A.S. Meyer (eds), 319-55. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Flege, J.E., Frieda, W.M. & Nozawa, T
1997Amount of native-language (L1) use affects the pronunciation of an L2. Journal of Phonetics 25: 169-86. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Flege, J.E., Munro, M.J. & MacKay, I.R.A
1995Effects of age of second language learning on the production of English consonants. Speech Communication 16: 1-26. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Flege, J.E. & Wang, C
1989Native-language phonotactic constraints affect how well Chinese subjects perceive the word-final English /t/-/d/ contrast. Journal of Phonetics 17: 299-315.Google Scholar
Flege, J.E., Yeni-Komshian, G.H. & Liu, S
1999Age constraints on second language acquisition. Journal of Memory & Language 41: 78-104. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Friel, B.M. & Kennison, S.M
2001Identifying German-English cognates, false cognates, and non-cognates: Methodological issues and descriptive norms. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 4: 249-74. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldrick, M. & Larson, M
2008Phonotactic probability influences speech production. Cognition 107: 1155-64. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Guion, S., Flege, J.E. & Loftin, J.D
2000The effect of L1 use on pronunciation in Quichua- Spanish bilinguals. Journal of Phonetics 28: 27-42. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gullberg, M., Roberts, L., Dimroth, C., Veroude, K., & Indefrey, P
2010Adult language learning after minimal exposure to an unknown natural language. Language Learning 60(Suppl. 2): 5-24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gullberg, M., Roberts, L. & Dimroth, C
2012What word-level knowledge can adult learners acquire after minimal exposure to a new language? International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching (IRAL) 50: 239-76.Google Scholar
Hall, T.A
1993The phonology of German /R/. Phonology 10(1): 83-105. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hamann, S
2009The learner of a perception grammar as a source of sound change. In Phonology in Perception, P. Boersma & S. Hamann (eds), 111-49. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Hohne, E.A. & Jusczyk, P.W
1994Two-month-old-infants' sensitivity to allophonic differences. Perception & Psychophysics 56: 613-23. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jusczyk, P.W
1997The Discovery of Spoken Language. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jusczyk, P.W., Cutler, A. & Redanz, N
1993Infants' preference for the predominant stress patterns in English. Child Development 64: 675-87. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jusczyk, P.W., Luce, P.A. & Charles-Luce, J
1994Infants' sensitivity to phonotactic patterns in the native language. Journal of Memory & Language 35(5): 630-645. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kohler, K
2010German. In Handbook of the International Phonetic Association: A Guide to the use of the International Phonetic Alphabet, I. Maddieson & M. Barry (eds), 86-89. Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
Levelt, W.J.M
1999Producing spoken language: A blueprint of the speaker. In The Neurocognition of Language, C.M. Brown & P. Hagoort (eds), 83-122. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Li, P
1996Spoken word recognition of code-switched words by Chinese-English bilinguals. Journal of Memory & Language 35: 757-74. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lisker, L. & Abramson, A.S
1967Some effects of context on voice onset time in English stops. Language & Speech 10: 1-28.Google Scholar
Luce, P.A., Pisoni, D.B. & Goldinger, S.D
1990Similarity neighbourhoods of spoken words. In Cognitive Models of Speech Processing: Psycholinguistic and Computational Perspectives, G.T.M. Altmann (ed.), 122-47. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Mattys, S.L. & Jusczyk, P.W
2001aDo infants segment words or recurring contiguous patterns? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance 27(3): 644-55. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2001bPhonotactic cues for segmentation of fluent speech by infants. Cognition 78: 91-121. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mattys, S.L., Jusczyk, P.W., Luce, P.A., & Morgan, J.L
1999Phonotactic and prosodic effects on word segmentation in infants. Cognitive Psychology 38(4): 465-94. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mattys, S.L., White, L. & Melhorn, J.F
2005Integration of multiple speech segmentation cues: A hierarchical framework. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 134(4): 477-500. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McQueen, J.M
1998Segmentation of continuous speech using phonotactics. Journal of Memory & Language 39: 21-46. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Moreton, E. & Thomas, E.R
2007Origins of Canadian raising in voiceless-coda effects: A case study in phonologization. In Laboratory Phonology IX, J. Cole & J.I. Hualde (eds), 37-64. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Peperkamp, S. & Dupoux, E
2002A typological study of stress 'deafness'. In Laboratory Phonology VII, C. Gussenhoven & N. Warner (eds), 203-240. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Piske, T
2007Implications of James E. Flege's research for the foreign language classroom. In Language Experience in Second Language Speech Learning: In Honor of James Emil Flege [Language Learning & Language Teaching 17], M.J. Munro & O.-S. Bohn (eds), 301-314. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Piske, T., MacKay, I., & Flege, J.E
2001Factors affecting degree of foreign accent in an L2: A review. Journal of Phonetics 29: 181-215. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Piske, T., Flege, J.E., MacKay, I.R.A., & Meador, D
2002The production of English vowels by fluent early and late Italian-English bilinguals. Phonetica 59: 49-71. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pisoni, D.R., & Lively, S.E
1995Variability and invariance in speech perception. A new look at some old problems in perceptual learning. In Speech Perception and Linguistic Experience: Theoretical and Methodological Issues in Cross-language Speech Research, W. Strange (ed.), 433-59. Timonium MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Polka, L
1992Characterizing the influence of native experience on adult speech perception. Perception & Psychophysics 52: 37-52. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rast, R
2008Foreign Language Input: Initial Processing. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
2010The role of linguistic input in the first hours of adult language learning. Language Learning 60 (Suppl.2): 64-84. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rast, R. & Dommergues, J.-Y
Shoemaker, E., & Rast, R
2013Extracting words from the speech stream at first exposure. Second Language Research 29(2): 165-83. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tench, P
2003Non-native speakers' misperception of English vowels and consonants: Evidence from Korean adults in UK. International Rreview of Applied Linguistics in Teaching (IRAL) 41: 145-73.Google Scholar
Tremblay, A
2008Is L2 lexical access prosodically constrained? On the processing of word stress by French Canadian L2 learners of English. Applied Psycholinguistics 29: 553-84. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vitevitch, M.S. & Luce, P.A
1999Probabilistic phonotactics and neighborhood activation in spoken word recognition. Journal of Memory & Language 40: 374-408. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Weber, A. & Cutler, A
2006First-language phonotactics in second language listening. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 119: 597-607. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Werker, J.F. & Tees, R.C
1984Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual re- organisation during the first year of life. Infant Behavior & Development 7: 49-63. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wright, R
2004A review of perceptual cues and cue robustness. In Phonetically based Phonology, B. Hayes, R. Kirchner & D. Steriade (eds), 34-57. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 1 other publications

Carroll, Susanne E. & Angela George
2018. What absolute beginners learn from input. Instructed Second Language Acquisition 2:2  pp. 112 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 11 march 2023. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.