Article published in:
Interaction Studies
Vol. 18:2 (2017) ► pp. 276298
References

References

Bargh, J. A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L.
(1996) Automaticity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(2), 230–244. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Barroso, F., Freedman, N., Grand, S., & van Meel, J.
(1978) The evocation of two types of hand movements in information processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4, 321–329. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Barroso, F., Freedman, N., & Grand, S.
(1980) Self-touching performance, and attentional processes. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 50, 1083–1089. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Barroso, F., & Feld, J. K.
(1986) Self-touching and attentional processes: The role of task difficulty, selection stage, and sex differences. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 10, 51–64. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bavelas, J. B., Chovil, N., Lawrie, D. A., & Wade, A.
(1992) Interactive gestures. Discourse Processes, 15, 469–489. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bavelas, J. B., Chovil, N., Coates, L., & Roe, L.
(1995) Gestures specialized for dialogue. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 394–405. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Butzen, N., Bissonnette, V., & McBrayer, D.
(2005) Effects of modelling and topic stimulus on self-referent touching. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 101, 413–420. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Casillas, M., & Frank, M. C.
(2012) Cues to turn boundary prediction in adults and preschoolers. In S. Brown-Schmidt, J. Ginzburg, & S. Larsson (Eds.), Proceedings of SemDial 2012 (SeineDial): The 16th Workshop on the Semantics and Pragmatics of Dialogue (pp. 61–69). Paris: Université Paris-Diderot.Google Scholar
(2013) The development of predictive processes in children’s discourse understanding. In M. Knauff, M. Pauen, N. Sebanz, & I. Wachsmuth (Eds.), Proceedings of the 35th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 299–304). Austin, TX: Cognitive Society.Google Scholar
Caspers, J.
(2003) Local speech melody as a limiting factor in the turn-taking system in Dutch. Journal of Phonetics, 31, 251–276. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A.
(1999) The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893–910. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ciolek, T. M., & Kendon, A.
(1980) Environment and the spatial arrangement of conversational encounters. Sociological Inquiry, 50(3‐4), 237–271. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Condon, W., & Ogston, W.
(1971) Speech and body motion synchrony of the speaker-hearer. In D. Horton & J. Jenkins (Eds.), The perception of language (pp. 150–184). Columbus, OH: Charles E. Merrill.Google Scholar
Couper-Kuhlen, E., & Selting, M.
(1996) Towards an interactional perspective on prosody and a prosodic perspective on interaction. In E. Couper-Kuhlen & M. Selting (Eds.), Prosody in conversation (pp. 11–56). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Diezinger, F., & Anderson, J. F.
(1986) Starting from scratch: A first look at a “displacement activity” in group-living rhesus monkeys. American Journal of Primatology, 11, 117–124. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dittman, A. T.
(1972) The body movement-speech rhythm relationship as a cue to speech encoding. In A. W. Siegman & B. Pope (Eds.), Studies in dyadic communication (pp. 135–151). New York: Pergamon. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Duncan, S. D.
(1972) Some signals and rules for taking speaking turns in conversations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 23, 283–292. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1974) On the structure of speaker-auditor interaction during speaking turns. Language and Society, 3, 161–180. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Duncan, S. J., & Niederehe, G.
(1974) On signalling that it’s your turn to speak. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 10, 234–247. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Duncan, S. J., & Fiske, D. W.
(1977) Face-to-face interaction: Research, methods, and theory. New York: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Easley, S. P., Coelho, A. M., & Taylor, L. L.
(1987) Scratching, dominance, tension, and displacement in male baboons. American Journal of Primatology, 13, 397–411. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P.
(2004) Emotional and conversational nonverbal signals. In J. M. Larrazabal & L. A. Perez Miranda (Eds.), Language, knowledge, and representation (pp. 39–50). Alphen aan den Rijn: Kluwer Academic Publishers. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V.
(1969) The repertoire of nonverbal behavior: Categories, origins, usage and coding. Semiotica, 1, 49–98. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1972) Hand movements. Journal of communication, 22(4), 353–374. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Feyereisen, P., & De Lannoy, J. D.
(1991) Gestures and speech: Psychological investigations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fischer-Lokou, J., Martin, A., Guéguen, N., & Lamy, L.
(2011) Mimicry and propagation of prosocial behavior in a natural setting. Psychological reports, 108(2), 599–605. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Freedman, N.
(1972) The analysis of movement behavior during the clinical interview. In A. Seigman & B. Pope (Eds.), Studies in Dyadic Communication (pp. 153–175). New York: Pergamon Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Friesen, W. V., Ekman, P., & Wallbott, H.
(1979) Measuring hand movements. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 4(2), 97–112. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gambi, C., Jachmann, T. K., & Staudte, M.
(2015) The role of prosody and Gaze in turn-end anticipation. In D. C. Noelle, R. Dale, A. S. Warlaumont, J. Yoshimi, T. Matlock, C. D. Jennings, & P. Maglio (Eds.), Proceedings of the annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 764–769). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Gardner, R.
(2001) When listeners talk: Response tokens and listener stance, Vol. 92. Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins Publishing. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Garnefski, N., Teerds, J., Kraaij, V., Legerstee, J., & van den Kommer, T.
(2004) Cognitive emotion regulation strategies and depressive symptoms: Differences between males and females. Personality and Individual Differences, 36(2), 267–276. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Garrod, S., & Pickering, M. J.
(2004) Why is conversation so easy? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(1), 8–11. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2009) Joint action, interactive alignment, and dialog. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1(2), 292–304. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gleitman, L. R., January, D., Nappa, R., & Trueswell, J. C.
(2007) On the give and take between event apprehension and utterance formulation. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 544–569. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, S., & Rosenthal, R.
(1986) Self-touching behavior in the job interview: Antecedents and consequences. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 10(1), 65–80. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., & Singer, M. A.
(2003) From children’s hands to adults’ ears: Gesture’s role in teaching and learning. Developmental Psychology, 39, 509–520. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, C.
(1980) Restarts, pauses, and the achievement of a state of mutual gaze at turn-beginning. Sociological Inquiry, 50, 272–302. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grand, S.
(1977) On Hand movements during speech: Studies of the role of self-stimulation in communication under conditions of psychopathology, sensory deficit, and bilingualism. In N. Freedman & S. Grand (Eds.), Communicative structures and psychic structures: A psychoanalytic interpretation of communication (pp. 199–221). New York: Plenum Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Griffin, Z. M., & Bock, K.
(2000) What the eyes say about speaking. Psychological Science, 4, 274–279. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hadidian, J.
(1980) Yawning in an old world monkey, Macaca nigra (Primates: Cercopithecidae). Behaviour, 75, 133–147. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Harrigan, J. A., Kues, J. R., Steffen, J. J., & Rosenthal, R.
(1987) Self-touching and impressions of others. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 13, 497–512. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hatta, T., & Dimond, S. J.
(1984) Differences in face touching by japanese and British people. Neuropsychologia, 22, 531–534. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Heldner, M., & Edlund, J.
(2010) Pauses, gaps and overlaps in conversations. Journal of Phonetics, 38, 555–568. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ho, S., Foulsham, T., & Kingstone, A.
(2015) Speaking and listening with the eyes: Gaze signaling during dyadic interactions. PLoS ONE, 10(8), e0136905. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Keitel, A., & Daum, M. M.
(2015) The use of intonation for turn anticipation in observed conversations without visual signals as source of information. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 108. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kendon, A.
(1967) Some functions of gaze direction in social interaction. Acta Psychologica, 26, 22–63. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1972) Some relationships between body motion and speech. An analysis of an example. In A. Seigman & B. Pope (Eds.), Studies in dyadic communication (pp. 177–210). New York: Pergamon Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1980) Gesticulation and speech: Two aspects of the process of utterance. In M. R. Key (Ed.), The relationship of verbal and nonverbal communication (pp. 207–227). The Hague: Mouton and Co.Google Scholar
(2004) Gesture. Visible action as utterance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2010) Spacing and orientation in co-present interaction. In A. N. Carl Vogel & A. H. A. Nijholt (Eds.), Development of multimodal interfaces: Active listening and synchrony lecture notes in computer science (pp. 1–15). Berlin-Heidelberg: Springer. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Krebs, J. R., & Dawkins, R.
(1984) Animal signals: Mind-reading and manipulation. In J. R. Krebs & N. B. Davies (Eds.), Behavioural ecology: An evolutionary approach (pp. 380–402). Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications.Google Scholar
Kummer, H.
(1968) Social organization of Hamadrys Baboons: A field study. Chicago: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
Lakin, J. L., Jefferis, V. E., Cheng, C. M., & Chartrand, T. L.
(2003) The chameleon effect as social glue: Evidence for the evolutionary significance of nonconscious mimicry. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 27(3), 145–162. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lammertink, I., Casillas, M., Benders, T., Post, B., & Fikkert, P.
(2015) Dutch and English toddlers’ use of linguistic cues in predicting upcoming turn transitions. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 495. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lerner, G. H.
(2003) Selecting next speaker: The context-sensitive operation of a context-free organization. Language in Society, 32, 177–201. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Levelt, W. M.
(1999) Producing spoken language: A blueprint of the speaker. In C. Brown & P. Hagoort (Eds.), The neurocognition of language (pp. 83–122). Oxford: Oxford Press.Google Scholar
Levinson, S.
(2006) On the human interactional engine. In N. J. Enfield & S. Levinson (Eds.), Roots of human sociality. Culture, cognition and human interaction (pp. 39–69). New York: Berg.Google Scholar
Levinson, S., & Torreira, F.
(2015) Timing in turn-taking and its implications for processing models of language. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 731. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Local, J. K., Kelly, J., & Wells, W. H.
(1986) Towards a phonology of conversation: turn-taking in Tyneside English. Journal of Linguistics, 22(2), 411–437. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Maestripieri, D., Schino, G., Aureli, F., & Troisi, A.
(1992) A modest proposal: displacement activities as an indicator of emotions in primates. Animal Behaviour, 44, 967–979. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mahl, G. F.
(1966) Expression of the unverbalized in autistic actions: Psychoanalytic observations. Paper prepared for the Third Research in Psychotherapy Conference . Chicago, June 1-4.
McFarland, D. J.
(1983) Functional analysis of competing homeostatic systems. In J. R. S. Hales (Ed.), Thermal physiology (pp. 299–307). New York: Raven Press.Google Scholar
McNeill, D.
(1992) Hand and mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Neuberg, S. L., & Cottrell, C. A.
(2002) Intergroup emotions: A biocultural approach. In D. M. Mackie & E. R. Smith (Eds.), From prejudice to intergroup emotions: Differentiated reactions to social groups (pp. 265–283). London-New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Pavani, S., Maestripieri, D., Schino, G., Turilazzi, P. G., & Scucchi, S.
(1991) Factors influencing scratching behaviour in long-tailed Macaques (Macaca fascicularis) . Folia Primatologica, 57, 34–38. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J.
(1955) The language and thought of the child. New York: Noonday Press.Google Scholar
Riest, C., Jorschick, A. B., & De Ruiter, J. P.
(2015) Anticipation in turn-taking: Mechanisms and information sources. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 89. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, S. G., Torreira, F., & Levinson, S. C.
(2015) The effects of processing and sequence organization on the timing of turn taking: A corpus study. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 509. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rosenfeld, H. M.
(1966) Approval-seeking and approval-inducing function of verbal and nonverbal responses in dyads. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4(6), 597–605. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rossano, F.
(2013) Sequence organization and timing of bonobo mother-infant interactions. Interaction Studies, 14, 160–189. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rögels, P. L. J. M., Roelen, E., & Van Meel, J. M.
(1990) The function of self-touching, posture shift, and motor discharge in children from 3 to 6 years of age. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 70, 1169–1178. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ruiter, de, J. P., Mitterer, H., & Enfield, N. J.
(2006) Projecting the end of a speaker's turn: A cognitive cornerstone of conversation. Language, 82, 515–535. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sacks, H., Schegloff, E., & Jefferson, G.
(1974) A simplest systematic for the organization of turn-taking in conversation. Language, 50, 696–735. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sacks, H., & Schegloff, E. A.
(2002) Home position. Gesture, 2(2), 133–146. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schaller, G. B.
(1963) The mountain gorilla. Chicago: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
Scheflen, A. E.
(1964) The significance of posture in communication systems. Psychiatry: Journal for the Study of Interpersonal Processes, 27(4), 316–331. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schegloff, E. A.
(1996) Turn organization: One intersection of grammar and interaction. Studies in Interactional Sociolinguistics, 13, 52–133.Google Scholar
(1998) Body torque. Social Research, 65, 535–596.Google Scholar
(2000) Overlapping talk and the organization of turn-taking for conversation. Language and Society, 29, 1–63. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schino, G., Scucchi, S., Maestripieri, D., & Turillazzi, P. G.
(1988) Allogrooming as a tension-reduction mechanism: A behavioral approach. American Journal of Primatology, 16, 43–50. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schino, G., Troisi, A., Perretta, G., & Monaco, V.
(1991) Measuring anxiety in nonhuman primates: Effects of lorazepam on macaque scratching. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 38, 889–891. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Selting, M.
(1996) On the interplay of syntax and prosody in the constitution of turn-constructional units and turns in conversation. Pragmatics, 6, 357–388. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sloetjes, H., & Wittenburg, P.
(2008) Annotation by category: ELAN and ISO DCR. In Proceedings of the Sixth International Language Resources and Evaluation (LRECí08), Marrakech, Morocco, May.
Smuts, B. B.
(1985) Sex and friendship in baboons. New York: Aldine.Google Scholar
Sperber, D., & Wilson, D.
(1995) Relevance: Communication and cognition (2nd ed.). Oxford/Cambridge: Blackwell Publishers.Google Scholar
Stivers, T., Enfield, N. J., Brown, P., Englert, C., Hayashi, M., Heinemann, T., Hoymann, G., Rossano, F., Ruijter, J. P. de, Yoon, K-E., & Levinson, S. C.
(2009) Universals and cultural variation in turn-taking in conversation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(26), 10587–10592. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Streeck, J., & Hartge, U.
(1992) Gestures at the transition place. In P. Auer & A. Di Luzio (Eds.), The contextualization of language (pp. 135–157). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Strömbergsson, S., Hjalmarsson, A., Edlund, J., & House, D.
(2013) Timing responses to questions in dialogue. In Proceedings of Interspeech 2013 (pp. 2584–2588). Lyon: International Speech Communication Association.
Thomas, A. P., & Bull, P.
(1981) The role of pre‐speech posture change in dyadic interaction. British Journal of Social Psychology, 20(2), 105–111. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tinbergen, N.
(1951) The study of instinct. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Torreira, F., Bögels, S., & Levinson, S.
(2015) Breathing for answering: The time course of response planning in conversation. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 284. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wacewicz, S., Żywiczyński, P., & Chiera, A.
(2017) An evolutionary approach to low-level conversational cooperation. Language Sciences, 6, 91–104. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Waxer, P. H.
(1977) Nonverbal cues for anxiety: An examination of emotional leakage. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 36, 306–313. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Weilhammer, K., & Rabold, S.
(2003) Durational aspects in Turn Taking. In Proceedings of the International Conference of Phonetic Sciences, Barcelona, Spain.
Wells, B., & Peppé, S.
(1996) Ending up in Ulster: Prosody and turn-taking in English dialects. Studies in Interactional Sociolinguistics, 12, 101–130.Google Scholar
Zeigler, H. P.
(1964) Displacement activity and motivational theory: A case study in the history of ethology. Psychological Bulletin, 61, 362–376. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Żywiczyński, P., Orzechowski, S., & Wacewicz, S.
(2017) Self-regulators–A hidden dimension of interaction: Movement similarity and temporal proximity increase the perception of interpersonal coordination in third party observers. Language Communication, 54, 82–90. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 2 other publications

Wacewicz, Sławomir & Przemysław Żywiczyński
2018. Language origins. Interaction Studies. Social Behaviour and Communication in Biological and Artificial Systems 19:1-2  pp. 167 ff. Crossref logo
Wacewicz, Sławomir & Przemysław Żywiczyński
2020.  In How the Brain Got Language – Towards a New Road Map [Benjamins Current Topics, 112],  pp. 167 ff. Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 06 december 2021. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.