References

References

Ambridge, B., Theakston. A.L., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M.
(2006) The distributed learning effect for children’s acquisition of an abstract grammatical construction. Cognitive Development, 21, p. 74–193. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bever, T.G.
(1970) The cognitive basis for linguistic structures. In J. R. Hayes (Ed.), Cognition and the development of language (pp. 279–362). New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bielec, D.
(1998) Polish: An essential grammar. London: Rutledge.[ p. 250 ]Google Scholar
Birdsong, D.
(1992) Ultimate attainment in second language acquisition. Language, 68, p. 706–755. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2004) Second language acquisition and ultimate attainment. In A. Davies & C. Elder (Eds.), Handbook of applied linguistics (pp. 82–105). London: Blackwell. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2006) Age and second language acquisition and processing. A selective overview. Language Learning, 56, p. 9–46. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Birdsong, D., & Molis, M.
(2001) On the evidence for maturational constraints in second language acquisition. Journal of Memory and Language, 44, p. 235–249. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bley-Vroman, R.
(1990) The logical problem of foreign language learning. Linguistic Analysis, 20, p. 3–49.Google Scholar
(2009) The evolving context of the fundamental difference hypothesis. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 31, p. 175–198. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Brooks. P.J., & Sekerina. I.A.
(2005/2006) Shortcuts to quantifier interpretation in children and adults. Language Acquisition, 13, p. 177–206. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Brooks, P.J., & Sekerina, I.A.
(2006) Shallow processing of universal quantification: A comparison of monolingual and bilingual adults. Proceedings of the 28th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 2450).Google Scholar
Bybee, J.
(1995) Regular morphology and the lexicon. Language and Cognitive Processes, 10, p. 425–455. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2001) Phonology and language use. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cacioppo, J.T., Petty, R.E., & Kao, C.F.
(1984) The efficient assessment of need for cognition. Journal of Personality Assessment, 48, p. 306–307. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cattell, R.B., & Cattell, H.E. P.
(1973) Measuring intelligence with the culture-fair tests. Champaign, IL: Institute for Personality and Ability Testing.Google Scholar
Chipere, N.
(2001) Native speaker variations in syntactic competence: Implications for first language teaching. Language Awareness, 10, p. 107–124. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Coppieters, R.
(1987) Competence differences between native and near-native speakers. Language, 63, p. 544–573. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cowart, W.
(1997) Experimental syntax: Applying objective methods to sentence judgements. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Crain, S., & Lillo-Martin, D.
(1999) An introduction to linguistic theory and language acquisition. Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Crain, S., Thornton, R., Boster, C., Conway, L., Lillo-Martin, D., & Woodams, E.
(1996) Quantification without qualification. Language Acquisition, 5, p. 83–153. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dąbrowska, E.
(1997) The LAD goes to school: A cautionary tale for nativists. Linguistics, 35, p. 735–766. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2001) Learning a morphological system without a default: The Polish genitive. Journal of Child Language, 28, p. 545–574. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2004) Language, mind and brain: Some psychological and neurological constraints on theories of grammar. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
(2008a) The later development of an early-emerging system: The curious case of the Polish genitive. Linguistics, 46, p. 629–650. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2008b) The effects of frequency and neighbourhood density on adult speakers’ productivity with Polish case inflections: An empirical test of usage-based approaches to morphology. Journal of Memory and Language, 58, p. 931–951. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2009) Constructing a second language: Some final thoughts. Annual Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 7, p. 277–290.[ p. 251 ] CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dąbrowska, E., & Street, J.
(2006) Individual differences in language attainment: Comprehension of passive sentences by native and non-native English speakers. Language Sciences, 28, p. 604–615. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dąbrowska, E., Farmer, T. A., & Street, J.
in progress). (Non)Convergence in L1 and L2 acquisition.
Dąbrowska, E., & Szczerbiñski, M.
(2006) Polish children’s productivity with case marking: the role of regularity, type frequency, and phonological coherence. Journal of Child Language, 33, p. 559–597. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Farmer, T.A., Misyak, J.B., & Christiansen, M.H.
in press). Individual differences in sentence processing. In M. Spivey, M. Joannisse, & K. McRae Eds. Cambridge handbook of psycholinguistics Cambridge Cambridge University Press Crossref
Ferreira, F.
(2003) The misinterpretation of noncanonical sentences. Cognitive Psychology, 47, p. 164–203. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ferreira, F., Bailey. K.G.D., & Ferraro, V.
(2002) Good-enough representations in language comprehension. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, p. 11–15. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ferreira, F., & Patson. N.D.
(2007) The ‘good enough’ approach to language comprehension. Language and Linguistics Compass, 1, p. 71–83. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Flege, J.E., Yeni-Komshian, G.H., & Liu, S.
(1999) Age constraints on second-language acquisition. Journal of Memory and Language, 41, p. 78–104. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S., Pickering, S., Knight, C., & Stegmann, Z.
(2004) Working memory skills and educational attainment: Evidence from national curriculum assessments at 7 and 14 years of age. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, p. 1–16. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Geurts, B.
(2003) Quantifying kids. Language Acquisition, 11, p. 197–218. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ginsborg, J.
(2006) The effects of socio-economic status on children’s language acquisition and use. In J. Clegg & J. Ginsborg (Eds.), Language and social disadvantage (pp. 9–27). Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Gordon, P., & Chafetz, J.
(1990) Verb-based versus class-based accounts of actionality effects in children’s comprehension of passives. Cognition, 36, p. 227–254. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gries, S. Th., & Stefanowitsch, A.
(2004) Extending collostructional analysis: A corpus-based perspective on ‘alternations’. International Journal of Corpus Linguistics 9, p. 97–129. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hart, B., & Risley, R.R.
(1999) The social world of children learning to talk. Baltimore, MD: Paul Brookes.Google Scholar
Hart, B., & Risley,T.R.
(1995) Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Paul Brooks.Google Scholar
Herschensohn, J.
(2009) Fundamental and gradient differences in language development. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 31, p. 259–289. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., Cymerman, E., & Levine, S.
(2002) Language input and child syntax. Cognitive Psychology, 45, p. 337–374. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, J., & Newport, E.
(1989) Critical period effects in second language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language. Cognitive Psychology, 21, p. 60–99. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Just, M.A., & Carpenter, P.A.
(1992) A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review, 99, p. 122–149. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
King, J., & Just, M.A.
(1991) Individual differences in syntactic processing: The role of working memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, p. 580–602. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lidz, J., & Williams, A.
(2009) Constructions on holiday. Cognitive Linguistics, 20, p. 177–189.[ p. 252 ] CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Locke, A., & Ginsborg, J.
(2003) Spoken language in the early years: the cognitive and linguistic development of three- to five-year-old children from socio-economically deprived backgrounds. Educational and Child Psychology, 20, p. 68–79.Google Scholar
MacDonald, M.C., & Christiansen, M.H.
(2002) Reassessing working memory: Comment on Just and Carpenter (1992) and Waters and Caplan (1996). Psychological Review, 109, p. 35–54. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Maratsos, M., Fox, D.E.C., Becker, J.A., & Chalkley, M.A.
(1985) Some restrictions on children’s passives. Cognition, 19, p. 167–191. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S.
(2008) Incomplete acquisition in bilingualism: Re-examining the age factor. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nowak, M.A., Komarova, N.L., & Niyogi, P.
(2001) Evolution of Universal Grammar. Science, 291, p. 114–118. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
O’Grady, W., Lee, O.-S., & Lee, J.-H.
(2011) Practical and theoretical issues in the study of language acquisition. Heritage Language Journal, 8, p. 23–40.Google Scholar
Orzechowska, A.
(1998) Rzeczownik. In R. Grzegorczykowa, R. Laskowski, & H. Wróbel (Eds.), Gramatyka współczesnego języka polskiego: Morfologia (2nd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 270–332). Warszawa: PWN.Google Scholar
Otwinowska-Kaszelanic, A.
(2000) Korpus języka mówionege młodego pokolenia Polaków. Warszawa: Wydawnictwo Akademickie DIALOG.Google Scholar
Pakulak, E., & Neville, H.
(2011) Maturational constraints on the recruitment of early processes for syntactic processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, p. 2752–2765. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pearlmutter, N.J., & MacDonald, M.C.
(1995.) Individual differences and probabilistic constraints in syntactic ambiguity resolution. Journal of Memory and Language, 34, p. 521–542.Google Scholar
Perfetti, C.A., & McCutchen, D.
(1987) Schooled language competence: Linguistic abilities in reading and writing. In S. Rosenberg (Ed.), Advances in applied psycholinguistics: Reading, writing, and language learning (Vol. 2, pp. 105–141). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Pinker, S., Lebeaux. D.S., & Frost, L.A.
(1987) Productivity and constraints in the acquisition of the passive. Cognition, 26, p. 195–267. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ross, J.R.
(1979) Where’s English? In C. J. Fillmore, D. Kempler, & W. S.-Y. Wang (Eds.), Individual differences in language ability and language behavior (pp. 127–163). New York, NY: Academic Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, R.W.
(1990) The role of consciousness in second language learning. Applied Linguistics, 11, p. 129–158. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schütze, C.T.
(1996) The Empirical Base of Linguistics: Grammaticality Judgments and Linguistic Methodology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Seidenberg, M.S.
(1997) Language acquisition and use: learning and applying probabilistic constraints. Science, 275, p. 1599–1603. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Skehan, P.
(1989) Individual differences in second-language learning. London: Edward Arnold.Google Scholar
Smoczyñska, M.
(1985) The acquisition of Polish. In D. I. Slobin (Ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition: The data (Vol. 1, pp. 595–683). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Sorace, A.
(1993) Incomplete vs divergent representations of unaccusativity in non-native grammars of Italian. Second Language Research, 9, p. 22–47. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Street, J., & Dąbrowska, E.
2010More individual differences in language attainment: How much do adult native speakers of English know about passives and quantifiers? Lingua 120, p. 2080–2094. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
in press). Lexically specific knowledge and individual differences in adult native speakers’ processing of the English passive. Applied Psycholinguistics.[ p. 253 ]
Swets, B., Desmet, T., Hambrick. D.Z., & Ferreira, F.
(2007) The role of working memory in syntactic ambiguity resolution: A psychometric approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, p. 64–81. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M.
(2003) Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of child language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Townsend, D.J., & Bever, T.G.
(2001) Sentence comprehension: Theintegration of habits and rules. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Waters, G.S., & Caplan, D.
(1996) The measurement of verbal working memory capacity and its relation to reading comprehension. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49, p. 51–79. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wells, J.B., Christiansen, M.H., Race, D.S., Acheson, D.J., & MacDonald, M.C.
(2009) Experience and sentence processing: Statistical learning and relative clause comprehension. Cognitive Psychology, 58, p. 250–271. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 101 other publications

No author info given
2015.  In Demonstratives and Possessives with Attitude [Human Cognitive Processing, 51], Crossref logo
No author info given
2015.  In Language Proficiency in Native and Non-native Speakers [Language Learning & Language Teaching, 41], Crossref logo
No author info given
2019.  In Sensory Linguistics [Converging Evidence in Language and Communication Research, 20], Crossref logo
No author info given
2019.  In Sensory Linguistics [Converging Evidence in Language and Communication Research, 20],  pp. 235 ff. Crossref logo
No author info given
2021.  In Cognitive Aphasiology – A Usage-Based Approach to Language in Aphasia [Constructional Approaches to Language, 31], Crossref logo
Aalberse, Suzanne & Aafke Hulk
2018. Introduction to the special issue – Heritage language studies and early child bilingualism research: Understanding the connection. International Journal of Bilingualism 22:5  pp. 491 ff. Crossref logo
Allen, Shanley E. M.
2017. Comparison as a fruitful way forward. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 7:6  pp. 668 ff. Crossref logo
Andringa, Sible
2014. THE USE OF NATIVE SPEAKER NORMS IN CRITICAL PERIOD HYPOTHESIS RESEARCH. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 36:3  pp. 565 ff. Crossref logo
Andringa, Sible & Ewa Dąbrowska
2019. Individual Differences in First and Second Language Ultimate Attainment and Their Causes. Language Learning 69  pp. 5 ff. Crossref logo
Andrén, Mats
2014. Multimodal constructions in children. Gesture 14:2  pp. 141 ff. Crossref logo
Azaz, Mahmoud & Joshua Frank
2018. Bidirectional cross-linguistic influence in late bilingualism. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 8:4  pp. 411 ff. Crossref logo
Beatty‐Martínez, Anne L., Michelle R. Bruni, María Teresa Bajo & Paola E. Dussias
2021. Brain potentials reveal differential processing of masculine and feminine grammatical gender in native Spanish speakers. Psychophysiology 58:3 Crossref logo
Behrens, Heike
2020.  In Current Perspectives on Child Language Acquisition [Trends in Language Acquisition Research, 27],  pp. 91 ff. Crossref logo
Cacoullos, Rena Torres & Catherine E. Travis
2015. Gauging convergence on the ground: Code-switching in the community. International Journal of Bilingualism 19:4  pp. 365 ff. Crossref logo
Curcic, Maja, Sible Andringa & Folkert Kuiken
2019. The Role of Awareness and Cognitive Aptitudes in L2 Predictive Language Processing. Language Learning 69  pp. 42 ff. Crossref logo
Cuza, Alejandro & Joshua Frank
2015. On the role of experience and age-related effects: Evidence from the Spanish CP. Second Language Research 31:1  pp. 3 ff. Crossref logo
Davidson, Andrew
2019. Writing: the re-construction of language. Language Sciences 72  pp. 134 ff. Crossref logo
Annick De Houwer & Lourdes Ortega
2018.  In The Cambridge Handbook of Bilingualism, Crossref logo
De Smet, Isabeau & Freek Van de Velde
2020. Semantic differences between strong and weak verb forms in Dutch. Cognitive Linguistics 31:3  pp. 393 ff. Crossref logo
DeKeyser, Robert M.
2013. Age Effects in Second Language Learning: Stepping Stones Toward Better Understanding. Language Learning 63  pp. 52 ff. Crossref logo
DeLuca, Vincent, David Miller, Christos Pliatsikas & Jason Rothman
2019.  In The Handbook of the Neuroscience of Multilingualism,  pp. 170 ff. Crossref logo
Diessel, Holger
2019.  In The Grammar Network, Crossref logo
Ditges, Ruth, Elena Barbieri, Cynthia K. Thompson, Sandra Weintraub, Cornelius Weiller, Marek-Marsel Mesulam, Dorothee Kümmerer, Nils Schröter & Mariacristina Musso
2021. German Language Adaptation of the NAVS (NAVS-G) and of the NAT (NAT-G): Testing Grammar in Aphasia. Brain Sciences 11:4  pp. 474 ff. Crossref logo
Divjak, Dagmar, Ewa Dąbrowska & Antti Arppe
2016. Machine Meets Man: Evaluating the Psychological Reality of Corpus-based Probabilistic Models. Cognitive Linguistics 27:1  pp. 1 ff. Crossref logo
Dunn, Jonathan
2018. Finding variants for construction-based dialectometry: A corpus-based approach to regional CxGs. Cognitive Linguistics 29:2  pp. 275 ff. Crossref logo
Dyson, Bronwen Patricia
2016. Variation, individual differences and second language processing. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 6:4  pp. 341 ff. Crossref logo
Dąbrowska, Ewa
2016.  In Studies in Lexicogrammar [Human Cognitive Processing, 54],  pp. 55 ff. Crossref logo
Dąbrowska, Ewa
2018. Experience, aptitude and individual differences in native language ultimate attainment. Cognition 178  pp. 222 ff. Crossref logo
Dąbrowska, Ewa
2019. Experience, Aptitude, and Individual Differences in Linguistic Attainment: A Comparison of Native and Nonnative Speakers. Language Learning 69  pp. 72 ff. Crossref logo
Favier, Saoradh & Falk Huettig
2021. Are there core and peripheral syntactic structures? Experimental evidence from Dutch native speakers with varying literacy levels. Lingua 251  pp. 102991 ff. Crossref logo
Favier, Saoradh & Falk Huettig
2021. Long-term written language experience affects grammaticality judgements and usage but not priming of spoken sentences. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology  pp. 174702182110052 ff. Crossref logo
Flores, Cristina, Tanja Kupisch & Esther Rinke
2016.  In Handbook of Comparative Studies on Community Colleges and Global Counterparts [Springer International Handbooks of Education, ],  pp. 1 ff. Crossref logo
Flores, Cristina, Tanja Kupisch & Esther Rinke
2018.  In Handbook of Research and Practice in Heritage Language Education [Springer International Handbooks of Education, ],  pp. 621 ff. Crossref logo
Fonteyn, Lauren & Andrea Nini
2020. Individuality in syntactic variation: An investigation of the seventeenth-century gerund alternation . Cognitive Linguistics 31:2  pp. 279 ff. Crossref logo
Foucart, Alice
2015.  Prediction is a question of experience . Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 5:4  pp. 465 ff. Crossref logo
Frank, Joshua
2020. The Effect of Silence and Distance: Evidence from Recomplementation in US Heritage Spanish. Languages 5:4  pp. 66 ff. Crossref logo
Gablasova, Dana, Vaclav Brezina & Tony McEnery
2017. Exploring Learner Language Through Corpora: Comparing and Interpreting Corpus Frequency Information. Language Learning 67:S1  pp. 130 ff. Crossref logo
Garibyan, Armine, Evelin Balog & Thomas Herbst
2019. L2-constructions that go together – more on valency constructions and learner language. Yearbook of the German Cognitive Linguistics Association 7:1  pp. 9 ff. Crossref logo
Geeraert, Kristina , John Newman & R. Harald Baayen
2020. Variation Within Idiomatic Variation: Exploring the Differences Between Speakers and Idioms. East European Journal of Psycholinguistics 7:2 Crossref logo
Gonzales, Kalim, LouAnn Gerken & Rebecca L. Gómez
2018. How who is talking matters as much as what they say to infant language learners. Cognitive Psychology 106  pp. 1 ff. Crossref logo
Grant, Angela, Kristina Coulter & Natalie Phillips
2020.  In The Cambridge Handbook of Cognitive Aging,  pp. 480 ff. Crossref logo
Grüter, Theres
2013.  Individual differences and the nature of the processor. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 3:3  pp. 312 ff. Crossref logo
Hager, Malin & Natascha Müller
2015. Ultimate attainment in bilingual first language acquisition. Lingua 164  pp. 289 ff. Crossref logo
Haim, Orly
2021. Exploring perceived multilingual proficiency among immigrant youth with different arrival ages in the destination country. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development  pp. 1 ff. Crossref logo
Hartmann, Stefan & Michael Pleyer
2021. Constructing a protolanguage: reconstructing prehistoric languages in a usage-based construction grammar framework. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 376:1824 Crossref logo
Hautasaari, Ari & Naomi Yamashita
2015.  In Human-Computer Interaction – INTERACT 2015 [Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 9296],  pp. 183 ff. Crossref logo
Heyes, Cecilia
2019. Testing cognitive gadgets. Mind & Language Crossref logo
Hoffmann, Thomas
2018. Creativity and Construction Grammar: Cognitive and Psychological Issues. Zeitschrift für Anglistik und Amerikanistik 66:3  pp. 259 ff. Crossref logo
Hulstijn, Jan H.
2015.  In Implicit and Explicit Learning of Languages [Studies in Bilingualism, 48],  pp. 25 ff. Crossref logo
Hulstijn, Jan H.
2019. An Individual-Differences Framework for Comparing Nonnative With Native Speakers: Perspectives From BLC Theory. Language Learning 69  pp. 157 ff. Crossref logo
Ibbotson, Paul, Diana G. López & Alan J. McKane
2018. Goldilocks Forgetting in Cross-Situational Learning. Frontiers in Psychology 9 Crossref logo
Janda, Laura A.
2019. Quantitative perspectives in Cognitive Linguistics. Review of Cognitive Linguistics 17:1  pp. 7 ff. Crossref logo
Janda, Laura A. & Robert J. Reynolds
2019. Construal vs. redundancy: Russian aspect in context. Cognitive Linguistics 30:3  pp. 467 ff. Crossref logo
Johns, Michael A, Jorge R Valdés Kroff & Paola E Dussias
2019. Mixing things up: How blocking and mixing affect the processing of codemixed sentences. International Journal of Bilingualism 23:2  pp. 584 ff. Crossref logo
Kapatsinski, Vsevolod
2018.  In The Construction of Words [Studies in Morphology, 4],  pp. 547 ff. Crossref logo
Kidd, Evan & Seamus Donnelly
2020. Individual Differences in First Language Acquisition. Annual Review of Linguistics 6:1  pp. 319 ff. Crossref logo
Koornneef, Arnout & Eric Reuland
2016. On the Shallow Processing (Dis)Advantage: Grammar and Economy. Frontiers in Psychology 7 Crossref logo
Krause, Elif, Tanja Rinker & Carsten Eulitz
2020.  In Studies in Turkish as a Heritage Language [Studies in Bilingualism, 60],  pp. 127 ff. Crossref logo
Kupisch, Tanja & Jason Rothman
2018. Terminology matters! Why difference is not incompleteness and how early child bilinguals are heritage speakers. International Journal of Bilingualism 22:5  pp. 564 ff. Crossref logo
Lahmann, Cornelia, Rasmus Steinkrauss & Monika S. Schmid
2016. Factors Affecting Grammatical and Lexical Complexity of Long-Term L2 Speakers’ Oral Proficiency. Language Learning 66:2  pp. 354 ff. Crossref logo
Linkov, Václav
2017. Psychology is not primarily Empirical Science: A Comparison of Cultures in the Lexical Hypothesis Tradition as a Failure of Introspection. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science 51:2  pp. 285 ff. Crossref logo
Lloyd-Smith, Anika, Fatih Bayram & Michael Iverson
2020.  In Studies in Turkish as a Heritage Language [Studies in Bilingualism, 60],  pp. 63 ff. Crossref logo
López-Beltrán, Priscila & Matthew T. Carlson
2020. How usage-based approaches to language can contribute to a unified theory of heritage grammars. Linguistics Vanguard 6:1 Crossref logo
Matusevych, Yevgen, Afra Alishahi & Ad Backus
2016. Modelling verb selection within argument structure constructions. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 31:10  pp. 1215 ff. Crossref logo
Meisel, Jürgen M.
2017.  In The Handbook of Psycholinguistics,  pp. 635 ff. Crossref logo
Menke, Mandy R. & Paul A. Malovrh
2021.  In Advancedness in Second Language Spanish [Issues in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics, 31],  pp. 18 ff. Crossref logo
Mompean, Jose A.
2021. /r/-sandhi in the speech of Queen Elizabeth II. Journal of the International Phonetic Association  pp. 1 ff. Crossref logo
Montero-Melis, Guillermo
2021. Consistency in Motion Event Encoding Across Languages. Frontiers in Psychology 12 Crossref logo
Mäkilähde, Aleksi
2019.  In Normativity in Language and Linguistics [Studies in Language Companion Series, 209],  pp. 235 ff. Crossref logo
Navarro-Torres, Christian A., Dalia L. Garcia, Vrinda Chidambaram & Judith F. Kroll
2019. Cognitive Control Facilitates Attentional Disengagement during Second Language Comprehension. Brain Sciences 9:5  pp. 95 ff. Crossref logo
Noël, Dirk
2016. For a radically usage-based diachronic construction grammar. Belgian Journal of Linguistics 30  pp. 39 ff. Crossref logo
Noël, Dirk
2017. The development of non-deontic be bound to in a radically usage-based diachronic construction grammar perspective. Lingua 199  pp. 72 ff. Crossref logo
Noël, Dirk
2019. The author and the text in radically usage-based diachronic construction grammar, or why historical linguists have started analysing text again. Functions of Language 26:1  pp. 56 ff. Crossref logo
Noël, Dirk
2019. The decline of the Deontic nci construction in Late Modern English. Cognitive Linguistic Studies 6:1  pp. 22 ff. Crossref logo
Pajunen, Anneli & Esa Itkonen
2019.  In Normativity in Language and Linguistics [Studies in Language Companion Series, 209],  pp. 213 ff. Crossref logo
Perez-Cortes, Putnam & Sánchez
2019. Differential Access: Asymmetries in Accessing Features and Building Representations in Heritage Language Grammars. Languages 4:4  pp. 81 ff. Crossref logo
Pijpops, Dirk & Freek Van de Velde
2016. Constructional contamination: How does it work and how do we measure it?. Folia Linguistica 50:2 Crossref logo
Plath, Jennifer & Dominik Leiss
2018. The impact of linguistic complexity on the solution of mathematical modelling tasks. ZDM 50:1-2  pp. 159 ff. Crossref logo
Quick, Antje Endesfelder, Ad Backus & Elena Lieven
2021. Entrenchment effects in code-mixing: individual differences in German-English bilingual children. Cognitive Linguistics 0:0 Crossref logo
Rothman, J. & J. Treffers-Daller
2014. A Prolegomenon to the Construct of the Native Speaker: Heritage Speaker Bilinguals are Natives Too!. Applied Linguistics 35:1  pp. 93 ff. Crossref logo
Rothman, Jason, Fatih Bayram, Ian Cunnings & Jorge González Alonso
2019.  In The Cambridge Handbook of Language Learning,  pp. 13 ff. Crossref logo
Rothman, Jason, Ianthi Maria Tsimpli & Diego Pascual y Cabo
2016.  In Advances in Spanish as a Heritage Language [Studies in Bilingualism, 49],  pp. 13 ff. Crossref logo
Singleton, David
2019. Discussion. European Journal of Higher Education 9:3  pp. 343 ff. Crossref logo
Smirnova, Elena & Lotte Sommerer
2020.  In Nodes and Networks in Diachronic Construction Grammar [Constructional Approaches to Language, 27],  pp. 2 ff. Crossref logo
Street, James A.
2017. This is the native speaker that the non-native speaker outperformed: Individual, education-related differences in the processing and interpretation of Object Relative Clauses by native and non-native speakers of English. Language Sciences 59  pp. 192 ff. Crossref logo
Street, James A.
2020. More lexically-specific knowledge and individual differences in adult native speakers' processing of the English passive. Language Sciences 78  pp. 101254 ff. Crossref logo
Ströbel, Marcus, Elma Kerz & Daniel Wiechmann
2020. The Relationship Between First and Second Language Writing: Investigating the Effects of First Language Complexity on Second Language Complexity in Advanced Stages of Learning. Language Learning 70:3  pp. 732 ff. Crossref logo
Sullivan, Karen
2015. If you study a word do you use it more often? Lexical repetition priming in a corpus of Natural Semantic Metalanguage publications. Corpora 10:3  pp. 277 ff. Crossref logo
SUN, He, Siew Chin NG, Beth Ann O'BRIEN & Tom FRITZSCHE
2020. Child, family, and school factors in bilingual preschoolers’ vocabulary development in heritage languages. Journal of Child Language 47:4  pp. 817 ff. Crossref logo
SUN, HE, BIN YIN, NUR FARINA BEGUM BINTE AMSAH & BETH ANN O'BRIEN
2018. Differential effects of internal and external factors in early bilingual vocabulary learning: The case of Singapore. Applied Psycholinguistics 39:2  pp. 383 ff. Crossref logo
Tenbrink, Thora
2020.  In Cognitive Discourse Analysis, Crossref logo
Tyler, Andrea E. & Lourdes Ortega
2018.  In Usage-inspired L2 Instruction [Language Learning & Language Teaching, 49],  pp. 3 ff. Crossref logo
Uhrig, Peter
2020. Creative intentions — The fine line between ‘creative’ and ‘wrong’. Cognitive Semiotics 13:1 Crossref logo
van Hell, Janet G. & Fatemeh Abdollahi
2017.  In Developmental Perspectives in Written Language and Literacy, Crossref logo
Vanhove, Jan
2020. When labeling L2 users as nativelike or not, consider classification errors. Second Language Research 36:4  pp. 709 ff. Crossref logo
Verhagen, Véronique & Maria Mos
2016. Stability of familiarity judgments: Individual variation and the invariant bigger picture. Cognitive Linguistics 27:3  pp. 307 ff. Crossref logo
Vetchinnikova, Svetlana
2019.  In Phraseology and the Advanced Language Learner, Crossref logo
Yang, Xiao, Utako Minai & Robert Fiorentino
2018. Context-Sensitivity and Individual Differences in the Derivation of Scalar Implicature. Frontiers in Psychology 9 Crossref logo
Young-Scholten, Martha
2013. Low-educated immigrants and the social relevance of second language acquisition research. Second Language Research 29:4  pp. 441 ff. Crossref logo
Zenner, Eline, Gitte Kristiansen & Dirk Geeraerts
2016.  In Exploring Discourse Strategies in Social and Cognitive Interaction [Pragmatics & Beyond New Series, 262],  pp. 39 ff. Crossref logo
Zyzik, Eve
2017. Subject expression in L2 Spanish: Convergence of generative and usage-based perspectives?. Second Language Research 33:1  pp. 33 ff. Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 16 april 2021. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.