References

References

Armstrong, M. E.
(2014) Child comprehension of intonationally-encoded disbelief. In W. Orman & M.J. Valleau (Eds.), Proceedings of BUCLD 38. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Bottari, P., Cipriani, P., & Chilosi, A.M.
(1993/94) Protosyntactic devices in the acquisition of Italian free morphology. Language Acquisition, 3, 327–369 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bruhn de Garavito, J., & White, L.
(2002) The L2 acquisition of Spanish DPs: The status of grammatical features. In A.T. Pérez-Leroux & J.M. Liceras (Eds.), The acquisition of Spanish morphosyntax (pp. 151–176). Dordrecht: Kluwer. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cuza, A., & Pérez-Tattam, R.
(2015) Grammatical gender selection and phrasal word order in child heritage Spanish: A feature reassembly approach. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Flege, J., & MacKay, I.
(2011) What accounts for “age” effects on overall degree of foreign accent? In M. Wrembel, M. Kul, & K. Dziubalska-Kołaczyk (Eds.), Achievements and perspectives in the acquisition of second language speech: New Sounds 2010 (Vol. 2, pp. 65–82). Bern, Switzerland: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Fodor, J.D.
(1998) Parsing to learn. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 27(3), 339–374. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. D.
(2002) Psycholinguistics cannot escape prosody. In Proceedings of the SPEECH PROSODY 2002 Conference, Aix-en-Provence, France, April 2002. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Guasti, M.T., Gavarrò, A.J., De Lange, C., & Caprin, C.
(2008) Article Omission across child languages. Language Acquisition, 15(2), 89–119. Crossref[ p. 515 ]Google Scholar
Grüter, T., Lew-Wiliams, C., & Fernald, A.
(2012) Grammatical gender in L2: A production or a real-time processing problem. Second Language Research, 28, 191–215. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Guillelmon, D., & Grosjean, F.
(2001) The gender marking effect in spoken word recognition: the case of bilinguals. Memory and Cognition, 29, 503–511. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jia, G.
(1998) Beyond brain maturation: the critical period hypothesis in second language acquisition revisited. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. New York University.
Kuchenbrandt, I.
(2005) Gender acquisition in bilingual Spanish. In J. Cohen, K.T. McAlister, K. Rolstad, & J. MacSwan (Eds.), ISB4: Proceedings of the 4th International Symposium on Bilingualism (pp. 1252–1263). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Lleó, C., & Demuth, K.
(1999) Prosodic constraints on the emergence of grammatical morphemes: Crosslinguistic evidence from Germanic and Romance languages. In A. Greenhill, H. Littlefield, & C. Tano (Eds.), Proceedings of the 23rd Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 407–418). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
López-Ornat, S.
(1997) What Lies in between a pre-grammatical and a grammatical representation? In A.T. Pérez-Leroux & W.R. Glass (Eds.), Contemporary perspectives on the acquisition of Spanish, volume 1: developing grammars (pp. 3–20). Somerville: Cascadilla Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, F.
(2010) Real-time processing of gender-marked articles by native and non-native Spanish speakers. Journal of Memory and Language, 63, 447–464. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pórez-Leroux, A.T., & Thomas, D.
(2013) Comparing second language learners to other populations: Age, transfer, and learnability. In K. Geeslin (Ed.), Handbook of second language spanish (pp. 423–445). Walden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Phillips, C., & Ehrenhofer, L.
(2015) The role of language processing in language acquisition. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Snyder, W., Senghas, A., & Inman, K.
(2001) Agreement morphology and the acquisition of noun-drop in Spanish. Language Acquisition, 9, 157–173. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Yang, C.
(2002) Knowledge and learning in natural language. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar