References
Artero, S., Petersen, R., Touchon, J., & Ritchie, K.
(2006) Revised criteria for mild cognitive impairment: Validation within a longitudinal population study. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 22(5–6), 465–470. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Aurtenetxe, S., García-Pacios, J., del Río, D., López, M. E., Pineda-Pardo, J. A., Marcos, A., … Maestú, F.
(2016) Interference impacts working memory in mild cognitive impairment. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 10(OCT). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. H.
(2008) Analyzing linguistic data: A practical introduction to statistics using R. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M.
(2008) Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 390–412. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J.
(2013) Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 63(3), 255–278. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Baudiffier, V., Caplan, D., Gaonac’h, D., & Chesnet, D.
(2011) The effect of noun animacy on the processing of unambiguous sentences: Evidence from French relative clauses. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(10), 1896–1905. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Betancort, M., Carreiras, M., & Sturt, P.
(2009) The processing of subject and object relative clauses in Spanish: An eye-tracking study. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(10), 1915–1929. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bickel, C., Pantel, J., Eysenbach, K., & Schröder, J.
(2000) Syntactic comprehension deficits in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain and Language, 71(3), 432–448. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Borella, E., Carretti, B., & De Beni, R.
(2008) Working memory and inhibition across the adult life-span. Acta Psychologica, 128(1), 33–44. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Caplan, D., DeDe, G., Waters, G., Michaud, J., & Tripodis, Y.
(2011) Effects of age, speed of processing, and working memory on comprehension of sentences with relative clauses. Psychology and Aging, 26(2), 439–450. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Caplan, D., & Waters, G. S.
(2013) Memory mechanisms supporting syntactic comprehension. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(2), 243–268. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1999) Verbal working memory and sentence comprehension. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 22, 77–126. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Casado, P., Martín-Loeches, M., Muñoz, F., & Fernandez-Frías, C.
(2005) Are semantic and syntactic cues inducing the same processes in the identification of word order? Cognitive Brain Research, 24(3), 526–543. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chafetz, M. D., & Matthews, L. H.
(2004) A new interference score for the Stroop test. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology: The Official Journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists, 19(4), 555–567. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Croot, K., Hodges, J. R., & Patterson, K.
(1999) Evidence for impaired sentence comprehension in early Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 5(5), 393–404. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Vincenzi, M.
(1991) Syntactic parsing strategies in Italian. Dordrecht: Kluwer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1996) Syntactic analysis in sentence comprehension: Effects of dependency types and grammatical constraints. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 25(1), 117–133. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
DeCaro, R., Peelle, J. E., Grossman, M., & Wingfield, A.
(2016) The two sides of sensory-cognitiveiInteractions: Effects of age, hearing acuity, and working memory span on sentence comprehension. Frontiers in Psychology, 7(February), 236. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
DeDe, G., Caplan, D., Kemtes, K., & Waters, G.
(2004) The relationship between age, verbal working memory, and language comprehension. Psychology and Aging, 19(4), 601–616. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
del Río, D., López-Higes, R., & Martín-Aragoneses, M. T. M. T.
(2012) Canonical word order and interference-based integration costs during sentence comprehension: The case of Spanish subject- and object-relative clauses. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006), 65(11), 2108–2128. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
del Río, D., Maestú, F., López-Higes, R., Moratti, S., Gutiérrez, R., Maestú, C., & del-Pozo, F.
(2011) Conflict and cognitive control during sentence comprehension: Recruitment of a frontal network during the processing of Spanish object-first sentences. Neuropsychologia, 49(3), 382–391. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Di Domenico, A., & Di Matteo, R.
(2009) Processing Italian relative clauses: Working memory span and word order effects on RTs. The Journal of General Psychology, 136(4), 387–406. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R.
(1975) “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12(3), 189–198. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A.
(2004) The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(1), 101–135. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibson, E.
(1998) Linguistic complexity: locality of syntactic dependencies. Cognition, 68(1), 1–76. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Golden, C. J.
(2001) Test de colores y palabras Stroop manual. Madrid: TEA.Google Scholar
Goral, M., Clark-Cotton, M., Spiro, A., Obler, L. K., Verkuilen, J., & Albert, M. L.
(2011) The contribution of set Switching and working memory to sentence processing in older adults. Experimental Aging Research, 37(5), 516–538. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grodner, D., & Gibson, E.
(2005) Some consequences of the serial nature of linguistic input. Cognitive Science, 29(2), 261–290. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grossman, M., & Rhee, J.
(2001) Cognitive resources during sentence processing in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychologia, 39(13), 1419–1431. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hartman, M., & Hasher, L.
(1991) Aging and suppression: Memory for previously relevant information. Psychology and Aging, 6(4), 587–594. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hasher, L., Lustig, C., & Zacks, R.
(2007) Inhibitory mechanisms and the control of attention. In A. Conway, C. Jarrold, M. Kane, A. Miyake, & J. Towse (Eds.), Variation in working memory (pp. 227–249). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Homack, S., & Riccio, C. A.
(2004) A meta-analysis of the sensitivity and specificity of the Stroop Color and Word Test with children. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology : The Official Journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists, 19(6), 725–743. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hsu, N. S., & Novick, J. M.
(2016) Dynamic engagement of cognitive control modulates Recovery from misinterpretation during real-time language processing. Psychological Science, 27(4), 572–582. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jaeger, T. F.
(2008) Categorical Data Analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards Logit Mixed Models. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 434–446. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Just, M. A., Carpenter, P. A., Keller, T. A., Eddy, W. F., Thulborn, K. R., & Just, M. A.
(1996) Brain activation modulated by sentence comprehension. Science, 274, 114–116. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Just, M. A., Carpenter, P. A., & Woolley, J. D.
(1982) Paradigms and processes in reading comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 111(2), 228–238. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kemper, S., & Liu, C.-J.
(2007) Eye movements of young and older adults during reading. Psychology and Aging, 22(1), 84–93. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kutas, M., & King, J. W.
(1995) Who did what and when? Using word-and clause-Level ERPs to monitor working memory usage in reading. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7(3), 376–395. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Langenecker, S. A., Nielson, K. A., & Rao, S. M.
(2004) fMRI of healthy older adults during Stroop interference. NeuroImage, 21(1), 192–200. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lewis, R. L., Vasishth, S., & Van Dyke, J. A.
(2006) Computational principles of working memory in sentence comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(10), 447–454. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lobo, A., Saz, P., Marcos, G., Día, J. L., de la Cámara, C., Ventura, T., Aznar, S.
(1999) Revalidation and standardization of the cognition mini-exam (first Spanish version of the Mini-Mental Status Examination) in the general geriatric population. Medicina Clinica, 112(20), 767–774.Google Scholar
López-Higes, R., Prados, J. M., Rubio, S., Montejo, P., & Del Río, D.
(2016) Executive functions and linguistic performance in SCD older adults and healthy controls. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 14(00), 1–18. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
MacDonald, M. C., Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A.
(1992) Working memory constraints on the processing of syntactic ambiguity. Cognitive Psychology, 24(1), 56–98. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B., & Pléh, C.
(1988) The processing of restrictive relative clauses in Hungarian. Cognition, 29(2), 95–141. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mak, W. M., Vonk, W., & Schriefers, H.
(2006) Animacy in processing relative clauses: The hikers that rocks crush. Journal of Memory and Language, 54(4), 466–490. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marková, J., Horváthová, Ľ., Králová, M., & Cséfalvay, Z.
(2017) Sentence comprehension in Slovak-speaking patients with Alzheimer’s disease. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 52(4), 456–468. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nicenboim, B., Logačev, P., Gattei, C., & Vasishth, S.
(2016) When high-capacity readers slow down and low-capacity readers speed up: Working memory and locality effects. Frontiers in Psychology, 7(MAR), 280. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Payne, B. R., Gao, X., Noh, S. R., Anderson, C. J., & Stine-Morrow, E. A. L.
(2012) The effects of print exposure on sentence processing and memory in older adults: Evidence for efficiency and reserve. Neuropsychology, Development, and Cognition. Section B, Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 19(1–2), 122–149. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Payne, B. R., & Stine-Morrow, E. A. L.
(2016) Risk for mild cognitive impairment is associated with semantic integration deficits in sentence processing and memory. Journals of Gerontology - Series B Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 71(2), 243–253. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Petersen, R. C.
(2004) Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. Journal of Internal Medicine, 256(3), 183–194. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pettigrew, C., & Martin, R. C.
(2014) Cognitive declines in healthy aging: Evidence from multiple aspects of interference resolution. Psychology and Aging, 29(2), 187–204. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
R. Core Team
(2018) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.Google Scholar
Reisberg, B., Ferris, S. H., De Leon, M. J., & Crook, T.
(1982) The global deterioration scale for assessment of primary degenerative dementia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 139(9), 1136–1139. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rey-Mermet, A., & Gade, M.
(2018) Inhibition in aging: What is preserved? What declines? A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 25(5), 1695–1716. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rogalsky, C., Matchin, W., & Hickok, G.
(2008) Broca’s area, sentence comprehension, and working memory: An fMRI Study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 2(October), 14. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
See, S. T., & Ryan, E. B.
(1995) Cognitive mediation of adult age differences in language performance. Psychology and Aging, 10(3), 458–468. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sekerina, I. A., Campanelli, L., & Van Dyke, J. A.
(2016) Using the visual world paradigm to study retrieval interference in spoken language comprehension. Frontiers in Psychology, 7(JUN), 1–15. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sheikh, J. I., & Yesavage, J. A.
(1986) Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS) recent evidence and eevelopment of a shorter version. Clinical Gerontologist, 5(1–2), 165–173. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Small, J. A., Kemper, S., & Lyons, K.
(2000) Sentence repetition and processing resources in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain and Language, 75(2), 232–258. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Spieler, D. H., Balota, D. A., & Faust, M. E.
(1996) Stroop performance in healthy younger and older adults and in individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer’s type. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22(2), 461–479. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stine-Morrow, E. A. L., & Payne, B. R.
(2016) Age differences in language segmentation. Experimental Aging Research, 42(1), 83–96. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stine-Morrow, E. A. L., Ryan, S., & Leonard, J. S.
(2000) Age differences in on-line syntactic processing. Experimental Aging Research, 26(December), 315–322. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Taler, V., & Phillips, N. A.
(2008) Language performance in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment: A comparative review. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 30(5), 501–556. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tipper, S. P.
(2001) Does negative priming reflect inhibitory mechanisms? A review and integration of conflicting views. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. A, Human Experimental Psychology, 54(2), 321–343. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Traykov, L., Raoux, N., Latour, F., Gallo, L., Hanon, O., Baudic, S., … Rigaud, A.-S.
(2007) Executive functions deficit in mild cognitive impairment. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 20(4), 219–224. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Trueswell, J. C., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Garnsey, S. M.
(1994) Semantic influences on parsing: Use of thematic role information in syntactic ambiguity resolution. Journal of Memory and Language. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Unsworth, N., & Engle, R. W.
(2007) The nature of individual differences in working memory capacity: Active maintenance in primary memory and controlled search from secondary memory. Psychological Review, 114(1), 104–132. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Unsworth, N., Heitz, R. P., Schrock, J. C., & Engle, R. W.
(2005) An automated version of the operation span task. Behavior Research Methods, 37(3), 498–505. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Dyke, J. A.
(2007) Interference effects from grammatically unavailable constituents during sentence processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33(2), 407–430. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vuong, L. C., & Martin, R. C.
(2014) Domain-specific executive control and the revision of misinterpretations in sentence comprehension. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29(3), 312–325. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Waters, G., & Caplan, D.
(2002) Working memory and online syntactic processing in Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 57B(4), 298–311. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wechsler, D.
(1997) Wechsler Memory Scale (3rd ed.). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Winblad, B., Palmer, K., Kivipelto, M., Jelic, V., Fratiglioni, L., Wahlund, L.-O., … Petersen, R. C.
(2004) Mild cognitive impairment–beyond controversies, towards a consensus: Report of the International Working Group on Mild Cognitive Impairment. Journal of Internal Medicine, 256(3), 240–246. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yoon, J., Campanelli, L., Goral, M., Marton, K., Eichorn, N., & Obler, L. K.
(2015) The effect of plausibility on sentence comprehension among older adults and its relation to cognitive functions. Experimental Aging Research, 41(3), 272–302. DOI logoGoogle Scholar