Article published In:
Language, Interaction and Acquisition
Vol. 9:2 (2018) ► pp.226255
References
Allen, C., Nikolopoulos, T. P. & O’Donoghue, G. M.
(1998) Speech intelligibility in children after cochlear implantation. The American Journal of Otology, 191, 742–746.Google Scholar
Archbold, S. M., Nikolopoulos, T. P. & Tait, M.
(2000) Approach to communication, speech perception and intelligibility after paediatric cochlear implantation. British Journal of Audiology, 341, 257–264. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arfé, B., Rossi, C., & Sicoli, S.
(2015) The contribution of verbal working memory to deaf children’s oral and written production. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 20(3), 203–214. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. D.
(2000) The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 41, 417–423. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Benveniste, E.
(1966) Problèmes de linguistique générale (Tome 1). Paris: Gallimard.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M., Bright, P., James, C., Bishop, S. J., & van der Lely, H.
(2000) Grammatical SLI: A distinct subtype of developmental language impairment? Applied Psycholinguistics, 211, 159–181. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bourdin, B.
(2015) Un modèle capacitaire du traitement langagier chez l’enfant sourd : le cas de la morphosyntaxe. ANAE 138, Approche neuropsychologique des apprentissages chez l’enfant, Surdité : évolutions technologiques, de la prise en charge et des apprentissages, 469–476.Google Scholar
Bourdin, B., Ibernon, L., Le Driant, B., Levrez, C. & Vandromme, L.
(2016) Troubles morphosyntaxiques chez l’enfant sourd et chez l’enfant dysphasique : similarités et spécificités. Revue de Neuropsychologie, Neurosciences cognitives et cliniques, 8(3), 161–172. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brown, R.
(1973) A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chastain, C.
(1975) Reference and context. In K. Gunderson (ed.), Language, mind, and knowledge (194–269). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Clark, E.
(2001) Emergent categories. In M. Bowerman & S. C. Levinson (eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development (379–405). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Connor, C. M., Craig, H. K., Raudenbush, S. W., Heavner, K. & Zwolan, T. A.
(2006) The age at which young deaf children receive cochlear implants and their vocabulary and speech-production growth: Is there an added value for early implantation? Ear and Hearing, 271, 628–644. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Creissels, D.
(1993) Les notions primitives de la syntaxe. Réflexions sur l’articulation logique entre les notions sur lesquelles se fondent les descriptions grammaticales. In M. Maillard (ed.), Vers une rénovation de la grammaire et de sa terminologie (51–83). Lidil 81. Grenoble: Presses Universitaires de Grenoble.Google Scholar
(2006) Syntaxe générale, une introduction typologique (21 volumes). Paris-Cachan: Hermès Lavoisier.Google Scholar
Croft, W.
(1991) Syntactic categories and grammatical relations. The cognitive organization of information. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Culioli, A.
(1999) Pour une linguistique de l’énonciation. Formalisation et opérations de repérage (Tome 2). Paris et Gap: Ophrys.Google Scholar
Daza, M.-T., Phillips-Silver, J., Del Mar Ruiz-Cadra, M. & Lopez-Lopez, F.
(2014) Language skills and nonverbal cognitive processes associated with reading comprehension in deaf children. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 351, 3526–3533. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Duchesne, L., Sutton, A. & Bergeron, F.
(2009) Language achievement in children who received cochlear implants between 1 and 2 years of age: Group trends and individual patterns. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 14(4), 465–485. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Durand, J. & Tarrier, J.-M.
(2008) Deux thèses sur la transcription et le codage d’un grand corpus oral : le cas de PFC. In M. Bilger (ed.), Données orales. Les enjeux de la transcription (48–77). Cahiers de l’Université de Perpignan, 371. Perpignan: Presses Universitaires de Perpignan.Google Scholar
Edwards, J., Munson, B. & Beckman, M. E.
(2011) Lexicon-phonology relationships and dynamics of early language development. Journal of Child Language, 381, 35–40. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hallé, F. & Duchesne, L.
(2015) Habiletés morpho-syntaxiques des enfants sourds porteurs d’implants cochléaires : une revue systématique. Revue Canadienne d’Orthophonie et d’Audiologie, 39(3), 260–297.Google Scholar
Inscoe, J. R., Odell, A., Archbold, S. & Nikolopoulos, T.
(2009) Expressive spoken language development in deaf children with cochlear implants who are beginning formal education. Deafness and Education International, 111, 39–55. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jakubowicz, C. & Rigaut, C.
(2000) L’acquisition des clitiques nominatifs et des clitiques objets en français. Revue Canadienne de Linguistique, 45(1–2), 119–157. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Josse, D.
(1997) Brunet-Lézine révisé. Echelle de développement psychomoteur de la première enfance. Paris: Editions et Applications Psychologiques (EAP).Google Scholar
Jung, J. & Ertmer, D.
(2011) Early vocabulary size and grammatical abilities in young CI recipients and TD toddlers. Purdue University. Poster at the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association, San Diego, CA. Consulté le 10 mars 2017: [URL] .Google Scholar
Khomsi, A.
(2001) Evaluation du langage oral. Paris: les Editions du Centre de Psychologie Appliquée (ECPA).Google Scholar
Koo, D., Crain, K., Lasasoo, C. & Eden, G. F.
(2008) Phonological awareness and short-term memory in hearing and deaf individuals of different communication backgrounds. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 11451, 83–99. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Le Normand, M.-T. & Moreno-Torres, I.
(2014) The role of linguistic and environmental factors on grammatical development in French children with cochlear implants. Lingua, 1391, 26–38. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lecanuet, J. P.
(1995) L’expérience auditive prénatale. In I. Deliège & J. A. Sloboda (eds.), Naissance et développement du sens musical (7–38). Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.Google Scholar
Martinot, C.
(2005) Comment parlent les enfants de 6 ans ? Pour une linguistique de l’acquisition. Besançon: Presses Universitaires de Franche-Comté.Google Scholar
(2015) La reformulation : de la construction du sens à la construction des apprentissages en langue et sur la langue. In S. Volteau & J. Rançon (eds.), La reformulation : usages et contextes. Cognition, représentation, langage (Corela), hors-série n° 18.Google Scholar
Mehler, J., Lambertz, G., Jusczyk, P. & Amiel-Tison, C.
(1986) Discrimination of the mother tongue by newborn infants. Comptes Rendus de l’Academie des Sciences, 303(15), 637–640.Google Scholar
Morgenstern, A.
(2006) Un JE en construction. Ontogenèse de l’autodésignation chez l’enfant. Paris et Gap: Ophrys.Google Scholar
(2009) L’enfant dans la langue. Paris: Presses Sorbonne Nouvelle.Google Scholar
Morgenstern, A. & Parisse, C.
(2012) Constructing “basic” verbal constructions: a longitudinal study of the blossoming of constructions with six frequent verbs. In M. Bouveret & D. Le Gallois (eds.). Constructions in French (127–153). Constructional Approaches to Language Series, 131, Amsterdam / Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Norbury, C. F., Bishop, D. V. M. & Briscoe, J.
(2001) Production of English finite verb morphology: A comparison of SLI and mild-moderate hearing impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 44(1), 165–178. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nott, P., Cowan, R., Brown, P. M. & Wigglesworth, G.
(2009a) Early language development in children with profound hearing loss fitted with a device at a young age: Part I-the time period taken to acquire first words and first word combinations. Ear and Hearing, 30(5), 526–540. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009b) Early language development in children with profound hearing loss fitted with a device at a young age: Part II – content of the first lexicon. Ear and Hearing, 30(5), 541–551. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Parisse, C. & Le Normand, M.-T.
(2006) Une méthode pour évaluer la production du langage spontané chez l’enfant de 2 à 4 ans. Glossa, 971, 20–41.Google Scholar
Pisoni, D. B. & Geers, A.
(2000) Working memory in deaf children with cochlear implants: Correlations between digit span and measures of spoken language processing. Annals of Otology, Rhinology and Laryngology, 1091, 92–93. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rescorla, L., Alley, A. & Book, J. C.
(2001) Word frequencies in toddlers’ lexicons. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 441, 598–609. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schnedecker, C. & Landragin, F.
(2014) Les chaînes de référence : présentation. Langages, 1951, 3–22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Szagun, G.
(2000) The acquisition of grammatical and lexical structures in children with cochlear implants: a developmental psycholinguistic approach. Audiology and Neurotology, 5(1), 39–47. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Teberosky, A. & Vilaginès, E.
(2008) Le corpus d’enfants : quels outils ? In M. Bilger (ed.), Données orales. Les enjeux de la transcription (110–127). Cahiers de l’Université de Perpignan, 371. Perpignan: Presses Universitaires de Perpignan.Google Scholar
Tesnière, L.
(1959) Eléments de syntaxe structurale. Paris: Klincksieck.Google Scholar
Tuller, L. & Jakubowicz, C.
(2004) Développement de la morphosyntaxe du français chez des enfants sourds moyens. Le Langage et l’Homme : Logopédie, Psychologie, Audiologie, 141, 101–207.Google Scholar
Vincent-Durroux, L.
(2014) La langue orale des jeunes sourds profonds. Paris: De Boeck-Solal, collection Voix Parole Langage.Google Scholar