Abreu, A. M., Macaluso, E., Azevedo, R. T., Cesari, P., Urgesi, C., & Aglioti, S. M.
(2012) Action anticipation beyond the action observation network: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study in expert basketball players. Eur. J. Neurosci. 35, 1646–1654. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ahmad, A. H., & Abdul Aziz, C. B.
(2014) The Brain in Pain. The Malaysian Journal of Medical Sciences: MJMS, 21(Spec Issue), 46–54.Google Scholar
Allbritton, D. W., McKoon, G., & Gerrig, R.
(1995) Metaphor-based schemas and text representations: Making connections through conceptual Metaphors . Journal of Experimental Psychology, 21(3), 612–625.Google Scholar
Alsmith, A., & de Vignemont, F.
(2012) Embodying the mind and representing the body. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, special issue, 3(1), 1–13.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Alverson, H.
(1994) Semantics and Experience: Universal Metaphors of Time in English, Mandarin, Hindi, and Sesotho. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Ammaniti, M. & Gallese, V.
(2014) The Birth of Intersubjectivity. Psychodynamics, Neurobiology and the Self. New York and London: W. W. Norton & Company.Google Scholar
Andersen R. A., Snyder L. H., Bradley D. C., Xing J.
(1997) Multimodal representation of space in posterior parietal cortex and its use in planning movements. Ann. Rev. Neurosci 20:167–170.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Aziz-Zadeh, L., Maeda, F., Zaidel, E., Mazziotta, J., & Iacoboni, M.
(2002) Lateralization in motor facilitation during action observation: a TMS study. Exp Brain Res, 144(1), 127–131.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Babiloni, C., Babiloni, F., Carducci, F., Cincotti, F., Cocozza, G., Del Percio, C., Moretti, D. V., & Rossini, P. M.
(2002) Human cortical electroencephalography (EEG) rhythms during the observation of simple aimless movements: a high-resolution EEG study. Neuroimage, 17, 559–572.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bach, P., Peatfield, N., & Tipper, S. P.
(2007) Focusing on body sites: the role of spatial attention in action perception . Exp Brain Res, 178, 509–517.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bailey, D.
(1998) Getting a grip: A computational model of the acquisition of verb semantics for hand actions. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, International Computer Science Institute, University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Baker, P.
(2006) The Washington Post October 24, A01.Google Scholar
Baldissera, F., Cavallari, P., Craighero, L., & Fadiga, L.
(2001) Modulation of spinal excitability during observation of hand actions in humans. Eur J Neurosci, 13(1), 190–194.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barcelona, A.
(Ed.) (2000) Metaphor and Metonymy at the Crossroads. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. W.
(1999) Perceptual symbol systems. Behav Brain Sci, 22(4), 577–609; discussion 610–560.Google Scholar
(2008) Grounded cognition. Annu Rev Psychol, 59, 617–645.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bechtel, W. & Mundale, J.
(1999) Multiple Realizability Revisited: Linking Cognitive and Neural States. Philosophy of Science, 66, 175–207.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bergen, B.
(2005) Mental simulation in literal and figurative language understanding. In S. Coulson & B. Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk (Eds.) The literal and nonliteral in language and thought (pp.255–280). Berlin: Lang.Google Scholar
Bergen, B., Lindsay, S., Matlock, T. & Narayanan, S.
(2007) Spatial and linguistic aspects of visual imagery in sentence comprehension. Cognitive Science, 31, 733–764.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Berlucchi, G. & Aglioti, S.
(1997) The body in the brain: Neural bases of corporeal awareness. Trends in Neurosciences, 20, 560–564.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Blasko, D., & Briihl, D.
(1997) Reading and recall of metaphorical sentences: effects of fam-iliarity and context. Metaph.Symbol. 12, 261–285. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Blasko, D., & Connine, C.
(1993) Effects of familiarity and aptness on metaphor processing. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 19, 295–308. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Block, N.
(2005) Two neural correlates of consciousness . Trends Cogn Sci. Feb;9(2):46–52.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boers, F.
(2004) Expanding learners’ vocabulary through metaphor awareness: What expansion, what learners, what vocabulary?. In S. Niemeier, & M. Achard (Eds.) Cognitive linguistics, second language acquisition, and foreign language teaching (pp.211–234). Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boers, F., & Littlemore, J.
(2000) Cognitive style variables in participants’ explanations of conceptual Metaphors . Metaphor and Symbol, 15, 177–187.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boot, I. & D. Pecher
2010Similarity is closeness: Metaphorical mapping in a conceptual task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 63(5). 942–954.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bonnier, P.
(1905) L’Aschématie [Aschematia]. Revue Neurologique, 13, 605–609.Google Scholar
Borbely, A.
(2004) Toward a psychodynamic understanding of metaphor and metonymy: Their role in awareness and defense. Metaphor and Symbol, 19, 91–114.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Borghi, A.M., Barca, L., Binkofski, F. and Tummolini, L.
(2018) Theme issue ‘Varieties of abstract concepts: development, use and representation in the brain’, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, Vol. 373, No. 1752.Google Scholar
Borghi, A. M., Binkofski, F., Castelfranchi, C., Cimatti, F., Scorolli, C., Tummolini, L.
(2017) The challenge of abstract concepts . Psychol Bull. Mar;143(3):263–292.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boroditsky, L.
(2000) Metaphoric structuring: Understanding time through spatial Metaphors. Cognition, 75, 1–28.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2001) Does language shape thought? English and Mandarin speakers’ conceptions of time. Cognitive Psychology, 43, 1–22.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boroditsky, L. & Ramscar, M.
(2002) The roles of body and mind in abstract thought. Psychological Science, 13, 185–189.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Borroni, P., Montagna, M., Cerri, G., & Baldissera, F.
(2005) Cyclic time course of motor excitability modulation during the observation of a cyclic hand movement. Brain Res, 1065(1–2), 115–124.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bortfeld, H. & McGlone, M.
(2001) The continuum of metaphor processing. Metaphor and Symbol, 16, 75–86.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boulenger, V., Hauk, O., & Pulvermüller, F.
(2009) Grasping ideas with the motor system: semantic somatotopy in idiom comprehension. Cereb Cortex, 19(8), 1905–1914. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boulenger, V., Shtyrov, Y., & Pulvermüller, F.
(2012) When do you grasp the idea? MEG evidence for instantaneous idiom understanding. Neuroimage, 59(4), 3502–3513. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bowdle, B., & Gentner, D.
(2005) The career of metaphor. Psychol.Rev. 112, 193–216. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brandt, V. C., Moczydlowski, A., Jonas, M., Boelmans, K.,, Bäumer, T., Brass, M., Münchau, A.
(2017) Imitation inhibition in children with Tourette Syndrome. J Neuropsychol.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brass, M., Ruby, P., Spengler, S.
(2009) Inhibition of imitative behaviour and social cognition . Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci, 364, 2359–2367.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brentano, F.
(1874) Psychology from an Empirical Standpoint. Translated by A. C. Rancurello, D. B. Terrell & L. McAlister. London: Routledge, 1973.Google Scholar
Briñol, P., & Petty, R. E.
(2008) Embodied persuasion: Fundamental processes by which bodily responses can impact attitudes. In G. R. Semin & E. R. Smith (Eds.), Embodiment grounding: Social, cognitive, affective, and neuroscientific approaches (pp.184–207). Cambridge, England: CUP.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brooks, R.
(1991) Intelligence Without representation. Artificial Intelligence, 47, 139–159.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brookshire, G., Ivry, R., & Casasanto, D.
(2010) Modulation of motor-meaning congruity effects for valenced words. In S. Ohlsson & R. Catrambone (Eds.), Proceedings of the 32nd annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp.1940–1945). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Brown, T.
(2003) Making truth: Metaphor in science. Champaign, IL: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Brunyé, T. T., Gardony, A., Mahoney C. R. & Holly A. T.
(2012) Body-specific representations of spatial location. Cognition 123(2), 229–239.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Buccino, G., Vogt, S., Ritzl, A., Fink, G. R., Zilles, K., Freund, H. J., Rizzolatti, G.
(2004) Neural circuits underlying imitation learning of hand actions: an event-related fMRI study. Neuron. Apr 22;42(2):323–334Google Scholar
Cacciari, C., Bolognini, N., Senna, I., Pellicciari, M. C., Miniussi, C., & Papagno, C.
(2011) Literal, fictive and metaphorical motion sentences preserve the motion component of the verb: a TMS study. Brain Lang, 119(3), 149–157. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cacioppo, S., Juan, E., Monteleone, G.
(2017) Predicting Intentions of a Familiar Significant Other Beyond the Mirror Neuron System. Front Behav Neurosci. 25;11:155.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Calbris, G.
(1990) The semiotics of French gestures. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Cameron, L.
(2007) Patterns of metaphor in reconciliation talk. Discourse and Society, 18, 197–222.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carapezza, M., Cuccio V.
(2018) Abductive inferences in pragmatic processes, in Capone A., Carapezza M. (eds.), Perspectives in Pragmatics, Philosophy & Psychology, Vol. 18,: Further Advances in Pragmatics and Philosophy, pp.221–242, Springer, Cham. ISBN 978-3-319-72172-9.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carr, L., Iacoboni, M., Dubeau, M. C., Mazziotta, J. C., & Lenzi, G. L.
(2003) Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: a relay from neural systems for imitation to limbic areas. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, 100(9), 5497–5502. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Caruana, F., Cuccio, V.
(2017) Types of abduction in tool behaviour. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, Springer, Volume 16, Issue 2, pp 255–273.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Casasanto, D.
)2008) Similarity and proximity: When does close in space mean close in mind? Memory & Cognition 36(6). 1047–1056.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009) When is a linguistic metaphor a conceptual metaphor? In V. Evans, & S. Pourcel (Eds.), New directions in cognitive linguistics (pp.127–145). Amsterdam: Benjamins.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Casasanto, D. & Boroditsky, L.
(2008) Time in the mind: Using space to think about time. Cognition, 106, 579–593.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Casasanto, D. & K. Dijkstra
(2010) Motor action and emotional memory. Cognition 115(1). 179–185.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Casasanto, D. & R. Bottini
(2014) Spatial language and abstract concepts. WIREs Cognitive Science, 5. 139–149.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Citron, F. M. M. & A.E. Goldberg
(2014a) Social context modulates the effect of physical warmth on perceived interpersonal kindness: A study of embodied metaphors. Language and Cognition 6(1). 1–11.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Casasanto, D., & Gijssels, T.
(2015) What makes a metaphor an embodied metaphor?. Linguistics Vanguard. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chalmers , D.
(1995) Facing up to the problem of consciousness . Journal of Consciousness studies, 2(3), 200–219.Google Scholar
Chemero, A.
(2009) Radical embodied cognitive science . Cambridge, Massachusetts; London: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chen, J. -Y.
(2007) Do Chinese and English speakers think about time differently? Failure of replicating Boroditsky (2001). Cognition, 104, 427–436.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cochin, S., Barthelemy, C., Roux, S., & Martineau, J.
(1999) Observation and execution of movement: similarities demonstrated by quantified electroencephalography. Eur J Neurosci, 11, 1839–1842.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chao, L., Martin, A.
(2000) Representation of manipulable man-made objects in the dorsal stream. Neuroimage, 12, 478–484.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N.
(1965) Aspects of the theory of syntax. In Multilingual Matters. Cambridge: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Churchland, P.
(1986) Neurophilosophy: Toward a Unified Science of the Mind-Brain. Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cienki, A.
(1998) Metaphoric gestures and some of their relations to verbal metaphoric expressions. In J. -P. Koenig (Ed.) Discourse and cognition (pp.189–204). Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Cienki, A., & Mueller, C.
(Eds.) (2008) Metaphor and gesture. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, A.
(1997) Being there: putting brain, body, and world together again. Cambridge, Massachusetts; London: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
(2008) Supersizing the Mind: Embodiment, Action, and Cognitive Extension. Oxford: Oxford University Press.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016a) Embodied prediction. In T. Metzinger and J. M. Windt (eds.), Open MIND: 7(T), Cambridge, Massachussetts: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
(2016b) Predicting peace: Reply to Madary. In T. Metzinger and J. M. Windt (eds.), Open MIND: 7(T), , Cambridge, Massachussetts: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
(2016c) Surfing Uncertainty: Prediction, Action and the Embodied Mind. Oxford University PressDOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, A., & Grush, R.
(1999) Towards a cognitive robotics . Adaptive Behavior, 7(1), 5–16.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, S., Tremblay, F., & Ste-Marie, D.
(2004) Differential modulation of corticospinal excitability during observation, mental imagery and imitation of hand actions. Neuropsychologia, 42(1), 105–112.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cole, J.
(1995) Pride and a daily marathon. Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cole, J., & Paillard, J.
(1995) Living without touch and information about body position and movement. Studies on deafferented subjects. In J. Bermudez, A. Marcel, & N. Iylan (Eds.), The Body and the Self. (pp.24–266). Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Columbus, G., Sheikh, N. A., Côté-Lecaldare, M., Häuser, K., Baum, S. R., & Titone, D.
(2014) Individual differences in executive control relate to metaphor processing: an eye movement study of sentence reading. Frontiers in human neuroscience, 13(8), 1–12. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Costantini, M., Galati, G., Ferretti, A., Caulo, M., Tartaro, A., Romani, G. L., & Aglioti, S. M.
(2005) Neural systems underlying observation of humanly impossible movements: an FMRI study. Cereb. Cortex, 15, 1761–1767.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Coulter, R., Zaltman, G., & Coulter, K.
(2001) Interpreting consumer perceptions of advertising: An application of the Zaltman metaphor elicitation technique. Journal of Advertising, 30, 1–22.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crawford, Elizabeth L., Skye M. Margolies, John T. Drake & Meghan E. Murphy
2006Affect biases memory of location: Evidence for the spatial representation of affect. Cognition and Emotion 20(8). 1153–1169.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crawford, E.
(2009)  Conceptual metaphors of affect. Emotion Review, 1, 129–139.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Croft, W., & Cruse, A.
(2004) Cognitive linguistics. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cross, K. A., Torrisi, S., Reynolds Losin, E. A., & Iacoboni, M.
(2013) Controlling automatic imitative tendencies: interactions between mirror neuron and cognitive control systems. Neuroimage, 83, 493–504.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cuccio, V.
(2008) Dire lo spazio. Linguaggio spazialità e sindrome di Williams. Catania: Bonanno Editore.Google Scholar
(2015a) The notion of representation and the brain . Phenomenology and Mind, 7, 247–258.Google Scholar
(2015b)  Embodied Simulation and Metaphors. Towards a direct role of the body in language comprehension . Epistemologia, 37, 97–112.Google Scholar
Cuccio V.
(2017) Body-schema and body-image in metaphor processing in B. Hampe, (ed.), Metaphor. Embodied Cognition and Discourse, Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cuccio, V., Ambrosecchia, M., Ferri, F., Carapezza, M., Lo Piparo, F., Fogassi, L. & Gallese, V.
(2014) How the context matters. Literal and figurative meaning in the embodied language paradigm. PlosOne, 9(12), 1–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cuccio V., Gallese V.
(2018) Grounding abstraction in phylogeny: A comparative neuroscientific perspective, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. Vol. 373, 1752. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cuykens, H., & Zawada, B.
(Eds.) (2001) Polysemy in cognitive linguistics. Amsterdam, the Netherlands: John Benjamins.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Damasio, A.
(1994) Descartes’s error. motion, Reason, and the Human Brain. New York: Avon Books.Google Scholar
Decety, J. and Grezes, J.
(1999) Neural mechanisms subserving the perception of human action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 172–178.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Deignan, A.
(2006) Metaphor in corpus linguistics. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C.
(1969) Content and consciousness . London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
(1987) The intentional stance. Cambridge, Massachusetts; London: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Denny-Brown, D., Meyer, J. S., Horenstein, S.
(1952) The significance of perceptual rivalry resulting from parietal lesion. Brain, 75, 433–471.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Renzi, E., Cavalleri, F., & Facchini, S.
(1996) Imitation and utilisation behaviour. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry, 61(4), 396–400.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Desai, R. H., Binder, J. R., Conant, L. L., Mano, Q. R. & Seidenberg, M. S.
(2011) The neural career of sensory-motor Metaphors. J Cogn Neurosci, 23(9), 2376–2386. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Desai, R. H., Conant, L. L. Binder, J. R., Park, H., & Seidenberg, M. S.
(2013) A piece of the action: modulation of sensory-motor regions by action idioms and Metaphors. Neuroimage, 83, 862–869. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Vignemont, F.
(2010) The body-schema and the body-image. Pros and Cons. Neuropsychologya, 48(3), 669–680. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dreyfus, H. L
(2002), Intelligence without representation – Merleau-Ponty’s critique of mental representation. The relevance of phenomenology to scientific explanations, Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 1: 367–383, 2002.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dijkerman, H. C., & de Haan, E. H.
(2007) Somatosensory processes subserving perception and action. Behav Brain Sci, 30(2), 189–201; discussion 201–139. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dretske, F.
(1995) Naturalizing the Mind. Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Dudschig, C., de la Vega, I., & Kaup, B.
(2014) Embodiment and second-language: Automatic activation of motor responses during processing spatially associated L2 words and emotion L2 words in a vertical Stroop paradigm. Brain and Language, 132, 14–21.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eerden, B.
(2009) Anger in Asterix: The metaphorical representation of anger in comics and animated films. In C. Forceville & E. Urios-Aparisi (Eds), Multimodal Metaphor (pp.243–264). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Eubanks, P.
(2000) A war of words in the discourse of trade: The rhetorical constitution of metaphor. Carbondale, IL: Southern Illinois University Press.Google Scholar
Evans, V., & Green, M.
(2006) Cognitive linguistics: An introduction. Edinburgh, UK: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Pavesi, G., & Rizzolatti, G.
(1995) Motor facilitation during action observation: a magnetic stimulation study. J Neurophysiol, 73(6), 2608–2611.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fahlenbrach, K.
(2005) The emotional design of music videos: Approaches to audiovisual Metaphors. Journal of Moving Images Studies, 3(1), 22–28.Google Scholar
(2007) Embodied spaces: Film spaces as (leading) audiovisual Metaphors. In J. D. Anderson, & B. Fisher-Anderson (Eds.), Narration and Spectatorship in Moving Images (pp.105–123). Cambridge, MA, USA: Cambridge Scholars Press.Google Scholar
Fauconnier, G. & Turner, M.
(2002) The way we think: Conceptual blending and the mind’s hidden complexities. New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Ferrari, G.
(2006) Architectural space as metaphor in the Greek sanctuary. In M. Turner (Ed.), The artful mind (pp.225–242). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Feldman, J.
(2006) From molecule to Metaphor. A Neural Theory of Language . Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Feldman, J., & Narayanan, S.
(2004) Embodied Meaning in a Neural Theory of Language . Brain and Language, 89, 385–392.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ferraresi M.
(1983) Le abduzioni nel testo ,Versus, 34, 59–77.Google Scholar
Fillmore, C.
(1968) The case for case. In E. Bach & R. Harms (Eds.), Universals in linguistic theory (pp.1–90). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
(1982) Frame semantics. In Linguistic Society of Korea (Ed.), Linguistics in the morning calm (pp.111–138). Seoul: Hanshin.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A.
(1983) Modularity of mind: an essay on faculty psychology. Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. and Pylyshyn, Z.
(1988) Connectionism and Cognitive Architecture: A Critical Analysis, Cognition, 28: 3–71.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fisher, S.
(1972) Body-image. In D. Sills (Ed.), International Encyclopaedia of the Social Sciences. ii. New York: Collier-Macmillan; repr. in T. Polhemus (Ed.), The Body Reader: Social Aspects of the Human Body (pp.115–121). New York: Pantheon Books 1978.Google Scholar
Forceville, C.
(1998) Pictorial Metaphor in Advertising. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
(2002) The identification of target and source in pictorial Metaphors. Journal of Pragmatics, 34, 1–14.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2005) Visual representations of the idealized cognitive model of anger in the Asterix album. La Zizanie. Journal of Pragmatics, 37, 69–88.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2006) The source-path-goal schema in the autobiographical journey documentary. New Review of Film and Television Studies, 4(3), 241–261.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Foroni, F., & Semin, G. R.
(2009) Language that puts you in touch with your bodily feelings: the multimodal responsiveness of affective expressions. Psychol Sci, 20(8), 974–980.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fowler, C., Richardson, M., Marsh, K., & Shockley, K.
(2008) Language use, coordination, and the emergence of cooperative action. In A. Fuchs & V. Jirsa (Eds.), Coordination: Neural, behavioral, and social dynamics (pp.161–180). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Freedberg, D., and Gallese, V.
(2007) Motion, emotion and empathy in esthetic experience. Trends Cogn Sci.;11(5):197–203.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Freeman, D.
(1995) Catch(ing) the nearest way: Macbeth and cognitive metaphor. Journal of Pragmatics, 24, 689–708.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Friederici, A. D.
(2011) The brain basis of language processing: from structure to function. Physiological Reviews, 91(4), 1357–1392.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2002) Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(2), 78–84.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fusaroli, R. & Morgani, S.
(Eds.) (2013)  Conceptual Metaphor Theory: Thirty Years After, Thematic Issue. Cognitive Semiotics, 5(2), 1–13.Google Scholar
Gallagher, S.
(1986) Body image and body schema: A conceptual clarification. Journal of Mind and Behavior, 7, 541–554.Google Scholar
(2005) How the body shapes the mind . New York: Oxford University Press.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007) Simulation trouble. Social Neuroscience, 2(3–4), 353–365.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2008) Neural simulation and social cognition . In J. A. Pineda (Ed.), Mirror neuron systems: The role of mirroring processes in social cognition (pp.355–371). Totowa, NJ: Humana Press.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015) Reuse and body-formatted representations in simulation theory. Cognitive Systems Research, 34–35, 35– 43.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gallagher, S., & Cole, J.
(1995) Body image and body schema in a deafferented subject. J Mind and Behav, 16,369–389.Google Scholar
Gallagher, S. & Zahavi, D.
(2008) The Phenomenological Mind. 2nd edition, London; New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Gallese, V.
(2005)  Embodied simulation: From neurons to phenomenal experience. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, Vol. 4, 1, pp23–48.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2013) Mirror neurons, embodied simulation and a second-person approach to mindreading. Cortex, 49(10), 2954–2956.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gallese, V., & Cuccio V.
(2016) The paradigmatic body. embodied simulation, intersubjectivity, the bodily self and language. In T. Metzinger & J. Windt (Eds.), Open MIND, Cambridge, Massachussetts: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gallese, V. and Keysers, C.
(2001) Mirror neurons: A sensorimotor representation system. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, Vol. 24, 5, pp.983–984DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gallese, V., & Lakoff, G.
(2005) The brain’s concepts. The role of sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22(3), 455–479.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gallese, V.
(2008), Mirror neurons and the social nature of language: the neural exploitation hypothesis, Soc Neurosci. 3(3–4):317–333. doi: DOI logo.Google Scholar
Gallese, V., & Sinigaglia, C.
(2011) What is so special about embodied simulation?. Trends Cogn Sci, 15(11), 512–519.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gallistel, C. R., and King, A.
(2009) Memory and the Computational Brain, Malden: Wiley-Blackwell.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gangitano, M., Mottaghy, F. M., & Pascual-Leone, A.
(2001) Phase-specific modulation of cortical motor output during movement observation. Neuroreport, 12(7), 1489–1492.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gastaut, H., & Bert, J.
(1954) EEG changes during cinematographic presentation: moving picture activation of the EEG. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 6, 433–444.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gentner, D. & Grudin, J.
(1985) The evolution of mental Metaphors in psychology: A 90-year retrospective. American Psychologist, 40, 181–192.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gentner, D. Imai, & Boroditsky, L.
(2002) As time goes by: Understanding time as spatial metaphor. Language and Cognitive Processes, 17, 537–565.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gentner, D., and Brian, B.
2008Metaphor as structure-mapping. In Gibbs, R.W.. (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of metaphor and thought, pp.109–128.Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gentner, D., Imai M., & Boroditsky, L.
(2002) As time goes by: Understanding time as spatial metaphor. Language and Cognitive Processes, 17, 537–565.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gentner, D., & Kurtz, K.
(2006) Relations, objects, and the composition of analogies. Cognitive Science, 30, 609–642.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gernsbacher, M., & Faust, M.
(1991) The mechanism of suppressional component of general comprehension skill. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 17, 245–262. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gernsbacher, M., & Robertson, R.
(1995) Reading skill and suppression revisited. Psychol. Sci. 6, 165–169. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
J. Ghika, M. Tennis, J. Growdon, E. Hoffman, K. Johnson
(1995) Environment-driven responses in progressive supranuclear palsy, Journal of the neurological Sciences, Volume 130, Issue 1, Pages 104–111.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R. W.
(1991) Semantic analyzability in children’s understanding of idioms. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 613–620.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1992) What do idioms really mean? Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 485–506.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1994) The poetics of mind: Figurative thought, language, and understanding. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. W
(1999), Intentions and the experience of meaning. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. W.
(2003) Embodied meanings in performing, interpreting and talking about dance improvisation. In C. Albright and D. Gere (Eds.), Taken by surprise: A dance improvisation reader (pp.185–196). Middletown, CT: Wesleyan University Press.Google Scholar
(2005) The psychological status of image schemas. In B. Hampe (Ed.), From Perception to Meaning: Image-Schemas in cognitive linguistics (pp.113–130). Berlin: Mouton.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2006a) Metaphor interpretation as embodied simulation . Mind and Language, 21, 434–458.Google Scholar
(2006b) Embodiment and Cognitive Science. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
(2007) Why cognitive linguists should care more about empirical methods. In M. Gonzalez-Marquez, I. Mittelberg, S. Coulson, & M. J. Spivey (Eds.). Methods in Cognitive Linguistics (pp.2–118). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(Ed.) (2008) The Cambridge handbook of metaphor and thought. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011b) Evaluating Conceptual Metaphor Theory . Discourse Processes, 48(8), 529–562.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014) Embodied metaphor, pp.167–184. In J. Littlemore and J. R. Taylor (eds.). The Bloomsbury companion to cognitive linguistics, Bloomsbury, London: UK; Broadway: USA.Google Scholar
(2015a) Does deliberate metaphor theory have a future?. Journal of Pragmatics, 90, 73–76.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015b) Do pragmatic signals affect conventional metaphor understanding? A failed test of deliberate metaphor theory. Journal of Pragmatics, 90, 77–87.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017a) Metaphor Wars. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017b) The embodied and discourse view of metaphor: Why these are not so different and how they can be brought closer together (319–334). In In B. Hampe (Ed.), Metaphor: Embodied Cognition and Discourse (pp.27–45). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R. W. & Pelosi Silva de Macedo, A. C.
(2010) Metaphor and Embodied Cognition . D.E.L.T.A., 26, 679–700.Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J.
(1979) The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Boston: Houghton- Mifflin.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R., & O’Brien, J.
(1990) Idioms and mental imagery: The metaphorical motivation for idiomatic meaning. Cognition, 36, 35–68.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R., & Bogdonovich, J.
(1999) Mental imagery in interpreting poetic metaphor. Metaphor and Symbol, 14, 37–44.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R., & Franks, H.
(2002) Embodied metaphors in women’s’ narratives about their experiences with cancer. Health Communication, 14, 139–165.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R., Lima, P., & Francuzo, E.
(2004) Metaphor is grounded in embodied experience. Journal of Pragmatics, 36, 1189–1210.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R., Gould, J., & Andric, M.
(2006) Imagining metaphorical actions: Embodied simulations make the impossible plausible. Imagination, Cognition, & Personality, 25, 221–238.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R.W. Jr., and Cameron, L.
(2008) The social-cognitive dynamics of metaphor performance. Journal of Cognitive Systems Research, 9(1–2), 64–75.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R., Bogdonovich, J., Sykes, J., & Barr, D.
(1997) Metaphor in idiom comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 37, 141–154.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Giessner, S. R., & Schubert, T. W.
(2007) High in the hierarchy: How vertical location and judgments of leaders’ power are interrelated. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 104, 30–44.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Giora, R.
(2003) On Our Mind: Salience, Context, and Figurative Language. Oxford/New York: Oxford University Press.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2008) Is metaphor unique?. In R. W. Gibbs (Ed.), The Cambridge Handbook of Metaphor and Thought (pp.143–160). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Glenberg, A. M., & Kaschak, M. P.
(2002) Grounding language in action. Psychon Bull Rev, 9(3), 558–565.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Glenberg, A. M., Witt, J. K., & Metcalfe, J.
(2013) From the Revolution to Embodiment: 25 Years of Cognitive Psychology. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8, 573–585.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Glucksberg, S.
(2001) Understanding figurative language: From Metaphors to idioms. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2003).The psycholinguistics of Metaphors. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 92–96. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2008) How metaphors create categories – quickly. In Gibbs, R.W. (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of metaphor and thought, pp.67–83.Cambridge, MA:Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Goldman, A. and de Vignemont, F.
(2009), Is social cognition embodied?, Trends Cogn Sci 2009 Apr;13(4):154–159. doi: DOI logo. Epub 2009 Mar 9.Google Scholar
Glucksberg, S., Brown, M., & McGlone, M. S.
(1993)  conceptual Metaphors are not automatically accessed during idiom comprehension. Memory & Cognition, 21, 711–719.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goatly, A.
(1997) The language of Metaphors. New York, NY: Routledge.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007) Washing the brain: Metaphor and hidden ideology. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldman, A.
(2009) Mirroring, Simulating and Mindreading. Mind and Language, 24, 235–252.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012) A moderate approach to embodied cognitive science . Review of Philosophy and Psychology 3 (1): 71–88.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2013) The bodily formats approach to embodied cognition. In U. Kriegel (Ed.), Current controversies in philosophy of mind (pp.91–108). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Goldman, A. & de Vignemont, F.
(2009) Is social cognition embodied?, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13 (4) :154–159DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldstein, A., Arzouan, Y., & Faust, M.
(2012) Killing a novel metaphor and reviving a dead one: ERP correlates of metaphor conventionalization. Brain Lang., 123, 137–142. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldwasser, O.
(2005) Where is metaphor? Conceptual metaphor and alternative classification in the hieroglyphic script. Metaphor and Symbol, 20, 95–113.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gong, S. -P., & Ahrens, K.
(2007) Processing conceptual Metaphors in ongoing discourse. Metaphor and Symbol, 22, 313–330.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grady, J.
(1997a) Theories are building revisited. Cognitive Linguistics, 8(4), 267–290.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1997b) Foundations of Meaning: Primary Metaphors and Primary Scenes. Ph.D. dissertation, University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
(1999) A typology of motivation for conceptual metaphor: correlation vs. resemblance. In G. Steen & R. Gibbs (Eds.), Metaphor in Cognitive Linguistics (pp.79–100). Philadelphia: John Benjamins.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2005) Primary Metaphors as inputs to conceptual integration. Journal of Pragmatics, 37, 1595–1614.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grady, J., & Ascoli, G.
(2017) Sources and Targets in Primary Metaphor Theory: Looking Back and Thinking Ahead. In B. Hampe (Ed.), Metaphor: Embodied Cognition and Discourse (pp.27–45). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grady, J., & Johnson, C.
(1997) Converging Evidence for the Notions of subscene and primary scene (123–136). Proceedings of the Twenty-Third Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Graziano, M.
(2013)  Consciousness and the social brain . Oxford: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
Grezes, J., Armony, J. L., Rowe, J., & Passingham, R. E.
(2003) Activations related to “mirror” and “canonical” neurons in the human brain: an fMRI study. Neuroimage, 18, 928–937.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grezes, J., & Decety, J.
(2002) Does visual perception of object afford action? Evidence from a neuroimaging study. Neuropsychologia, 40, 212–222.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haggard, P., & Wolpert, D. M.
(2005) Disorders of Body Scheme. In H. J. Freund, M. Jeannerod, M. Hallett & R. Leiguarda (Eds.), Higher-Order Motor Disorders (pp.261–271). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hagoort, P., Hald, L. A., Bastiaansen, M. C. M., & Petersson, K. M.
(2004) Integration of word meaning and world knowledge in language comprehension . Science, 304, 438–441.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hampe, B.
(2005) From Peception to Meaning: Image-Schemas in Cognitive Linguistics. Berlin: Mouton.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017) Metaphor: Embodied Cognition and Discourse. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hahne A., & Friederici A. D.
(2002) Differential task effects on semantic and syntactic processes as revealed by ERPs. Cogn Brain Res, 13, 339–356.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1999) Electrophysiological evidence for two steps in syntactic analysis: early automatic and late controlled processes. J Cogn Neurosci, 11, 194–205.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hauser, M., Chomsky, N., & Fitch, W. T.
(2002) The language faculty: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science, 298, 1569–1579.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hari, R., Forss, N., Avikainen, S., Kirveskari, E., Salenius, S., & Rizzolatti, G.
(1998) Activation of human primary cortex during action observation: a neuromagnetic study. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, 95, 15061–15065.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haser, V.
(2005) Metaphor, metonymy, and experientialist philosophy: Challenging cognitive semantics. Berlin: Mouton.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haugeland, J.
(1991) Representational genera. In W. Ramsey, S. Stich & D. Rumelhart (Eds.), Philosophy and Connectionist Theory (pp.61–90). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hebb, D. O.
(1949) The organization of behaviour; a neuropsychological theory. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Held, R. & Hein A.
(1963) Movement-produced stimulation in the development of visually guided behavior. Jouranal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 56(5), 872–876.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Holst von, E., Mittelstaedt, H.
(1950) The reafference principle: interaction between the central nervous system and the periphery. Die Naturwissenschaften, 37, 464–476.Google Scholar
Hohwy, J.
(2014) The Predictive Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hummel, F., Andres, F., Alenmuller, E., Dichgans, J., & Gerloff, C.
(2002) Inhibitory control of acquired motor programmes in the human brain . Brain, 125, 404–420.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Humphreys, G. W., Riddoch, M. J.
(2001) Detection by action: neuropsychological evidence for action-defined templates in search. Nat Neurosci, 4, 84–88.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hutto, D. D., Myin, E.
(2013) Radicalizing enactivism. Basic minds without content. Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
(2017) Evolving Enactivism Basic Minds Meet Content. Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hutto, D. D., & Satne, G.
(2015) The Natural Origins of Content. Philosophia, 43(3).DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Iacoboni, M.
(2003) Understanding others: imitation, language, empathy. In S. Hurley, & N. Chater (Eds.), Perspectives on imitation: from cognitive neuroscience to social science (pp.77–100). Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Ibbotson, P., Lieven, E. V., & Tomasello, M.
(2013) The attention-grammar interface: eye-gaze cues structural choice in children and adults. Cognitive Linguistics, 24(3), 457–481.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ijzerman, H. & G.R. Semin
(2009) The thermometer of social relations mapping social proximity on temperature. Psychological Science 20(10). 1214–1220.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jacob, P.
(2008) What do Mirror Neurons Contribute to Human Social Cognition?. Mind and Language, 23, 190–223.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R.
(1983) Semantics and cognition. Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jakel, O.
(1999) Kant, Blumenberg, Weinrich. Some forgotten contributions to the cognitive theory of metaphor. In R. Gibbs, & G. Steen Metaphor in cognitive linguistics (pp.9–56). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Johnson, M.
(1987) The body in the mind: the bodily basis of meaning, imagination, and reason. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
(1993) Moral imagination: Implications of cognitive science for ethics. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, C
1999Metaphor vs. conflation in the acquisition of polysemy: The case of SEE. In M. K. Hiraga, C. Sinha, and S. Wilcox, (Eds. ), Cultural, typological and psychological issues in cognitive linguistics. Current Issues in Linguistic Theory, vol. 152, pp. 155–169. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Johnson, M.
(2005) The Philosophical Significance of Image Schemas. In B. Hampe (Ed.), From Perception to Meaning: Image Schemas in Cognitive Linguistics (pp.15–33). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Johnson, M. & Larsen, S.
(2003) Something in the way she moves. Metaphor and Symbol, 18, 63–84.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Johnson, M.
(2017) Embodied Mind, Meaning and reasoning. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jonides, J.
(1981) Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind’s eye’s movement. Attention and performance, 9(9), 187–203.Google Scholar
Jostmann, N. B., Lakens, D. & Schubert, T. W.
(2009) Weight as an embodiment of importance. Psychological Science, 20(9), 1169–1174.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Katz, A., & Ferratti, T.
(2001) Moment-by-moment comprehension of proverbs in discourse. Metaphor and Symbol, 11, 17–37.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Katz, A., & Taylor, T.
(2008) The journeys of life: Examining a conceptual metaphor with semantic and episodic memory recall. Metaphor and Symbol, 23, 148–173.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kelso, J. A. S.
(1995) Dynamic patterns: the self-organization of brain and behavior. Cambridge, Massachusetts; London: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kemmerer, D., Castillo J. G., Talavage, T., Patterson, S., & Wiley, C.
(2008) Neuroanatomical distribution of five semantic components of verbs: evidence from fMRI. Brain Lang, 107(1), 16–43. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kertész, A., C. Rákosi
(2009), Cyclic vs. circular argumentation in the Conceptual Metaphor Theory. Cognitive Linguistics 20: 703–732.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kessler, K., Biermann-Ruben, K., Jonas, M., Siebner, H. R., Baeumer, T., Muenchau, A., & Schnitzler, A.
(2006) Investigating the human mirror neuron system by means of cortical synchronization during the imitation of biological movements. Neuroimage, 33, 227–238.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Keysar, B., & Bly, B.
(1995) Intuitions of the transparency of idioms: Can one keep a secret by spilling the beans?. Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 89–109.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Keysar, B., Shen, Y., Glucksberg, S., & Horton, W.
(2000) Conventional language: How metaphoric is it?. Journal of Memory and Language, 43, 576–593.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kilner, J. M., Marchant, J. L., & Frith, C. D.
(2006) Modulation of the mirror system by social relevance. Scan, 1, 143–148.Google Scholar
Kintsch, W.
(1998) Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
(2000) Metaphor comprehension: a computational theory. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 7, 257–266. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2001) Predication. Cogn. Sci. 25, 173–202. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kintsch, W., & Bowles, A.
(2002) Metaphor comprehension: what makes a metaphor difficult to understand?. Metaphor & Symbol. 17, 249–262. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kohler, W.
(1969) The task of gestalt psychology. NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Kilner, J. M., & Friston, K. J., Frith, C. D.
(2007) Predictive coding: an account of the mirror neuron system. Cogn Process, 8, 159–166. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Koller, V.
(2004) Metaphor and gender in business media discourse: A critical cognitive study. Basingstoke, UK: Palgrave.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kövecses, Z.
(2000) Metaphor and emotion. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
(2002) Metaphor: A practical introduction. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
(2003) Language, figurative thought, and cross-cultural comparison. Metaphor and Symbol, 18, 311–320.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2005a) Metaphor in Culture. Universality and Variation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2005b) Metaphor and Culture. Acta Universitatis Sapientiae, Philologica, 2(2), 197–220.Google Scholar
(2006) Language, mind, and culture: A practical introduction. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
(2008) Conceptual metaphor theory: Some criticisms and alternative proposals. Annual Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 6, 168–184.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009) Metaphor, culture, and discourse: The pressure of coherence. In J. Zinken, & A. Musolff (Eds.), Metaphor in discourse (pp.11–24). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2010) Metaphor: A practical introduction. Second edition. New York, NY: Oxford University PresGoogle Scholar
(2015) Where Metaphors Come From. Reconsidering Context in Metaphor. Oxford: Oxford University Press.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kriegel, U.
(2013) Two Notions of Mental Representation. In U. Kriegel (Ed.), Current Controversies in Philosophy of Mind (pp.161–179). New York and London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D.
(2011) Thirty years and counting: Finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 621–647.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lai, V., & Curran, T.
(2013) ERP evidence for conceptual mappings and comparison processes during the comprehension of conventional and novel Metaphors. Brain & Language, 127(2), 484–496.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G.
(1987) Women, fire, and dangerous things: what categories reveal about the mind . Chicago: University of Chicago Press.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1990) The invariance hypothesis: Is abstract reason based on image-schemas?. Cognitive Linguistics, 1, 39–74.Google Scholar
(1993) The contemporary theory of metaphor. In A. Ortony (Ed.), Metaphor and thought: Vol. 2 (pp.202–251). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1995) Steps Toward a Neurocognitive Self: Conceptual System Research in the 21st Century And Its Role in Rethinking What a Person Is. In Dom Massaro and Robert Solso (eds.), The Science of the Mind; 2001 and Beyond. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1995.Google Scholar
(1996) Moral politics. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
(2002) Moral Politics: How Liberals and Conservatives Think. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2008) The Neural Theory of Metaphor. In R. Gibbs (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of metaphor and thought (pp.17–38). New York: Cambridge University Press.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014) Mapping the Brain’s metaphor circuitry: Is abstract thought metaphorical thought?. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 16(8), 958. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M.
(1980) Metaphors We Live By. Chicago and London: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
(1999) Philosophy in the Flesh: The Embodied Mind and Its Challenge to Western Thought. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Núñez, R.
(2000) Where mathematics comes from: How the embodied mind brings mathematics into being. New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Turner, M.
(1989) More than cool reason: A field guide to poetic metaphor. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Langacker, R. W.
(1987) Foundations of cognitive grammar, vol. 1: Theoretical prerequisites. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
1991Foundations of cognitive grammar, vol. 2: Descriptive application. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Langston, W.
(2002) Violating orientational Metaphors slows reading. Discourse Processes, 34, 281–310.Google Scholar
Larson, B., Nerlich, B., & Wallis, P.
(2006) Metaphor and biorisks. Science Communication, 26, 243–268.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Landau, Mark J., Brian P. Meier & Lucas A. Keefer
(2010) A metaphor-enriched social cognition. Psychological Bulletin 13(6). 1045–1067.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lebois, L. A. M., Wilson-Mendenhall, C. D., & Barsalou, L. W.
(2010) Are Automatic Conceptual Cores the Gold Standard of Semantic Processing? The Context-Dependence of Spatial Meaning in Grounded Congruency Effects. Cognitive Science, 39(8), 1764–1801.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leezenberg, M
(2009), From Cognitive Linguistics to Social Science: Thirty Years after Metaphors We Live By,Cognitive Semiotics, Volume 5, Issue 1–2, Pages 140–152, ISSN (Online) 2235–2066.Google Scholar
Lee, S. W. S., & Schwarz, N.
(2012) Bidirectionality, mediation, and moderation of metaphorical effects: The embodiment of social suspicion and fishy smells. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103(5), 737–749.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leenzeberg, M.
(2009) From Cognitive Linguistics to Social Science: Thirty Years after Metaphors We Live By. Cognitive Semiotics, 5(1–2), 140–152.Google Scholar
Levine, J.
(1983) Materialism and Qualia: The Explanatory Gap. Pacific Philosophical Quarterly, 64(4), 354–361.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lhermitte, F.
(1986) Human autonomy and the frontal lobes. Part II: Patient behavior in complex and social situations: the ‘environmental dependency syndrome’. Ann Neurol, 19(4), 335–343.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lopez, C.
(2016) The vestibular system: balancing more than just the body. Curr Opin Neurol., 29(1), 74–83. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Maalej, Z.
(2004) Figurative language in anger expressions in Tunisian Arabic: An extended view of embodiment. Metaphor and Symbol, 19(1), 51–75.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Macnamara, D. S., & Magliano, J.
(2009) Toward a comprehensive model of comprehension. In B. Ross (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, Vol. 51 (pp.297–384). Burlington, VT, USA: Academic Press.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Maeda, F., Kleiner-Fisman, G., & Pascual-Leone, A.
(2002) Motor facilitation while observing hand actions: specificity of the effect and role of observer’s orientation. J Neurophysiol, 87(3), 1329–1335.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Maidhof, C., & Koelsch, S.
(2011) Effects of selective attention on syntax processing in music and language. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(9), 2252–2267.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Martin, J.
(1990) A computational model of metaphor interpretation. San Diego, CA: Academic.Google Scholar
Mashal, N.
(2013) The role of working memory in the comprehension of unfamiliar and familiar Metaphors . Lang. Cogn. 5, 409–436. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mashal, N., & Faust, M.
(2009) Conventionalization of novel Metaphors: a shift in hemispheric asymmetry. Laterality 14, 573–589. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mason, Z.
(2004) CorMet: A computational, corpus-based conventional metaphor extraction system. Computational Linguistics, 30, 23–44.Google Scholar
Matlock, T., Ramscar, M., & Boroditsky, L.
(2005) The experiential link between spatial and temporal language. Cognitive Science, 29, 655–664.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McGlone, M.
(1996)  conceptual Metaphors and figurative language interpretation: Food for thought? Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 544–565.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007) What is the explanatory value of a conceptual metaphor? Language & Communication, 27, 109–126.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McGlone, M., & Harding, J.
(1998) Back (or forward?) to the future: The role of perspective in temporal language comprehension . Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 24, 1211–1223.Google Scholar
McNeill, D.
(1992) Hand in mind . Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Meier, B. P., & Robinson, M. D.
(2004) Why the sunny side is up. Psychological Science, 15, 243–247.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meier, B. P., Robinson, M. D., & Clore, G. L.
(2004) Why good guys wear white: Automatic inferences about stimulus valence based on brightness. Psychological Science, 15, 82–87.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meier, B. P., Robinson, M. D., Crawford, L. E., & Ahlvers, W. J.
(2007) When “light” and “dark” thoughts become light and dark responses: Affect biases brightness judgments. Emotion, 7, 366–376.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K.
(1977) Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198(4312), 75–78. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1983) Newborn infants imitate adult facial gestures. Child Dev, 54(3), 702–709.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1989) Imitation in Newborn Infants: Exploring the Range of Gestures Imitated and the Underlying Mechanisms. Dev Psychol, 25(6), 954–962. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Merleau-Ponty, M.
(1945) Phénoménologie de la perception . Paris: Gallimard.Google Scholar
Meteyard, L., Cuadrado, S. R., Bahrami, B., & Vigliocco, G.
(2012) Coming of age: a re-view of embodiment and the neuroscience of semantics. Cortex, 48(7), 788–804. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miall, R. C.
(2003), Connecting mirror neurons and forward models, Neuroreport, Vol 14 No 16, pp. 1–3.Google Scholar
Miall, R. C., & Wolpert, D. M.
(1996) Forward models for physiological motor control. Neural Networks, 9, 1265–1279.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miles, L. K., Nind, L. K. & Macrae, C. N.
(2010) Moving through time. Psychological Science 21, 222–223.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Millikan, R. G.
(1984) Language, thought and other biological catagories: new foundations for realism. Cambridge, Massachusetts; London: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Mishra, R. K.
(2015a) Interaction Between Attention and Language System in Humans. New Delhi: Springer.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015b) Listening through the native tongue: A review essay on Cutler’s Native listening: Language experience and the recognition of poken words. Philosophical Psychology, 28(7), 1–15.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miyake, A., Just, M., & Carpenter, P.
(1994) Working-memory constraints on the resolution of lexical ambiguity: maintaining multiple interpretations in neutral contexts. J. Mem. Lang. 33, 175–202. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montijn, J. S., Vinck, M., & Pennartz, C. M.
(2014) Population coding in mouse visual cortex: response reliability and dissociability of stimulus tuning and noise correlation. Front Comput Neurosci, 8, 58. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Murphy, G.
(1996) On metaphoric representations. Cognition, 60, 173–204.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Musolff, A.
(2004) Metaphor and political discourse: Analogical reasoning in debates about Europe. London, England: Palgrave-Macmillan.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Muthukumaraswamy, S. D., Johnson, B. W.
(2004) Changes in rolandic mu rhythm during observation of a precision grip. Psychophysiology, 41, 152–156.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Muthukumaraswamy, S. D., Johnson, B. W., McNair, N. A.
(2004) Mu rhythm modulation during observation of an object-directed grasp. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res, 19, 195–201.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nagel, E.
(1949) The meaning of reduction in the natural sciences. In R. C. Stauffer (Ed.), Science and civilization (pp.99–138). Madison: University of Wisconsin Press.Google Scholar
Narayanan, S.
(1997) Moving right along: A computational model of metaphoric reasoning about events. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, International Computer Science Institute, University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Nayak, N., & Gibbs, R.
(1990) Conceptual knowledge in the interpretation of idioms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 119, 315–330.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Neisser, U.
(1987) Concepts and conceptual development: Ecological and intellectual factors in categorization. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Neuper, C., Scherer, R., Wriesnegger, S., & Pfurtscheller, G.
(2009) Motor imagery and action observation: modulation of sensorimotor brain rhythms during mental control of a brain-computer interface. Clin Neurophysiol, 120, 239–247.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Niedenthal, P. M., Barsalou, L. W., Winkielman, P., Krauth-Gruber, S., & Ric, F.
(2005) Embodiment in attitudes, social perception, emotion, Pers Soc Psychol Rev, 9(3), 184–211. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nishitani, N., Hari, R.
(2000) Temporal dynamics of cortical representation for action. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, 97, 913–918.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Niziolek, C. A., Nagarajan, S. S., & Houde, J. F.
(2013) What does motor efference copy represent? Evidence from speech production. J Neurosci, 33(41), 16110–16116.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Noë, A.
(2004) Action in perception . Cambridge, Massachusetts; London: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Núñez, R., Motz, B., & Teuscher, U.
(2006) Time after time: The psychological reality of the ego- and time–reference–point distinction in metaphorical contruals of time. Metaphor and Symbol, 21, 133–146.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Núñez, R., & Sweetser, E.
(2006) With the future behind them: Convergent evidence from Aymara language and gesture in the crosslinguistic comparison of spatial construals of time. Cognitive Science, 30, 401–450.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Oberman, L. M., Ramachandran, V. S., Pineda, J. A.
(2008) Modulation of mu suppression in children with autism spectrum disorders in response to familiar or unfamiliar stimuli: the mirror neuron hypothesis. Neuropsychologia, 46, 1558–1565.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Orton, S.
(2004) Conceptual metaphor in the archaeological record: Methods and an example from the American Southwest. American Antiquity, 65, 613–645.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ortony, A.
(1979) Metaphor and thought. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ozcaliskan, S.
(2003) In a caravanserai with two doors I am walking day and night: Metaphors of death and life in Turkish. Cognitive Linguistics, 14, 281–320.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paillard, J.
(1999)  Body schema and body image – a double dissociation in deafferented patients. In G. N. Gantchev, S. Mori, & J. Massion (Eds.), Motor control: today and tomorrow (pp.197–214). Academic Publishing House: Sofia, Bulgaria.Google Scholar
(2005) Vectorial versus configural encoding of body space. A neural basis for a distinction between Body schema and Body Image. In H. De Preester & V. Knockaert (Eds.), Body Image and Body schema: Interdisciplinary perspectives on the body (pp.89–109). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Papeo, L., Rumiati, R. I., Cecchetto, C., & Tomasino, B.
(2012) On-line changing of thinking about words: the effect of cognitive context on neural responses to verb reading. J Cogn Neurosci, 24, 2348–2362.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Papeo, L., Vallesi, A., Isaja, A., & Rumiati, R. I.
(2009) Effects of TMS on different stages of motor and non-motor verb processing in the primary motor cortex. PLoS One, 4(2): e4508. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Papineau, D.
(1993) Philosophical naturalism. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Pauen, M.
(2012) The second person perspective. Inquiry, 55(1), 33–49.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pelosi, A. C.
(2007) A psycholinguistic analysis of the metaphor “difficulties are weights”. Linguagem en (Dis)curso – LemD, 7(3), 389–404.Google Scholar
Pexman, P., Ferratti, T., & Katz, A.
(2000) Discourse factors that influence the online reading of metaphor and irony. Discourse Processes, 29, 201–222.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pfaff, K., Gibbs, R., & Johnson, M.
(1997) Metaphor in using and understanding euphemism and dysphemism. Applied Psycholinguistics, 18, 59–83.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pfurtscheller, G., Brunner, C., Schlogl, A., da Silva F. H. L.
(2006) Mu rhythm desynchronisation and EEG single-trial classification of different motor imagery tasks. Neuroimage, 31, 153–159.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Phillips, B., & McQuarrie, E.
(2007) Road map or secret weapon: The role of conceptual metaphor in shaping marketing communications about exercise. In T. Lowrey (Ed.), Psycholinguistic phenomena in marketing communication (pp.135–154). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
Piaget, J.
(1971) Biology and Knowledge. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Pineda, J. A., & Hecht, E.
(2009) Mirroring and mu rhythm involvement in social cognition: are there dissociable subcomponents of theory of mind?. Biol Psychol, 80, 306–314.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S.
(2007) The stuff of thought. New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Port, R. F., & Van Gelder, T.
(1995) Mind as motion: explorations in the dynamics of cognition. Cambridge, Massachusetts; London: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Pellegrino di, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G.
(1992) Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study. Exp Brain Res, 91(1), 176–180.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pragglejaz Group
(2007) MIP: A method for identifying metaphorically-used words in discourse. Metaphor and Symbol, 22, 1–40.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pulvermüller, F.
(1999) Words in the brain’s language. Behav Brain Sci, 22(2), 253–279.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012) Meaning and the brain: The neurosemantics of referential, in-teractive, and combinatorial knowledge. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 25(5), 423–459. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pulvermüller, F., Shtyrov, Y., Hasting, A. S., & Carlyon, R. P.
(2008) Syntax as a reflex: Neurophysiological evidence for early automaticity of grammatical processing. Brain and Language, 104(3), 244–253.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Purves, D., Augustine, G. J., Fitzpatrick, D., Hall, W. C., La Mantia, A. S., McNamara, J. O., & Williams, S. M.
(Eds) (2004) Neuroscience. Third edition. Sunderland (MA): Sinauer Associates.Google Scholar
Putnam, H.
(1960) Minds and Machines.Journal of Symbolic Logic, 57–80.Google Scholar
Pynn, L. K., & DeSouza, J. F.
(2013) The function of efference copy signals: implications for symptoms of schizophrenia. Vision Res, 76, 124–133. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Radden, G., & Panther, K. U.
(Eds.) (1999) Metonymy in thought and language. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Refaie, E. E.
(2003) Understanding visual metaphor: The example of newspaper cartoons. Visual Communication, 2(1), 75–95.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rakova, M.
(2002) The philosophy of embodied realism: A high price to pay?. Cognitive Linguistics, 13(3), 215–244.Google Scholar
Ramsey, W.
(2007) Representation Reconsidered. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Raposo A., Moss H. E., Stamatakis E. A., Tyler L. K.
(2009) Modulation of motor and premotor cortices by actions, action words and action sentences. Neuropsychologia 47: 388–396.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Read, S., Cesa, I., Jones, D., & Collins, N.
(1990) When is the federal budget like a baby? Metaphor in political rhetoric. Metaphor and Symbol, 5, 125–149.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Recanati, F.
(2004) Literal meaning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Reddy, M. J.
(1979) The conduit metaphor: A case of frame conflict in our language about language. In A. Ortony (Ed.), Metaphor and Thought (pp.284–310). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Regier, T. P.
(1992) The acquisition of lexical semantics for spatial terms: a connectionist model of perceptual categorization. PhD dissertationGoogle Scholar
Regier, T.
(1996) The human semantic potential. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Reijnierse, W. G., Burgers, C., Krennmayr, T., and Steen, G. J.
Richardson, D., & Matlock, T.
(2007) The integration of figurative language and static depictions: An eye-movement study of fictive motion. Cognition, 102, 129–138.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Riskind, J.H.
1983Nonverbal expressions and the accessibility of life experience memories: A congruence hypothesis. Social Cognition 2(1). 62–86.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ritchie, D.
(2008) X IS A JOURNEY: embodied simulation in metaphor interpretation. Metaphor and Symbol, 23(3), 174–199.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rizzolatti, G., Fabbri-Destro, M., & Cattaneo, L.
(2009) Mirror neurons and their clinical relevance. Nat Clin Pract Neurol, 5(1), 24–34. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rizzolatti, G., Sinigaglia, C., & Anderson, F.
(2008) Mirrors in the brain: how our minds share actions and emotions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Robins, S., & Mayer, R.
(2000) The metaphor framing effect: Metaphorical reasoning about text-based dilemmas. Discourse Processes, 30, 57–86.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rothenberg, A.
(2008) Rembrandt’s creation of the pictorial metaphor of self. Metaphor and Symbol, 23(2), 108–129.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ruiz de Mendoza Ibáñez, F. J., & Santibáñez Sáenz, F.
(2003) Content and formal cognitive operations in construing meaning. Italian Journal of Linguistics, 15, 293–320.Google Scholar
Ruiz de Mendoza Ibáñez, F. J., & Mairal, U.
(2007) High-level metaphor and metonymy in meaning construction. In G. Radden, K. Kopolke, M. Berg, & P. Siemund (Eds.), Aspects of meaning construction (pp.33–51). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rumelhart, D
(1980) Schemata: The building blocks of cognition. In R. Spiro, B. Bruce, & W. Brewer (Eds.), Theoretical issues in reading comprehension (pp. 33–58). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ruiz de Mendoza Ibáñez, F. J. & Santibáñez Sáenz, F.
(2003) Content and formal cognitive operations in construing meaning. Italian Journal of Linguistics, 15, 293–320.Google Scholar
Salverda, A. P., & Altmann, G.
(2011) Attentional capture of objects referred to by spoken language. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37(4), 1122.Google Scholar
Sanford, D.
(2008) Metaphor and phonological reduction in English idiomatic expressions. Cognitive Linguistics, 19, 585–604.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Santana, E., & De Vega, M.
(2011) Metaphors are embodied, and so are their literal counterparts. Frontiers in Psychology, 2(90), 1–12.Google Scholar
Santiago, J., Lupiáñez, J., Pérez, E., & Funes, M. J.
(2007) Time (also) flies from left to right. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 512–516.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Savitt, S.
(1974) Rorty’s Disappearance Theory. Philosophical Studies, 28, 433–436.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sbriscia-Fioretti, B., Berchio, C., Freedberg, D., Gallese, V., Umiltà, M. A.
(2013) ERP modulation during observation of abstract paintings by Franz Kline. PLoS One, 9;8(10):e75241. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schnall, S., Benton, J., & Harvey, S.
(2008) With a clean conscience: Cleanliness reduces the severity of moral judgments. Psychological Science, 19, 1219–1222.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schubert, T. W.
(2005) Your highness: Vertical positions as perceptual symbols of power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 1–21.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schuch, S. E., Bayliss, A. P., Klein, C., & Tipper, S. P.
(2010) Attention modulates motor system activation during action observation: Evidence for inhibitory rebound. Experimental Brain Research, 205(2), 235–249.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schendan, H. E. and Ganis, G.
(2012) Electrophysiological Potentials Reveal Cortical Mechanisms for Mental Imagery, Mental Simulation, and Grounded (Embodied) Cognition. Frontiers in Psychology, Vol. 3, article 329.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Searle, J. R.
(1980) Minds, Brains and programs. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3, 417–457.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Searle, J. R., Dennett, D. C., & Chalmers, D. J.
(1997) The mystery of consciousness . New York: New York Review of Books.Google Scholar
Semino, E.
(2010) Descriptions of pain, metaphor and embodied simulation . Metaphor and Symbol, 25(4), 205–226.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shapiro, L.
(2014), The Routledge Handbook of Embodied Cognition. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
(2011) Embodied Cognition. New York: Routledge Press.Google Scholar
(2013) When is cognition embodied? In U. Kriegel (Ed.), Current controversies in philosophy of mind (pp.73–90). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Sherman, G. D. & Clore, G. L.
(2009) The color of sin. White and black are perceptual symbols of moral purity and pollution. Psychological Science, 20(8), 1019–1025.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shinohara, K. & Matsunaka, Y.
(2009) Pictorial Metaphors of emotion in Japanese comics. In C. J. Forceville, & E. Urios-Aparisi (Eds.), Multimodal Metaphor (pp.265–296). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Shtyrov, Y., Smith, M. L., Horner, A. J., Henson, R., Nathan, P. J., Bullmore, E. T., & Pulvermuller, F.
(2012) Attention to language: novel MEG paradigm for registering involuntary language processing in the brain . Neuropsychologia, 50, 2605–2616.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Singh, N., & Mishra, R. K.
(2013) Second language proficiency modulates conflict-monitoring in an oculomotor Stroop task: evidence from Hindi-English bilinguals. Frontiers in Psychology, 12(4), 322. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015) Unintentional Activation of Translation Equivalents in Bilinguals Leads to Attention Capture in a Cross-Modal Visual Task. PlonsOne, 10(3). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Singerland, E.
(2003) Effortless action: Wu-wei as conceptual metaphor and spiritual ideal in early China. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Siquerra, M., & Gibbs, R.
(2007) Children’s acquisition of primary Metaphors: A cross-linguistic study. Organon, 43, 161–179.Google Scholar
Skoufaki, S.
(2009) Investigating the source of idiom transparency intuitions. Metaphor and Symbol, 24, 20–41.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Slobin, D. I.
(1985) Crosslinguistic evidence for the Language-Making Capacity. In D. I. Slobin (Ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition: Vol. 2. Theoretical issues (pp.1157–1256). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Spaulding, S.
(2013) Mirror Neurons and Social Cognition. Mind and Language, 28(2), 233–257.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sperry, R. W.
(1950) Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual inversion. J Comp Physiol Psychol, 43(6) 482–489.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Spivey, M.
(2007) The continuity of mind . New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Steen, G. J.
(1994) Understanding metaphor in literature: An empirical approach. London: Longman.Google Scholar
(2007) Finding metaphor in grammar and usage. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2008) The paradox of metaphor: Why we need a three-dimensional model for metaphor. Metaphor & Symbol, 23(4), 213–241.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011a) The contemporary theory of metaphor–now new and improved!. Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 9(1), 26–64.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2013) Deliberate metaphor affords conscious metaphorical cognition. Journal of Cognitive Semiotics 5 (1–2), 179–197.Google Scholar
(2015) Developing, testing and interpreting Deliberate Metaphor Theory . Journal of Pragmatics, 90, 67–72.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Steen, G.
(2017), Deliberate Metaphor Theory: Basic assumptions, main tenets, urgent issues, Intercultural Pragmatics 2017; 14(1): 1–24.Google Scholar
Steen, G., Reijnierse, W. G., & Burgers, C. F.
(2014) When do natural language Metaphors influence reasoning? A follow-up study to Thibodeau and Boroditsky (2013). PLoS One, 9(12): e113536. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Steen, G.J., A.G., Dorst, B., Herrmann, A. Kaal, T. Krennmayr & T. Pasma
(2010) A Method for Linguistic Metaphor Identification: From MIP to MIPVU Converging Evidence in Language and Communication Research, 14.Google Scholar
Stefanowitsch, A., & Gries, S.
(2006) Corpus-based approaches to metaphor and metonymy. Berlin, Germany: Mouton.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Storbeck, J., & Clore, G. L.
(2008) Affective arousal as information: How affective arousal influences judgments, learning, and memory. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 1824–1843.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Strack, F., Martin, L. L. & Stepper, S.
(1988) Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: a non obtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis . J Pers Soc Psychol, 54(5), 768–777.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Strafella, A. P., & Paus, T.
(2000) Modulation of cortical excitability during action observation: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Neuroreport, 11(10), 2289–2292.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sur, S. & Sinha, V. K.
(2009) Event-related potentials: An overview. Industrial Psychiatry Journal,18(1), 70–73. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sweetser, E.
(1990) From etymology to pragmatics: Metaphorical and cultural aspects of semantic structure. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Symes, E., Tucker, M., Ellis, R., Vainio, L., Ottoboni, G.
(2008) Grasp preparation improves change-detection for congruent objects. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform, 34(4), 854–871. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Talmy, L.
(1988) Force dynamics in language and cognition. Cognitive science, 12(1), 49–100.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2000).Toward a Cognitive Semantics, Volume 2. Cambridge, Massachusetts; London: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Taub, S.
(2001) Language from the body. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tendahl, M., & Gibbs, R.
(2008) Complementary perspectives on metaphor: Cognitive linguistics and relevance theory. Journal of Pragmatics, 40, 1823–1864.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B.
(1994) A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. Cambridge, Massachusetts; London: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Thelen, E., Schöner, G., Scheier, C., & Smith, L.
(2001) The Dynamics of Embodiment: A Field Theory of Infant Perseverative Reaching. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 24, 1–86.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thibodeau, P., & Durgin, F.
(2008) Productive figurative communication: Conventional Metaphors facilitate the comprehension of related novel Metaphors. Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 521–540.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tiemersma, D.
(1989)  Body schema and body image : an interdisciplinary and philosophical study. Amsterdam; Bristol, PA: Swets & Zeitlinger.Google Scholar
Tilley, C.
(1999) Metaphor and material culture. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Tipper S. P., Paul, M., & Hayes, A.
(2006) Vision-for-action: the effects of object property discrimination and action state on affordance compatibility effects. Psychonomic Bull Rev, 13, 493–498.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tracy, J.L. & D. Matsumoto
(2008) The spontaneous expression of pride and shame: Evidence for biologically innate nonverbal displays. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 105(33). 11655–11660.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tucker, M., & Ellis, R.
(1998) On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform, 24, 830–846.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Turner, M.
(1991) Reading Minds: The Study of English in the Age of Cognitive Science. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
(1996) The literary mind . Princeton., NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Ulloa, E. R., Pineda, J. A.
(2007) Recognition of point-light biological motion: mu rhythms and mirror-neuron activity. Behav Brain Res, 183:188–194.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Umiltà, M. A., Escola, L., Intskirveli, I., Grammont, F., Rochat, M., Caruana, F., Jezzini, A., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G.
(2008) When pliers become fingers in the monkey motor system. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, 105(6), 2209–2213.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Urgesi, C., Candidi, M., Fabbro, F., Romani, M., & Aglioti, S. M.
(2006) Motor facilitation during action observation: topographic mapping of the target muscle and influence of the onlooker’s posture. Eur J Neurosci, 23(9), 2522–2530.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wagenmakers, E. J., Beek, T., Dijkhoff, L. and Gronau Q. F.
(2016) Registered Replication Report: Strack, Martin, & Stepper (1988). Perspectives on Psychological Science, Vol. 11(6) 917–928.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vainio, L., Ellis, R., Tucker, M.
(2007) The role of visual attention in action priming. Q J Exp Psychol A, 60, 241–261.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Dam, W. O., Rueschemeyer, S. A., Lindemann, O., Bekkering, H.
(2010) Context effects in embodied lexical-semantic processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 1–6. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
VanDijk, T. A. & Kintsch, W.
(1983) Strategies of discourse comprehension. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Van Elk, M., Slors, M., & Bekkering, H.
(2010) Embodied language understanding requires an enactivist paradigm of cognition. Frontiers in Cognition, 1–9. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van Gelder, T.
(2005), What might cognition be if not computation?, Journal of Philosophy 92 (7):345–381 (1995).DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vervaeke, J., & Kennedy, J.
(1996) Metaphors in language and thought: Disproof and multiple meanings. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 11, 273–284.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Varela, F., Thompson, E., & Rosch, E.
(1991) The Embodied Mind: Cognitive Science and Human Experience. Cambridge, Massachussets: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wagner, S., & Gunter, T.
(2004) Determining inhibition: individual differences in the ‘lexicon context’ trade-off during lexical ambiguity resolution in working memory . Exp.Psychol. 51, 290–299. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wallace, S.
(1982) Figure and ground: The interrelationships of linguistic categories. Tense-aspect: Between semantics and pragmatics, 1, 201–223.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Waters, G., Caplan, D., & Yampolsky, S.
(2003) On-line syntactic processing under concurrent memory load. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(1), 88–95.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilcox, P. P.
(1993) Metaphorical Mapping in American Sign Language. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of New Mexico.Google Scholar
Wilcox, P.
(2000) Metaphor in American sign language. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.Google Scholar
Willems, R.
(2017) Cognitive Neuroscience of Natural Language Use. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Williams, L. E., & Bargh, J. A.
(2008a) Experiencing physical warmth influences interpersonal warmth. Science, 322, 606–607.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2008b) Keeping one’s distance: The influence of spatial distance cues on affect and evaluation. Psychological Science, 19, 302–308.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilson, M
(2002) Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 625–636.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilson, N., & Gibbs, R.
(2007) Real and imagined body movement primes metaphor comprehension. Cognitive Science, 31, 721–731.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Winter, S.
(2002) A clearing in the forest: Law, life, and mind . Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Winter, B. & T. Matlock
(2013) Making judgments based on similarity and proximity. Metaphor and Symbol 28(4). 219–232.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wiseman, R.
(2007) Ancient Roman Metaphors for communication. Metaphor and Symbol, 22, 41–78.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Williams, L. E. & J.A. Bargh
(2008) Experiencing physical warmth promotes interpersonal warmth. Science 322(5901). 606–607.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wojciehowski, H., & Gallese, V.
(2011) How Stories Make Us Feel: Toward an Embodied Narratology. California Italian Studies, 2,1.Google Scholar
Yu, N.
(1999) The contemporary theory of metaphor: A perspective from Chinese. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
(2003) Chinese Metaphors of thinking. Cognitive Linguistics, 14, 141–165.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2008) The Chinese heart in a cognitive perspective: Culture, body, and language. Berlin, Germany: Mouton.Google Scholar
Zbikowski, L.
(2002) Conceptualizing music. Cognitive structure, theory, and analysis. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zhong, C. B., & Liljenquist, K.
(2006) Washing away your sins: Threatened morality and physical cleansing. Science, 313, 1451–1452.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zinken, J.
(2007) Discourse Metaphors: The link between figurative language and habitual analogies. Cognitive Linguistics, 18(3), 445–466.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zinken, J., Hellsten, I., & Nerlich, B.
(2008) Discourse Metaphors. In R. Dirven, R. Frank, T. Ziemke & J. Zlatev (Eds.), Body, Language, and Mind Vol. 2: Sociocultural Situatedness (pp.363–385). Berlin: Mouton.Google Scholar
Zwaan, R. A.
(2014) Embodiment and language comprehension: reframing the discussion. Trends Cogn Sci, 18(5), 229–234.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015) Situation models, mental simulations, and abstract concepts in discourse comprehension. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, June 2015.Google Scholar