Abbot-Smith, K., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Exemplar-learning and schematization in a usage-based account of syntactic acquisition. The Linguistic Review, 231, 275–290. 

Allen, K., Pereira, F., Botvinick, M., & Goldberg, A.E. (2012). Distinguishing grammatical constructions with fMRI pattern analysis. Brain and Language, 123(3), 174–182. 

Arbib, M.A., & Lee, J. (2008). Describing visual scenes: Towards a neurolinguistics based on construction grammar. Brain Research, 12251, 146–162. 

Arnon, I., & Snider, N. (2010). More than words: Frequency effects for multi-word phrases. Journal of Memory and Language, 621, 67–82. 

Baayen, R.H., Hendrix, P., & Ramscar, M. (2013). Sidestepping the combinatorial explosion: An explanation of N-gram frequency effects based on naive discriminative learning. Language and Speech, 56(3), 329–347. 

Bajada, C.J., Ralph, M.A.L., & Cloutman, L.L. (2015). Transport for language South of the Sylvian Fissure: The routes and history of the main tracts and stations in the ventral language network. Cortex, 691, 141–151. 

Bannard, C., & Matthews, D. (2008). Stored word sequences in language learning: The effect of familiarity on children’s repetition of four-word combinations. Psychological Science, 191, 241–248. 

Bardouille, T., & Bow, S. (2012). State-related changes in MEG functional connectivity reveal the task-positive sensorimotor network. PLoS One, 71, 1–8. 

Bell, A., Brenier, J.M., Gregory, M., Girand, C., & Jurafsky, D. (2009). Predictability effects on durations of content and function words in conversational English. Journal of Memory and Language, 601, 92–111. 

Bemis, D.K., & Pylkkänen, L. (2012). Basic linguistic composition recruits the left anterior temporal lobe and left angular gyrus during both listening and reading. Cerebral Cortex, bhs170.
Bemis, D.K., & Pylkkänen, L. (2013). Flexible composition: MEG evidence for the deployment of basic combinatorial linguistic mechanisms in response to task demands. PloS One, 81, e73949. 

Bien, H., Baayen, H.R., & Levelt, W.J.M. (2011). Frequency effects in the production of Dutch deverbal adjectives and inflected verbs. In R. Bertram, J. Hyönä, & M. Laine (Eds.), Morphology in language comprehension, production and acquisition (pp. 683–715). London: Psychology Press.
Binder, J., Desai, R., Graves, W., & Conant, L. (2009, DEC). Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex, 191, 2767–2796. 

Binney, R.J., Parker, G.J.M., & Lambon Ralph, M.A. (2012). Convergent Connectivity and Graded Specialization in the Rostral Human Temporal Lobe as Revealed by Diffusion-Weighted Imaging Probabilistic Tractography. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 241, 1998–2014. 

Bock, J. (1986). Meaning, sound, and syntax - Lexical priming in sentence production. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition, 121, 575–586. 

Bonaiuto, J. & Arbib, M.A. (2010). Extending the mirror neuron system model, II: what did I just do? A new role for mirror neurons. Biological Cybernetics, 1021, 341–59. 

Bonner, M.F., Peelle, J.E., Cook, P.A., & Grossman, M. (2013). Heteromodal conceptual processing in the angular gyrus. NeuroImage, 711, 175–186. 

Bornkessel, I., Zysset, S., Friederici, A.D., von Cramon, D.Y., & Schlesewsky, M. (2005). Who did what to whom? The neural basis of argument hierarchies during language comprehension. NeuroImage, 261, 221–233. 

Brennan, J., Nir, Y., Hasson, U., Malach, R., Heeger, D., & Pylkkänen, L. (2012). Syntactic structure building in the anterior temporal lobe during natural story listening. Brain and Language, 1201, 163–173. 

Brookes, M.J., Woolrich, M., Luckhoo, H., Price, D., Hale, J.R., Stephenson, M.C., Barnes, G.R., Smith, S.M., & Morris, P.G., (2011). Investigating the electrophysiological basis of resting state networks using magnetoencephalography. Procedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 1081, 16783–16788. 

Bybee, J., & McClelland, J.L. (2005). Alternatives to the combinatorial paradigm of linguistic theory based on domain general principles of human cognition. The Linguistic Review, 221, 381–410. 

Caplan, D., Chen, E., & Waters, G. (2008). Task-dependent and task-independent neurovascular responses to syntactic processing. Cortex, 441, 257–275. 

Catani, M., Jones, D., & Fytche, D. (2005). Perisylvian language networks of the human brain. Annals of Neurology, 571, 8–16. 

Catani, M., Mesulam, M.M., Jakobsen, E., Malik, F., Matersteck, A., Wieneke, C., Thompson, C.K., Thiebaut de Schotten, M., Dell’Acqua, F., Weintraub, S., & Rogalski, E. (2013). A novel frontal pathway underlies verbal fluency in primary progressive aphasia. Brain, awt163, 2619–2628. 

Cohen, J. (1983). The cost of dichotomization. Applied Psychological Measurement, 71, 249–253. 

Columbus, G. (2010). Processing MWUs: Are MWU subtypes psycholinguistically real? In D. Wood (Ed.), Perspectives on formulaic language: Acquisition and communication (pp. 194–212). London and New York: Continuum.
Columbus, G. (2012). An analysis of the processing of multiword units in sentence reading and unit presentation using eye movement data: Implications for theories of mwus. PhD dissertation, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Canada.
Culicover, P.W., & Jackendoff, R. (2005). Simpler syntax. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. 

De Cat, C., Klepousniotou, E., & Baayen, R.H. (2015). Representational deficit or processing effect? An electrophysiological study of noun-noun compound processing by very advanced L2 speakers of English. Frontiers in Psychology, 61, 77 

Dehaene‐Lambertz, G., Dehaene, S., Anton, J.L., Campagne, A., Ciuciu, P., Dehaene, G.P., Denghien, I., Jobert, A., Lebihan, D., Sigman, M., Pallier, C., & Poline, J.B. (2006). Functional segregation of cortical language areas by sentence repetition. Human Brain Mapping, 271, 360–371. 

Del Prato, P., & Pylkkanen, L. (2014). MEG evidence for conceptual combination but not numeral quantification in the left anterior temporal lobe during language production. Language Sciences, 51, 524.
den Ouden, D.B., Saur, D., Mader, W., Schelter, B., Lukic, S., Wali, E., Timmer, J., & Thompson, C.K. (2012). Network modulation during complex syntactic processing. NeuroImage, 591, 815–823. 

Dikker, S., & Pylkkänen, L. (2013). Predicting language: MEG evidence for lexical preactivation. Brain and language, 1271, 55–64. 

Dronkers, N., Wilkins, D., Van Jr., V., Redfern, B., & Jaeger, J.J. (2004). Lesion analysis of the brain areas involved in language comprehension. Cognition, 921, 145–177. 

Duffau, H., Moritz-Gasser, S., & Mandonnet, E. (2014). A re-examination of the neural basis of language processing: Proposal of a dynamic hodotopical model from data provided by brain stimulation mapping during picture naming. Brain and Language, 1311, 1–10. 

Ellis, N.C., & Simpson-Vlach, R. (2009). Formulaic language in native speakers: Triangulating psycholinguistics, corpus linguistics, and education. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory, 5(1), 61–78. 

Elman, J.L. (2009). On the meaning of words and dinosaur bones: Lexical knowledge without a lexicon. Cognitive Science, 331, 547–582. 

Embick, D., Hackl, M., Schaeffer, J., Kelepir, M., & Marantz, A. (2001). A magnetoencephalographic component whose latency reflects lexical frequency. Cognitive Brain Research, 101, 345–348. 

Erickson, L.C., Zielinski, B.A., Zielinski, J.E., Liu, G., Turkeltaub, P.E., Leaver, A.M., & Rauschecker, J.P. (2014). Distinct cortical locations for integration of audiovisual speech and the McGurk effect. Language Sciences, 51, 534.
Faraway, J.J. (2006). Extending linear models with R: Generalized linear, mixed effects and nonparametric regression models. Boca Raton, FL: Chapman & Hall/CRC.
Fedorenko, E., Duncan, J., & Kanwisher, N. (2012). Language-selective and domain-general regions lie side by side within Broca’s area. Current Biology, 221, 2059–2062. 

Fornito, A., Zalesky, A., & Breakspear, M. (2013). Graph analysis of the human connectome: Promise, progress, and pitfalls. NeuroImage 801, 426–444. 

Goldberg, A.E. (2003). Constructions: A new theoretical approach to language. Trends in Cognitive Science, 7(5), 219–224. 

Goldberg, A. (2006). Constructions at work: the nature of generalization in language. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Gregory, M.L., Raymond, W.D., Bell, A., Fosler-Lussier, E., & Jurafsky, D. (1999). The effects of collocational strength and contextual predictability in lexical production. CLS–99, 351, 151–166.
Grewe, T., Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I., Zysset, S., Wiese, R., von Cramon, D.Y., & Schlesewsky, M. (2007). The role of the posterior superior temporal sulcus in the processing of unmarked transitivity. NeuroImage, 351, 343–352. 

Griffiths, J.D., Marslen-Wilson, W.D., Stamatakis, E.A., & Tyler, L.K. (2013). Functional organization of the neural language system: Dorsal and ventral pathways are critical for syntax. Cerebral Cortex, 231, 139–147. 

Grill-Spector, K., Henson, R., & Martin, A. (2006). Repetition and the brain: Neural models of stimulus-specific effects. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 101, 14–23. 

Hagoort, P. (2005). On Broca, brain, and binding: A new framework. Trends in Cognitive Science, 91, 416–423. 

Hagoort, P. (2013). MUC (Memory, Unification, Control) and beyond. Frontiers in Psychology, 41. 

Haller, S., Klarhoefer, M., Schwarzbach, J., Radue, E.W., & Indefrey, P. (2007). Spatial and temporal analysis of fMRI data on word and sentence reading. European Journal of Neuroscience, 261, 2074–2084. 

Hansen, P.C., Kringelbach, M.L., & Salmelin, R. (2010). MEG: An introduction to methods. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. 

Hastie, T., & Tibshirani, R. (1990). Generalized additive model regression. New York, NY: Chapman & Hall.
Hebb, D.O. (1949). The organization of behavior. New York, NY: Wiley & Sons.
Hein, G., & Knight, R.T.R.T. (2008). Superior temporal sulcus – it’s my area: Or is it? Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 201, 2125–2136. 

Hendrix, P. (2009). Electrophysiological effects in language production: A picture naming study using generalized additive modeling. MA dissertation, Radboud University, Nijmegen, the Netherlands.
Herrmann, B., Maess, B., Hahne, A., Schröger, E., & Friederici, A.D. (2011). Syntactic and auditory spatial processing in the human temporal cortex: An MEG study. NeuroImage, 571, 624–633. 

Hickok, G., & Poeppel, D. (2004). Dorsal and ventral streams: A framework for understanding aspects of the functional anatomy of language. Cognition, 921, 67–99. 

Hickok, G., & Poeppel, D. (2007). The cortical organization of speech processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 81, 393–402. 

Hocking, J., & Price, C.J. (2008). The role of the posterior superior temporal sulcus in audiovisual processing. Cerebral Cortex, 181, 2439–2449. 

Hurley, R.S., Paller, K.A., Rogalski, E.J., & Mesulam, M.M. (2012). Neural mechanisms of object naming and word comprehension in primary progressive aphasia. The Journal of Neuroscience, 321, 4848–4855. 

Indefrey, P., Brown, C.M., Hellwig, F., Amunts, K., Herzog, H., & Seitz, R.J. (2001). A neural correlate of syntactic encoding during speech production.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
, 981, 5933–5936.
Indefrey, P., & Levelt, W.J. (2004). The spatial and temporal signatures of word production components. Cognition, 921, 101–144. 

Jackendoff, R. (2002a). Foundations of language. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. 

Jackendoff, R. (2002b). What’s in the lexicon? In S. Nooteboom, F. Weerman, & F. Wijnen (Eds.), Storage and computation in the language faculty (pp. 23–58). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Press. 

Jackendoff, R. (2007). A parallel architecture perspective on language processing. Brain Research, 11461, 2–22. 

Jackendoff, R. (2011). What is the human language faculty: Two views. Language, 871, 586–624. 

Janssen, N., & Barber, H.A. (2012). Phrase frequency effects in language production. PloS One, 71, e33202. 

Jiang, N., & Nekrasova, T. (2007). The processing of formulaic sequences by second language speakers. The Modern Language Journal, 911, 433–445. 

Kapatsinski, V., & Radicke, J. (2009). Frequency and the emergence of prefabs: Evidence from monitoring. In R. Corrigan, E. Moravcsik, H. Ouali, & K. Wheatley (Eds.), Formulaic language, Vol. ii: Acquisition, loss, psychological reality, functional explanations (pp. 499–522). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 

Keele, L. (2008). Semiparametric regression for the social sciences. New York, NY: Chapman & Hall/CRC.
Keller, F., & Lapata, M. (2003). Using the web to obtain frequencies for unseen bigrams. Journal Computational Linguistics, 291, 459–484. 

Kryuchkova, T., Tucker, B.V., Wurm, L.H., & Baayen, R.H. (2012). Danger and usefulness are detected early in auditory lexical processing: Evidence from electroencephalography. Brain and Language, 1221, 81–91. 

Lahnakoski, J.M., Glerean, E., Salmi, J., Jääskeläinen, I.P., Sams, M., Hari, R., & Nummenmaa, L. (2012). Naturalistic fMRI mapping reveals superior temporal sulcus as the hub for the distributed brain network for social perception. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 61, 233. 

Lamb, S. (1966). Outline of stratificational grammar. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.
Lau, E.F., Gramfort, A., Hämäläinen, M.S., & Kuperberg, G.R. (2013). Automatic semantic facilitation in anterior temporal cortex revealed through multimodal neuroimaging. The Journal of Neuroscience, 331, 17174–17181. 

Levelt, W.J. (1983). Monitoring and self-repair in speech. Cognition, 141, 41–104. 

Lewis, G., & Poeppel, D. (2014). The role of visual representations during the lexical access of spoken words. Brain and Language, 1341, 1–10. 

Lewis, G., Solomyak, O., & Marantz, A. (2011). The neural basis of obligatory decomposition of suffixed words. Brain and Language, 1181, 118–127. 

Li, L., Miller, E.K., & Desimone, R. (2004). The representation of stimulus familiarity in anterior inferior temporal cortex. Journal of Neuropsychology, 691, 1918–1929.
Llorens, A., Trébuchon, A., Riès, S., Liégeois-Chauvel, C., & Alario, F. (2014). How familiarization and repetition modulate the picture naming network. Brain and Language, 1331, 47–58. 

Lu, Z., & Kaufman, L. (2003). Magnetic source imaging of the human brain. New York, NY: Routledge. 

MacCallum, R., Zhang, S., Preacher, K., & Rucker, D. (2002). On the practice of dichotomization of quantitative variables. Psychological Methods, 71, 19–40. 

Magnusdottir, S., Fillmore, P., den Ouden, D.B., Hjaltason, H., Rorden, C., Kjartansson, O., Bonilha, L., & Fridriksson, J. (2013). Damage to left anterior temporal cortex predicts impairment of complex syntactic processing: A lesion‐symptom mapping study. Human Brain Mapping, 341, 2715–2723. 

Makuuchi, M., Bahlmann, J., Anwander, A., & Friederici, A.D. (2009). Segregating the core computational faculty of human language from working memory.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
, 1061, 8362–8367.
Makuuchi, M., & Friederici, A.D. (2013). Hierarchical functional connectivity between the core language system and the working memory system. Cortex, 491, 2416–2423. 

Marantz, A. (2005). Generative linguistics within the cognitive neuroscience of language. The Linguistic Review, 221, 429–445. 

Marchini, J., Heaton, C., & Ripley, B. (2012). fastICA: FastICA Algorithms to perform ICA and Projection Pursuit (R package version 1.1-16).
Marek, A., Habets, B., Jansma, B.M., Nager, W., & Muente, T.F. (2007). Neural correlates of conceptualisation difficulty during the preparation of complex utterances. Aphasiology, 211, 1147–1156. 

Martin, N., Weisberg, R., & Saffran, E. (1989). Variables influencing the occurrence of naming errors – implications for models of lexical retrieval. Journal of Memory and Language, 281, 462–485. 

Mei, L., Xue, G., Lu, Z-L., He, Q., Zhang, M., et al. (2014). Artificial language training reveals the neural substrates underlying addressed and assembled phonologies. PLoS ONE, 91, e93548. 

Mesulam, M.M. (1998). From sensation to cognition. Brain, 1211, 1013–1052. 

Mesulam, M., Rogalski, E., Wieneke, C., Cobia, D., Rademaker, A., Thompson, C., & Weintraub, S. (2009). Neurology of anomia in the semantic variant of primary progressive aphasia. Brain, awp138, 132(9), 2553–2565. 

Mesulam, M.M., Wieneke, C., Hurley, R., Rademaker, A., Thompson, C.K., Weintraub, S., & Rogalski, E.J. (2013). Words and objects at the tip of the left temporal lobe in primary progressive aphasia. Brain, 1361, 601–618. 

Meyer, C.F., Grabowski, R., Han, H.-Y., Mantzouranis, K., & Moses, S. (2003). The world wide web as linguistic corpus. In P. Leistyna & C.F. Meyer (Eds.), Language and computers, corpus analysis: Language structure and language use (pp. 241–254). Amsterdam: Rodopi. 

Mion, M., Patterson, K., Acosta-Cabronero, J., Pengas, G., Izquierdo-Garcia, D., Hong, Y.T., Fryer, T.D., Williams, G.B., Hodges, J.R., & Nestor, P.J. (2010). What the left and right anterior fusiform gyri tell us about semantic memory. Brain, 1331, 3256–3268. 

Motulsky, H., & Christopoulos, A. (2004). Fitting models to biological data using linear and non-linear regression. A practical guide to curve fitting. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Newman, A.J., Supalla, T., Hauser, P., Newport, E.L., & Bavelier, D. (2010). Dissociating neural subsystems for grammar by contrasting word order and inflection.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
, 1071, 7539–7544.
Noonan, K.A., Jefferies, E., Visser, M., & Lambon Ralph, M.A. (2013). Going beyond inferior prefrontal involvement in semantic control: evidence for the additional contribution of dorsal angular gyrus and posterior middle temporal cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 251:1824–1850. 

Noppeney, U., & Price, C.J. (2004). An fMRI study of syntactic adaptation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 161, 702–713. 

Okada, K., & Hickok, G. (2006). Left posterior auditory-related cortices participate both in speech perception and speech production: Neural overlap revealed by fMRI. Brain and Language, 961, 112–117. 

Oomen, C., & Postma, A. (2001). Effects of time pressure on mechanisms of speech production and self-monitoring. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 301, 163–184. 

Patterson, K., Nestor, P.J., & Rogers, T.T. (2007). Where do you know what you know? The representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 81, 976–987. 

Price, C. (2010). The anatomy of language: A review of 100 fMRI studies published in 2009. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences: The Year in Cognitive Neuroscience, 11911, 62–88. 

Price, C. (2012). A review and synthesis of the first 20 years of PET and fMRI studies of heard speech, spoken language and reading. NeuroImage, 151, 816–847. 

Pylkkänen, L., Stringfellow, A., Flagg, E., & Marantz, A. (2000). A neural response sensitive to repetition and phonotactic probability: MEG investigations of lexical access. In
Proceedings of Biomag
(pp. 363–367).
Pylkkänen, L., Stringfellow, A., & Marantz, A. (2002). Neuromagnetic evidence for the timing of lexical activation: An MEG component sensitive to phonotactic probability but not to neighborhood density. Brain and Language, 811, 666–678. 

Pylkkanen, L., & Marantz, A. (2003). Tracking the time course of word recognition with meg. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 71, 187–189. 

Pylkkänen, L., Feintuch, S., Hopkins, E., & Marantz, A. (2004). Neural correlates of the effects of morphological family frequency and family size: An MEG study. Cognition, 911, B35–B45. 

Rauschecker, J.P., & Scott, S.K. (2009). Maps and streams in the auditory cortex: Nonhuman primates illuminate human speech processing. Nature Neuroscience, 121, 718–724. 

R Development Core Team. (2012). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria (ISBN 3-900051-07-0).
Roelofs, A., Meyer, A.S., & Levelt, W.J. (1998). A case for the lemma/lexeme distinction in models of speaking: Comment on Caramazza and Miozzo (1997). Cognition, 69(2), 219–230. 

Saur, D., Kreher, B.W., Schnell, S., Kümmerer, D., Kellmeyer, P., Vry, M.S., Umarova, R., Musso, M., Glauche, V., Abel, S., Huber, W., Rijntjes, M., Hennig, J., & Weiller, C. (2008). Ventral and dorsal pathways for language.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
, 1051, 18035–18040.
Segaert, K., Kempen, G., Petersson, K.M., & Hagoort, P. (2013). Syntactic priming and the lexical boost effect during sentence production and sentence comprehension: An fMRI study. Brain and Language, 124(2), 174–183. 

Schwartz, M.F., Kimberg, D.Y., Walker, G.M., Faseyitan, O., Brecher, A., Dell, G.S., & Coslett, H.B. (2009). Anterior temporal involvement in semantic word retrieval: VLSM evidence from aphasia. Brain, 1321, 3411–3427. 

Sekiguchi, T., Koyama, S., & Kakigi, R. (2000). The effect of word repetition on evoked magnetic responses in the human brain. Japanese Psychological Research, 42(1), 3–14. 

Simon, D.A., Lewis, G., & Marantz, A. (2012). Disambiguating form and lexical frequency effects in MEG responses using homonyms. Language and Cognitive Processes, 271, 275–287. 

Siyanova-Chanturia, A., Conklin, K., & Heuven, W.J.van. (2011). Seeing a phrase “time and again” matters: The role of phrasal frequency in the processing of multiword sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 371, 776–784. 

Solomyak, O., & Marantz, A. (2009). Lexical access in early stages of visual word processing: A single-trial correlational MEG study of heteronym recognition. Brain and Language, 1081, 191–196. 

Sosa, A.V., & MacFarlane, J. (2002). Evidence for frequency-based constituents in the mental lexicon: Collocations involving the word of. Brain and Language, 821, 227–236. 

Taulu, S., & Simola, J. (2006). Spatiotemporal signal space separation method for rejecting nearby interference in MEG measurements. Physics in Medicine and Biology, 511, 1759–1768. 

Thelwall, M., & Sud, P. (2012). Webometrics research with the Bing Search API 2.0. Journal of Informetrics, 61, 44–52. 

Tremblay, A. (2009). Processing advantages of lexical bundles: Evidence from self-paced reading, word and sentence recall, and free recall with event-related brain potential recordings. PhD dissertation. University of Alberta.
Tremblay, A. (2013). icaOcularCorrection: Independent components analysis (ICA) based eye-movement correction (R package version 2.4.3.5.1 alpha – pre-release).
Tremblay, A., & Baayen, R.H. (2010). Holistic processing of regular four-word sequences: A behavioral and ERP study of the effects of structure, frequency, and probability on immediate free recall. In D. Wood (Ed.), Perspectives on formulaic language: Acquisition and communication (pp. 151–173). London and New York: Continuum.
Tremblay, A., Derwing, B., Libben, G., & Westbury, C. (2011). Processing advantages of lexical bundles: Evidence from self-paced reading and sentence recall tasks. Language Learning, 611, 569–613. 

Tremblay, A. & Newman, A.J. (2015). Modeling non-linear relationships in ERP data using mixed-effects regression with R examples. Psychophysiology, 1–16. 

Troyer, M., O’Donnell, T.J., Fedorenko, E., & Gibson, E. (2011). In N.A. Taatgen & H. van Rijn (Eds.), Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 336–341). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
Tyler, L.K., Marslen-Wilson, W.D., Randall, B., Wright, P., Devereux, B.J., Zhuang, J., Papoutsi, M., & Stamatakis, E.A. (2011). Left inferior frontal cortex and syntax: function, structure and behaviour in patients with left hemisphere damage. Brain, 1341, 415–431. 

Ullman, M. (2007). The biocognition of the mental lexicon. In M.G. Gaskell (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 267–286). Oxford: Oxford University Press. 
Vigneau, M., Beaucousin, V., Herve, P., Duffau, H., Crivello, F., & Houde, O. (2006). Meta-analyzing left hemisphere language areas: Phonology, semantics, and sentence processing. NeuroImage, 301, 1414–1432. 

Visser, M., & Ralph, M.L. (2011). Differential contributions of bilateral ventral anterior temporal lobe and left anterior superior temporal gyrus to semantic processes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 231, 3121–3131. 

Vrba, J., Taulu, S., Nenonen, J., & Ahonen, A. (2010). Signal space separation beamformer. Brain Topography, 231, 128–133. 

Walker, G.M., Schwartz, M.F., Kimberg, D.Y., Faseyitan, O., Brecher, A., Dell, G.S., & Coslett, H.B. (2011). Support for anterior temporal involvement in semantic error production in aphasia: New evidence from VLSM. Brain and Language, 1171, 110–122. 

Westerlund, M., & Pylkkänen, L. (2014). The role of the left anterior temporal lobe in semantic composition vs. semantic memory. Neuropsychologia, 571, 59–70. 

Willems, R.M., Özyürek, A., & Hagoort, P. (2009). Differential roles for left inferior frontal and superior temporal cortex in multimodal integration of action and language. NeuroImage, 471, 1992–2004. 

Wilson, S.M., Demarco, A.T., Henry, M.L., Gesierich, B., Babiak, M., Mandelli, M.L., Miller, B.L., & Gorno-Tempini, M.L. (2014). What role does the anterior temporal lobe play in sentence-level processing? Neural correlates of syntactic processing in semantic variant primary progressive Aphasia. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26, 970. 

Wood, S.N. (2006). Generalized additive models. New York, NY: Chapman & Hall/CRC. 

Wood, S.N. (2012). mgcv: Mixed GAM Computation Vehicle with GCV/AIC/REML Smoothness Estimation (R package version 1.7-22).
Zhang, L., Xi, J., Xu, G., Shu, H., Wang, X., & Li, P. (2011). Cortical dynamics of acoustic and phonological processing in speech perception. PloS One, 61, e20963. 

Zuur, A.F., Ieno, E.N., Walker, N.J., Saveliev, A.A., & Smith, G.M. (2009). Mixed effects models and extensions in ecology with R. New York, NY: Springer. 
