Article published In:
The Mental Lexicon
Vol. 12:2 (2017) ► pp.263282
References
Andrews, S.
(1992) Frequency and neighbourhood effects on lexical access: Lexical similarity or orthographic redundancy? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 18(2), 234–254.Google Scholar
Baayen, R. H.
(2008) Analyzing Linguistic Data: A Practical Introduction to Statistics using R. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Balota, D. A., Yap, M. J., Cortese, M. J., Hutchison, K. A., Kessler, B., Loftis, B., … Treiman, R.
(2007) The English Lexicon Project. Behavior Research Methods, 39(3), 445–459. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Biedermann, G.
(1966) The recognition of tachistoscopically presented five-letter words as a function of digram frequency. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 51, 208–209. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Binder, J. R., Medler, D. A., Westbury, C. F., Liebenthal, E., & Buchanan, L.
(2006) Tuning of the human left fusiform gyrus to sublexical orthographic structure. Neuroimage, 33(2), 739–748. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Broadbent, D., & Gregory, M.
(1968) Visual perception of words differing in letter digram frequency. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 7(2), 569–571. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chetail, F.
(2015) Reconsidering the role of orthographic redundancy in visual word recognition. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(645), 1–10. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chetail, F., Balota, D., Treiman, R., & Content, A.
(2015) What can megastudies tell us about the orthographic structure of English words? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68(8), 1519–1540. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Coltheart, M., Davelaar, E., Jonasson, T., & Besner, D.
(1977) Access to the internal lexicon. In S. Dornic (Ed.), Attention and Performance, VI (pp. 535–555). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J.
(2001) DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108(1), 204–256. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cutler, A.
(1981) Making up materials is a confounded nuisance: or Will we be able to run any psycholinguistic experiments at all in 1990? Cognition, 10(1–3), 65–70. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Davis, C.
(2005) N-Watch: A program for deriving neighborhood size and other psycholinguistic statistics. Behavior Research Methods, 37(1), 65–70. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dehaene, S.
(2009) Reading in the brain: The new science of how we read. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S., Cohen, L., Sigman, M., & Vinckier, F.
(2005) The neural code for written words: a proposal. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(7), 335–341. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dienes, Z.
(2014) Using Bayes to get the most out of non-significant results. Frontiers in Psychology, 51, 1–17. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Duyck, W., Desmet, T., Verbeke, L. P. C., & Brysbaert, M.
(2004) WordGen: A tool for word selection and nonword generation in Dutch, English, German, and French. Behavior Research Methods Instruments & Computers, 36(3), 488–499. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ferrand, L., New, B., Brysbaert, M., Keuleers, E., Bonin, P., Méot, A., … Pallier, C.
(2010) The French Lexicon Project: Lexical decision data for 38,840 French words and 38,840 pseudowords. Behavior Research Methods, 42(2), 488–496. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Frankish, C., & Barnes, L.
(2008) Lexical and sublexical processes in the perception of transposed-letter anagrams. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(3), 381–391. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gelman, A., & Rubin, D. B.
(1995) Avoiding model selection in Bayesian social research. Sociological Methodology, 251, 165–173. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gernsbacher, M. A.
(1984) Resolving 20 years of inconsistent interactions between lexical familiarity and orthography, concreteness, and polysemy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113(2), 256–281. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gigerenzer, G.
(1998) Surrogates for theories. Theory and Psychology, 81, 195–204. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grainger, J., & Whitney, C.
(2004) Does the huamn mnid raed wrods as a wlohe? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(2), 58–59. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grainger, J., & Ziegler, J.
(2011) A dual-route approach to orthographic processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 21, 1–13. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Keuleers, E., & Brysbaert, M.
Keuleers, E., Diependaele, K., & Brysbaert, M.
(2010) Practice effects in large-scale visual word recognition studies: A lexical decision study on 14,000 Dutch mono-and disyllabic words and nonwords. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 1–15. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Keuleers, E., Lacey, P., Rastle, K., & Brysbaert, M.
(2012) The British Lexicon Project: Lexical decision data for 28,730 monosyllabic and disyllabic English words. Behavior Research Methods, 44(1), 287–304. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kinoshita, S., & Norris, D.
(2013) Letter order is not coded by open bigrams. Journal of Memory and Language, 69(2), 135–150. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kruschke, J.
(2014) Doing Bayesian data analysis: A tutorial with R, JAGS, and Stan. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Loxton, D.
(2015, May 24th). History and Hyman’s Maxim [Blog post]. Retrieved from [URL]
Medler, D. A., & Binder, R. J.
(2005) MCWord: An on-line orthographic database of the English language. Retrieved 20.5.2015 from [URL]
McClelland, J. L., & Johnston, J. C.
(1977) The role of familiar units in perception of words and nonwords. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 22(3), 249–261. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morey, R. D., & Rouder, J. N.
(2014) Package “BayesFactor”. Retrieved 9.8.2014, from [URL]
New, B., Ferrand, L., Pallier, C., & Brysbaert, M.
(2006) Reexamining the word length effect in visual word recognition: New evidence from the English Lexicon Project. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13(1), 45–52. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Perea, M., & Carreiras, M.
(2008) Do orthotactics and phonology constrain the transposed-letter effect? Language and Cognitive Processes, 23(1), 69–92. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
R Core Team
(2013) R: A language environment for statistical computing [Computer software manual]. Vienna. Retrieved from [URL]
Rayner, K., Pollatsek, A., Drieghe, D., Slattery, T. J., & Reichle, E. D.
(2007) Tracking the mind during reading via eye movements: Comments on Kliegl, Nuthmann, and Engbert (2006). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136(3), 520–529. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rice, G., & Robinson, D.
(1975) The role of bigram frequency in the perception of words and nonwords. Memory & Cognition, 3(5), 513–518. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rouder, J. N., Speckman, P. L., Sun, D. C., Morey, R. D., & Iverson, G.
(2009) Bayesian t tests for accepting and rejecting the null hypothesis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(2), 225–237. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rumelhart, D. E., & Siple, P.
(1974) Process of recognizing tachistoscopically presented words. Psychological Review, 81(2), 99–118. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schmalz, X.
(2015, July 17th). Hyman’s Maxim: The most important principle in observational sciences? [Blog post]. Retrieved from [URL]
Schoonbaert, S., & Grainger, J.
(2004) Letter position coding in printed word perception: Effects of repeated and transposed letters. Language and Cognitive Processes, 19(3), 333–367. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Simonsohn, U.
(2015, September 4th). The default Bayesian test is prejudiced against small effects [Blog post]. Retrieved from [URL]
van Heuven, W., Mandera, P., Keuleers, E., & Brysbaert, M.
(2014) SUBTLEX-UK: A new and improved word frequency database for British English. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(6), 1176–1190. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vitevitch, M. S., & Luce, P. A.
(2016) Phonological neighborhood effects in spoken word perception and production. Annual Review of Linguistics, 21, 75–94. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Weekes, B.
(1997) Differential Effects of Number of Letters on Word and Nonword Naming Latency. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50A(2), 439–456. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Whitney, C.
(2001) How the brain encodes the order of letters in a printed word: The SERIOL model and selective literature review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8(2), 221–243. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2008) Comparison of the SERIOL and SOLAR theories of letter-position encoding. Brain and Language, 107(2), 170–178. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yarkoni, T., Balota, D., & Yap, M.
(2008) Moving beyond Coltheart’s N: A new measure of orthographic similarity. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(5), 971–979. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 8 other publications

Itkonen, Sami, Tuomo Häikiö, Seppo Vainio & Minna Lehtonen
2024. LASTU: A psycholinguistic search tool for Finnish lexical stimuli. Behavior Research Methods DOI logo
Lelonkiewicz, Jarosław R., Maria Ktori & Davide Crepaldi
2020. Morphemes as letter chunks: Discovering affixes through visual regularities. Journal of Memory and Language 115  pp. 104152 ff. DOI logo
Lelonkiewicz, Jarosław R., Maria Ktori & Davide Crepaldi
2023. Morphemes as letter chunks: Linguistic information enhances the learning of visual regularities. Journal of Memory and Language 130  pp. 104411 ff. DOI logo
Lelonkiewicz, Jarosław R., Michael T. Ullman & Davide Crepaldi
2022. Knowledge of Statistics or Statistical Learning? Readers Prioritize the Statistics of their Native Language Over the Learning of Local Regularities. Journal of Cognition 5:1  pp. 18 ff. DOI logo
Mandera, Paweł, Emmanuel Keuleers & Marc Brysbaert
2020. Recognition times for 62 thousand English words: Data from the English Crowdsourcing Project. Behavior Research Methods 52:2  pp. 741 ff. DOI logo
Schmalz, Xenia, Kristina Moll, Claudio Mulatti & Gerd Schulte-Körne
2019. Is Statistical Learning Ability Related to Reading Ability, and If So, Why?. Scientific Studies of Reading 23:1  pp. 64 ff. DOI logo
Schmalz, Xenia, Barbara Treccani & Claudio Mulatti
2021. Developmental Dyslexia, Reading Acquisition, and Statistical Learning: A Sceptic’s Guide. Brain Sciences 11:9  pp. 1143 ff. DOI logo
Vidal, Yamil, Eva Viviani, Davide Zoccolan & Davide Crepaldi
2021. A general-purpose mechanism of visual feature association in visual word identification and beyond. Current Biology 31:6  pp. 1261 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 5 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.