Article published in:
The Mental Lexicon
Vol. 16:2/3 (2021) ► pp. 397421
References
Ambridge, B.
(2020a) Against stored abstractions: A radical exemplar model of language acquisition. First Language, 40 (5–6), 509–559. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2020b) Abstractions made of exemplars or ‘You’re all right, and I’ve changed my mind’: Response to commentators. First Language, 40 (5–6), 640–659. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Anglin, J. M.
(1977) Word, object, and conceptual development.‏ Norton & Co.Google Scholar
Aronoff, M.
(2007) Language: Between words and grammar. In G. Jarema & G. Libben, G. (Eds.), The mental lexicon: Core perspectives (pp. 55–79). Brill.Google Scholar
Bachra, B. N.
(2001) The phonological structure of the verbal roots in Arabic and Hebrew (Vol. 341). Brill. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baldwin, D. A.
(1993) Early referential understanding: Infants’ ability to recognize referential acts for what they are. Developmental Psychology, 29 (5), 832. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Banai, K., Nir, B., Moav-Scheff, R., & Bar-Ziv, N.
(2020) A role for incidental auditory learning in auditory-visual word learning among kindergarten children. Journal of Vision, 20 (3), 4–4. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Berman, R. A.
(2003) Children’s lexical innovations. Language Acquisition and Language Disorders, 28 1, 243–292. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2013) Modern Hebrew. In R. Hetzron (Ed.), The Semitic languages (pp. 312–333). Routledge.Google Scholar
Berman, R., & Seroussi, B.
(2011) Derived nouns in Modern Hebrew: Structural and psycholinguistic perspectives. Italian Journal of Linguistics, 23 (1), 105–125.Google Scholar
Bloom, P.
(2000) How children learn the meanings of words (Vol. 3771). MIT Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, P., & Markson, L.
(1998) Capacities underlying word learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2 (2), 67–73. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chen, C. H., Zhang, Y., & Yu, C.
(2018) Learning object names at different hierarchical levels using cross-situational statistics. Cognitive Science, 42 1, 591–605. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gelman, S. A., & O’Reilly, A. W.
(1988) Children’s inductive inferences within superordinate categories: The role of language and category structure. Child Development, 59 (4), 876–887. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gennari, S. P., Sloman, S. A., Malt, B. C., & Fitch, W. T.
(2002) Motion events in language and cognition. Cognition, 83 (1), 49–79. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Goldinger, S. D.
(1996) Words and voices: episodic traces in spoken word identification and recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22 (5), 1166.Google Scholar
Gupta, P.
(2003) Examining the relationship between word learning, nonword repetition, and immediate serial recall in adults. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 56 (7), 1213–1236. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hall, D. G.
(1991) Acquiring proper nouns for familiar and unfamiliar animate objects: Two-year-olds’ word-learning biases. Child Development, 62 (5), 1142–1154. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Horton, M. S., & Markman, E. M.
(1980) Developmental differences in the acquisition of basic and superordinate categories. Child Development, 708–719. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Huang, Y. T., & Arnold, A. R.
(2016) Word learning in linguistic context: Processing and Memory Effects. Cognition, 156 1, 71–87. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jaeger, T. F.
(2008) Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language, 59 (4), 434–446. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Labov, W.
(1972) The transformation of experience in narrative syntax. In W. Labov, Language in The Inner City (pp. 354–396). University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
Lieber, R.
(2005) English word-formation processes. In P. Stekauer, & R. Lieber (Eds.), Handbook of word-formation (pp. 375–427). Springer. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lupyan, G.
(2012) Linguistically modulated perception and cognition: the label-feedback hypothesis. Frontiers in Psychology, 3 1. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lupyan, G., Rakison, D. H., & McClelland, J. L.
(2007) Language is not just for talking: Redundant labels facilitate learning of novel categories. Psychological Science, 18 (12), 1077–1083. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Markman, A. B., & Wisniewski, E. J.
(1997) Similar and different: The differentiation of basic-level categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23 (1), 54–70.Google Scholar
Mather, E., & Plunkett, K.
(2012) The role of novelty in early word learning. Cognitive Science, 36 (7), 1157–1177. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Maxfield, J. T., & Zelinsky, G. J.
(2012) Searching through the hierarchy: How level of target categorization affects visual search. Visual Cognition, 20 (10), 1153–1163. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Markman, E. M., & Wachtel, G. F.
(1988) Children’s use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words. Cognitive Psychology, 20 (2), 121–157. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mervis, C. B., & Rosch, E.
(1981) Categorization of natural objects. Annual Review of Psychology, 32 (1), 89–115. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Minda, J. P., Desroches, A. S., & Church, B. A.
(2008) Learning rule-described and non-rule-described categories: a comparison of children and adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34 (6), 1518–1533.Google Scholar
Moav-Scheff, R., Yifat, R., & Banai, K.
(2015) Phonological memory and word learning deficits in children with specific language impairment: A role for perceptual context?. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 45 1, 384–399. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Muchnik, M.
(2017) Personal names in Modern Hebrew: A morphosyntactic and gender analysis. Folia Linguistica, 51 (2), 369–390. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Murphy, G.
(2004) The big book of concepts. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Murphy, G. L., & Brownell, H. H.
(1985) Category differentiation in object recognition: typicality constraints on the basic category advantage. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11 (1), 70–84.Google Scholar
Murphy, G. L., & Wisniewski, E. J.
(1989) Categorizing objects in isolation and in scenes: What a superordinate is good for. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15 (4), 572–586.Google Scholar
Nazzi, T., & Gopnik, A.
(2001) Linguistic and cognitive abilities in infancy: When does language become a tool for categorization?. Cognition, 80 (3), B11–B20. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nosofsky, R. M.
(1986) Attention, similarity, and the identification–categorization relationship. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115 (1), 39–57. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ravid, D.
(2003) A developmental perspective on root perception in Hebrew and Palestinian Arabic. Language Acquisition and Language Disorders, 28 1, 293–320. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2006) Word-level morphology: A psycholinguistic perspective on linear formation in Hebrew nominals. Morphology, 16 (1), 127–148. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rosch, E., Mervis, C. B., Gray, W. D., Johnson, D. M., & Boyes-Braem, P.
(1976) Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 8 (3), 382–439. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rosenhouse, J.
(2002) Personal names in Hebrew and Arabic: Modern trends compared to the past. Journal of Semitic Studies, 47 (1), 97–114. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Share, D. L.
(1999) Phonological recoding and orthographic learning: A direct test of the self-teaching hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 72 (2), 95–129. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Share, D. L., & Shalev, C.
(2004) Self-teaching in normal and disabled readers. Reading and Writing, 17 (7–8), 769–800. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Shatil, N.
(2006) The Hebrew noun system: A structural cognitive perspective. Lĕšonénu: A Journal for the Study of the Hebrew Language and Cognate Subjects, 68 1, 243–282.Google Scholar
Shimron, J.
(2003) Semitic languages: Are they really root-based?. Language Acquisition and Language Disorders, 28 1, 1–28. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sloat, C.
(1969) Proper nouns in English. Language, 45 (1), 26–30. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Slobin, D. I.
(2005) Linguistic representations of motion events: What is signifier and what is signified. In C. Maeder, O. Fischer, & W. Herlofsky (Eds.), Iconicity inside out: Iconicity in language and literature (pp. 307–322). John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Slobin, Dan. I.
(2006) What makes manner of motion salient. Explorations in linguistic typology, discourse, and cognition. In M. H. S. Robert (Ed.), Space in languages: Linguistic systems and cognitive categories. John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Smith, L. B., Jones, S. S., & Landau, B.
(1996) Naming in young children: A dumb attentional mechanism?. Cognition, 60 (2), 143–171. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stošić, D.
(2013) Manner of motion, evaluative and pluractional morphology. Space in South Slavic. Oslo Studies in Language, 5 (1), 61–89.Google Scholar
Taft, M., & Forster, K. I.
(1975) Lexical storage and retrieval of prefixed words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14 1, 638–647. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Talmy, L.
(1991) Path to realization: a typology of event integration. Buffalo Papers in Linguistics, 91 (1), 147–187.Google Scholar
Tamminen, J., Davis, M. H., Merkx, M., & Rastle, K.
(2012) The role of memory consolidation in generalisation of new linguistic information. Cognition, 125 (1), 107–112. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tamminen, J., Davis, M. H., & Rastle, K.
(2015) From specific examples to general knowledge in language learning. Cognitive Psychology, 79 1, 1–39. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Akhtar, N.
(1995) Two-year-olds use pragmatic cues to differentiate reference to objects and actions. Cognitive Development, 10 (2), 201–224. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Waxman, S. R., & Gelman, S. A.
(2009) Early word-learning entails reference, not merely associations. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13 (6), 258–263. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Xu, F., & Tenenbaum, J. B.
(2007) Word learning as Bayesian inference. Psychological Review, 114 (2), 245. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Yablonski, M., Ben-Shachar, M.
(2018) The morpheme interference effect in Hebrew: A generalization across the verbal and domains. The Mental Lexicon, 11 (2),277–307. CrossrefGoogle Scholar