Article published In:
Pragmatics & Cognition
Vol. 24:2 (2017) ► pp.297313
References
Barner, David, Neon Brooks & Alan Bale
2011Accessing the unsaid: The role of scalar alternatives in children’s pragmatic inference. Cognition 118(1). 84–93. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bott, Lewis & Ira A. Noveck
2004Some utterances are underinformative: The onset and time course of scalar inferences. Journal of Memory and Language 51(3). 437–457. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, Eve V.
1990On the pragmatics of contrast. Journal of Child Language 17(2). 417–431. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Davies, Catherine & Napoleon Katsos
2010Over-informative children: Production/comprehension asymmetry or tolerance to pragmatic violations? Lingua 120(8). 1956–1972. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Feeney, Aidan, Susan Scrafton, Amber Duckworth & Simon J. Handley
2004The story of some: Everyday pragmatic inference by children and adults. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology 58(2). 121. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Foppolo, Francesca, Maria Teresa Guasti & Gennaro Chierchia
2012Scalar implicatures in child language: Give children a chance. Language Learning and Development 8(4). 365–394. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Geurts, Bart
2010Quantity implicature. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Guasti, Maria Teresa, Gennaro Chierchia, Stephen Crain, Francesca Foppolo, Andrea Gualmini & Luisa Meroni
2005Why children and adults sometimes (but not always) compute implicatures. Language and Cognitive Processes 20(5). 667–696. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grice, Herbert Paul
1975Logic and conversation. In Peter Cole & Jerry L. Morgan (eds.), Syntax and semantics, vol. 31, 41–58. New York: Academic Press (Reprinted in Grice, Herbert Paul 1989 Studies in the way of words. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press).Google Scholar
Huang, Yi Ting & Jesse Snedeker
2009Online interpretation of scalar quantifiers: Insight into the semantics-pragmatics interface. Cognitive Psychology 58(3). 376–415. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Katsos, Napoleon
2014Scalar implicature. In Danielle Matthews (ed), Pragmatic development in first language acquisition, 183–198. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Katsos, Napoleon & Dorothy V. Bishop
2008Pragmatic Tolerance. Paper presented at the XI International Congress for the Study of Child Language (IASCL) , Edinburgh, UK, 28 July–1 August.
2011Pragmatic tolerance: Implications for the acquisition of informativeness and implicature. Cognition 120(1). 67–81. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Katsos, Napoleon & Nafsika Smith
2010Pragmatic Tolerance and speaker-comprehender asymmetries. In Katie Franich, Kate M. Iserman & Lauren L. Keil (eds.), Proceedings of the 34th Boston University Conference in Language Development, 221–232. Cascadilla Press, MA, USA.Google Scholar
Marchena, Ashley de, Inge-Marie Eigsti, Amanda Worek, Kim Emiko Ono & Jesse Snedeker
2011Mutual exclusivity in autism spectrum disorders: Testing the pragmatic hypothesis. Cognition 119(1). 96–113. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Neys, Wim De & Walter Schaeken
2007When people are more logical under cognitive load: Dual task impact on scalar implicature. Experimental Psychology 54(2). 128–133. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Noveck, Ira A.
2001When children are more logical than adults: Experimental investigations of scalar implicature. Cognition 78(2). 165–188. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Noveck, Ira A. & Andres Posada
2003Characterizing the time course of an implicature: An evoked potentials study. Brain and Language 85(2). 203–210. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Noveck, Ira A. & Anne Reboul
2008Experimental pragmatics: A Gricean turn in the study of language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 12(11). 425–431. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Papafragou, Anna & Julien Musolino
2003Scalar implicatures: Experiments at the semantics-pragmatics interface. Cognition 86(3). 253–282. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Papafragou, Anna & Niki Tantalou
2004Children’s computation of implicatures. Language Acquisition 12(1). 71–82. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pouscoulous, Nausicaa, Ira A. Noveck, Guy Politzer & Anne Bastide
2007A developmental investigation of processing costs in implicature production. Language Acquisition 14(4). 347–375. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Skordos, Dimitrios & Anna Papafragou
2016Children’s derivation of scalar implicatures: Alternatives and relevance. Cognition 1531. 6–18. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 5 other publications

Feng, Shuo
2024. L2 tolerance of pragmatic violations of informativeness. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 14:2  pp. 147 ff. DOI logo
Sikos, Les, Minjae Kim & Daniel J. Grodner
2019. Social Context Modulates Tolerance for Pragmatic Violations in Binary but Not Graded Judgments. Frontiers in Psychology 10 DOI logo
Syrett, Kristen, Jennifer Austin & Liliana Sanchez
2021. Establishing upper bounds in English monolingual and Heritage Spanish-English bilingual language development. Language Acquisition 28:1  pp. 39 ff. DOI logo
Wang, Shuyan
2023. Effects of Processing Limits on Computing Scalar Implicatures: Evidence from Child English and Child Mandarin. Language Learning and Development 19:3  pp. 345 ff. DOI logo
Zhang, Jun & Yan Wu
2023. Epistemic reasoning in pragmatic inferencing by non-native speakers: The case of scalar implicatures. Second Language Research 39:3  pp. 697 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 12 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.