When spatializing time, individuals can either say that they walk towards the future as though time is a
stationary landscape (called ego-moving perspective), or that the future time moves towards them (called time-moving perspective).
A substantial body of experimental research has shown that people’s adoption of these two temporal perspectives may be malleable,
influenced by a broad set of factors. In the current research, we examined the novel possibility that the mere crowdedness of the
environment can influence people’s abstract thinking about time. We contended that exposure to the crowd may be linked to
increased anxiety, which can in turn lead to a greater preference for the time-moving perspective in the resolution of temporal
ambiguity. Two experiments found that social crowding, whether induced via a visualization task or through an assignment to a
crowded workstation, was sufficient to alter participants’ perspectives on the movement of events in time. Further, we found that
anxiety mediated the relationship between crowdedness and temporal reasoning. Taken together, these results offer unique insights
into the cognitive consequences of social crowding and provide a more complete understanding of how people adduce temporal
relationship.
(2022) Time
as space vs. time as quantity in Spanish: a co-speech gesture study. Language and
Cognition,
14
(1), 1–18.
Bargh, J. A., & Melnikoff, D.
(2019) Does
physical warmth prime social warmth?. Social
Psychology,
50
(3), 207–210.
Baum, A., & Koman, S.
(1976) Differential
response to anticipated crowding: Psychological effects of social and spatial density. Journal
of Personality and Social
Psychology,
34
(3), 526.
Baxter, J. C., & Deanovich, B. F.
(1970) Anxiety
arousing effects of inappropriate crowding. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology,
35
(2), 17–178.
Bender, A., & Beller, S.
(2014) Mapping
spatial frames of reference onto time: A review of theoretical accounts and empirical
findings. Cognition,
132
(3), 342–382.
Boroditsky, L.
(2018) Language
and the construction of time through space. Trends in
Neurosciences,
41
(10), 651–653.
Boroditsky, L., & Ramscar, M.
(2002) The
roles of body and mind in abstract thought. Psychological
Science,
13
(2), 185–189.
Cacioppo, J. T., Gardner, W. L., & Berntson, G. G.
(1999) The
affect system has parallel and integrative processing components: Form follows
function. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology,
76
(5), 839–855.
Chabris, C., Heck, P. R., Mandart, J., Benjamin, D., & Simons, D.
(2019) No evidence that experiencing psychical warmth promotes interpersonal warmth: Two failures to replicate Willams & Bargh (2008). Social Psychology,
50
1, 127–132.
Clark, H. H.
(1973) Space,
time, semantics, and the child. In T. E. Moore (Ed.), Cognitive
development and the acquisition of
language (pp. 27–63). New York: Academic Press.
Consiglio, I., De
Angelis, M., & Costabile, M.
(2018) The
effect of social density on word of mouth. Journal of Consumer
Research,
45
(3), 511–528.
Cooperrider, K., Slotta, J., & Núñez, R.
(2022) The
ups and downs of space and time: Topography in Yupno language, culture, and cognition. Language
and
Cognition,
14
(1), 131–159.
Duffy, S. E., & Feist, M. I.
(2014) Individual
differences in the interpretation of ambiguous statements about time. Cognitive
Linguistics,
25
(1), 29–54.
Duffy, S. E., Feist, M. I., & McCarthy, S.
(2014) Moving
through time: The role of personality in three real-life contexts. Cognitive
Science,
38
(8), 1662–1674.
Duffy, S. E., & Feist, M. I.
(2017) Power in time: The influence of power posing on metaphoric perspectives on time. Language and Cognition, 9(4), 637–647.
Elliot, A. J.
(1999) Approach
and avoidance motivation and achievement goals. Educational
Psychologist,
34
(3), 149–169.
Elliot, A. J., & Thrash, T. M.
(2002) Approach-avoidance
motivation in personality: Approach and avoidance temperaments and goals. Journal of
Personality and Social
Psychology,
82
(5), 804–818.
Eroglu, S. A., Machleit, K., & Barr, T. F.
(2005) Perceived
retail crowding and shopping satisfaction: the role of shopping values. Journal of Business
Research,
58
(8), 1146–1153.
Evans, G. W., & Lepore, S. J.
(1992) Conceptual
and analytic issues in crowding research. Journal of Environmental
Psychology,
12
(2), 163–173.
Evans, G. W., & Wener, R. E.
(2007) Crowding
and personal space invasion on the train: Please don’t make me sit in the middle. Journal of
Environmental
Psychology,
27
(1), 90–94.
Evans, G. W., Ricciuti, H. N., Hope, S., Schoon, I., Bradley, R. H., Corwyn, R. F., & Hazan, C.
(2010) Crowding
and cognitive development: The mediating role of maternal responsiveness among 36-month-old
children. Environment and
Behavior,
42
(1), 135–148.
Gelbrich, K., & Sattler, B.
(2014) Anxiety,
crowding, and time pressure in public self-service technology acceptance. Journal of Services
Marketing28
(1), 82–94.
Glick, P., Demorest, J. A., & Hotze, C. A.
(1988) Keeping
your distance: group membership, personal space, and requests for small favors. Journal of
Applied Social
Psychology,
18
(4), 315–330.
Griffit, W., & Veitch, R.
(1971) Hot
and crowded: influence of population density and temperature on interpersonal affective
behavior. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology,
17
(1), 92–98.
Hall, E. T.
(1966) The
hidden dimension (Vol. 6091). Garden City, NY: Doubleday.
Harmon-Jones, E.
(2003) Anger
and the behavioral approach system. Personality and Individual
Differences,
35
(5), 995–1005.
Harmon-Jones, E.
(2019) On
motivational influences, moving beyond valence, and integrating dimensional and discrete views of
emotion. Cognition and
Emotion,
33
(1), 101–108.
Hauser, D. J., Carter, M. S., & Meier, B. P.
(2009) Mellow
Monday and furious Friday: The approach-related link between anger and time
representation. Cognition and
Emotion,
23
(6), 1166–1180.
Huang, X., Huang, Z., & Wyer Jr, R. S.
(2018) The
influence of social crowding on brand attachment. Journal of Consumer
Research,
44
(5), 1068–1084.
Hediger, H.
(1955) Studies
of the psychology and behavior of captive animals in zoos and circuses. Oxford, UK: Criterion Books.
Higgins, E. T.
(1997) Beyond
pleasure and pain. American
Psychologist,
52
(12), 1280–1300.
Hui, M. K., & Bateson, J. E.
(1991) Perceived
control and the effects of crowding and consumer choice on the service experience. Journal of
Consumer
Research,
18
(2), 174–184.
Hou, Y., Zhang, K., & Li, G.
(2021) Service
robots or human staff: How social crowding shapes tourist preferences. Tourism
Management,
83
1, 104242.
James, W.
(1890) The
principles of
psychology (vol. 21). NY: Henry Holt & Co.
Kennedy, D. P., Gläscher, J., Tyszka, J. M., & Adolphs, R.
(2009) Personal
space regulation by the human amygdala. Nature
Neuroscience,
12
(10), 1226–1227.
(1980) Metaphors
we live by. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Lang, P.
(1995) Studies
of motivation and attention. American
Psychologist,
50
(5), 372–385.
Lee, A., & Ji, L. J.
(2014) Moving
away from a bad past and toward a good future: Feelings influence the metaphorical understanding of
time. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
General,
143
(1), 21–26.
Li, H.
(2020) Will
it really happen? Disambiguating of the hypothetical and real “Next Wednesday’s meeting” question in Mandarin
speakers. Lingua,
237
1, 102806.
Li, H.
(2021) Time
heals all wounds: analysis of changes in temporal focus and implicit space–time mappings among survivors of the 2019 China
earthquake over time. Language and
Cognition,
13
(4), 595–612.
Li, H., & Cao, Y.
(2021) Move
with the flow: Metaphorical perspectives on time in Chinese Taoists and atheists. The
International Journal for the Psychology of
Religion,
31
(4), 225–234.
Lu, J. G., Lee, J. J., Gino, F., & Galinsky, A. D.
(2018) Polluted
morality: Air pollution predicts criminal activity and unethical behavior. Psychological
Science,
29
(3), 340–355.
Maeng, A., Tanner, R. J., & Soman, D.
(2013) Conservative
when crowded: Social crowding and consumer choice. Journal of Marketing
Research,
50
(6), 739–752.
Margolies, S. O., & Crawford, L. E.
(2008) Event
valence and spatial metaphors of time. Cognition and
Emotion,
22
(7), 1401–1414.
Marteau, T. M., & Bekker, H.
(1992) The
development of a six-item short-form of the state scale of the Spielberger State – Trait Anxiety Inventory
(STAI). British Journal of Clinical
Psychology,
31
(3), 301–306.
Matlock, T., Ramscar, M., & Boroditsky, L.
(2005) On
the experiential link between spatial and temporal language. Cognitive
Science,
29
(4), 655–664.
Maxwell, S. E., Lau, M. Y., & Howard, G. S.
(2015) Is
psychology suffering from a replication crisis? What does “failure to replicate” really
mean?. American
Psychologist,
70
(6), 487–498.
McGlone, M. S., & Harding, J. L.
(1998) Back
(or forward?) to the future: The role of perspective in temporal language
comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and
Cognition,
24
(5), 1211–1223.
Medimorec, S.
(2022) The
effects of day of the week on temporal ambiguity resolution. Psychological
Reports,
125
(2), 882–889.
Neuberg, S. L., Kenrick, D. T., & Schaller, M.
(2011) Human
threat management systems: Self-protection and disease avoidance. Neuroscience &
Biobehavioral
Reviews,
35
(4), 1042–1051.
Núñez, R., & Cooperrider, K.
(2013) The
tangle of space and time in human cognition. Trends in Cognitive
Sciences,
17
(5), 220–229.
Núñez, R. E., Motz, B. A., & Teuscher, U.
(2006) Time
after time: The psychological reality of the ego-and time-reference-point distinction in metaphorical construals of
time. Metaphor and
Symbol,
21
(3), 133–146.
Novelli, D., Drury, J., Reicher, S., & Stott, C.
(2013) Crowdedness
mediates the effect of social identification on positive emotion in a crowd: A survey of two crowd
events. PloS
One,
8
(11), e78983.
Oldham, G. R., & Fried, Y.
(1987) Employee
reactions to workspace characteristics. Journal of Applied
Psychology,
72
(1), 75–80.
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F.
(2004) SPSS
and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior
Research Methods, Instruments, &
Computers,
36
(4), 717–731.
Pashler, H., & Wagenmakers, E. J.
(2012) Editors’
introduction to the special section on replicability in psychological science: A crisis of
confidence?. Perspectives on Psychological
Science,
7
(6), 528–530.
Richmond, J., Clare
Wilson, J., & Zinken, J.
(2012) A
feeling for the future: How does agency in time metaphors relate to feelings?. European Journal
of Social
Psychology,
42
(7), 813–823.
Rodríguez, L.
(2022) Dickens
in Chol. Language and
Cognition,
14
(2), 303–331.
Schaeffer, G. H., & Patterson, M. L.
(1980) Intimacy,
arousal, and small group crowding. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology,
38
(2), 283–290.
Simmons, J. P., Nelson, L. D., & Simonsohn, U.
(2011) False-positive
psychology: Undisclosed flexibility in data collection and analysis allows presenting anything as
significant. Psychological
Science,
22
(11), 1359–1366.
Shen, S., & Li, H.
(2021) It’s
owl time!: the relationship between chronotype and resolution of temporal
ambiguity. Chronobiology
International,
38
(8), 1103–1108.
Sinha, C., Sinha, V. D. S., Zinken, J., & Sampaio, W.
(2011) When
time is not space: The social and linguistic construction of time intervals and temporal event relations in an Amazonian
culture. Language and
Cognition,
3
(1), 137–169.
Smith, S., & Haythorn, W. W.
(1972) Effects
of compatibility, crowding, group size, and leadership seniority on stress, anxiety, hostility, and annoyance in isolated
groups. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology,
22
(1), 67–79.
Stickles, E., & Lewis, T. N.
(2018) Wednesday’s
meeting really is on Friday: A meta-analysis and evaluation of ambiguous spatiotemporal
language. Cognitive
Science,
42
(3), 1015–1025.
Stocker, K., & Hartmann, M.
(2019) “Next
Wednesday’s meeting has been moved forward two days”: The time-perspective question is ambiguous in Swiss German, but not in
standard German. Swiss Journal of
Psychology,
78
(1–2), 61–67.
Vassilopoulos, S. P.
(2005) Social
anxiety and the vigilance-avoidance pattern of attentional processing. Behavioural and
Cognitive
Psychotherapy,
33
(1), 13–24.
(2020) Can
co-speech gestures alone carry the mental time line?. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Learning, Memory, and
Cognition,
46
(9), 1768–1781.
Worchel, S., & Teddie, C.
(1976) The
experience of crowding: A two-factor theory. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology,
34
(1), 30–40.
Xu, J., Shen, H., & Wyer Jr, R. S.
(2012) Does
the distance between us matter? Influences of physical proximity to others on consumer
choice. Journal of Consumer
Psychology,
22
(3), 418–423.
Zheng, W., Liu, Y., Liu, C. H., Chen, Y. H., Cui, Q., & Fu, X.
(2019) The
influence of event valence and emotional states on the metaphorical comprehension of
time. Frontiers in
Psychology,
10
1, 410.