Chapter published in:
Growing Old with Two Languages: Effects of Bilingualism on Cognitive Aging
Edited by Ellen Bialystok and Margot D. Sullivan
[Studies in Bilingualism 53] 2017
► pp. 161183
References

References

Adesope, O. O., Lavin, T., Thompson, T., & Ungerleider, C.
(2010) A systematic review and meta-analysis of the cognitive correlates of bilingualism. Review of Educational Research, 80 (2), 207–245. Crossref link
Bialystok, E.
(2015) Bilingualism and the development of executive function: The role of attention. Child Development Perspectives, 9 (2), 117–121. Crossref link
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Green, D. W., & Gollan, T. H.
(2009) Bilingual minds. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 10, 89–129. Crossref link
Bialystok, E., Craik, F., & Luk, G.
(2008) Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34 (4), 859–873. Crossref link
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M.
(2004) Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19(2), 290–303. Crossref link
Bialystok, E., & Feng, X.
(2009) Language proficiency and executive control in proactive interference: Evidence from monolingual and bilingual children and adults. Brain & Language, 109, 93–100. Crossref link
Bialystok, E., & Luk, G.
(2012) Receptive vocabulary differences in monolingual and bilingual adults. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 397–401. Crossref link
Bialystok, E., Luk, G., Peets, K. F., & Yang, S.
(2010) Receptive vocabulary differences in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13 (4), 525–531. Crossref link
Bialystok, E., Poarch, G. J., Luo, L., & Craik, F. I. M.
(2014) Effects of bilingualism and aging on executive function and working memory. Psychology and Aging, 29 (3), 696–705. Crossref link.
Das-Smaal, E., de Jong, P. F., & Koopmans, J. R.
(1993) Working memory, attentional regulation and the star counting test. Personality and Individual Differences, 14(6), 815–824. Crossref link
Eriksson, J., Vogel, E. K., Lansner, A., Bergström, F., & Nyberg, L.
(2015) Neurocognitive architecture of working memory. Neuron, 88 (1), 33–46. Crossref link
Fernandes, M. A., Craik, F. I. M., Bialystok, E., & Kreuger, S.
(2007) Effects of bilingualism, aging, and semantic relatedness on memory under divided attention. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 6 (2), 128–141. Crossref link
Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., Cera, C. M., & Sandoval, T. C.
(2008) More use almost always means smaller a frequency effect: Aging, bilingualism, and the weaker links hypothesis. Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 787–814. Crossref link
Gollan, T. H., & Silverberg, N. B.
(2001) Tip-of-the-tongue states in Hebrew-English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 4, 63–83. Crossref link
Grundy, J. G., & Timmer, K.
(in press). Bilingualism and working memory capacity: A comprehensive meta-analysis. Second Language Research. Crossref link
Hasher, L., Zacks, R. T., & May, C. P.
(1999) Inhibitory control, circadian arousal, and age. In D. Gopher and A. Koriat (Eds.), Attention and performance XVII: Cognitive regulation of performance: Interaction of theory and application. Attention and performance (pp. 653–675). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Ivanova, I., & Costa, A.
(2008) Does bilingualism hamper lexical access in speech production? Acta Psychologica, 127, 277–288. Crossref link
Jacoby. L. L.
(1991) A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 30 (5), 513–541. Crossref link
Jonides, J., Marshuetz, C., Smith, E. E., Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., Koeppe, R. A., & Hartley, A.
(2000) Age differences in behavior and PET activation reveal differences in interference resolution in verbal working memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12 (1), 188–196. Crossref link
Jonides, J., & Nee, D. E.
(2006) Brain mechanisms of proactive interference in working memory. Neuroscience, 139, 181–193. Crossref link
Kroll, J. F., Dussias, P. E., Bogulski, C. A., & Valdes-Kroff, J.
(2012) Juggling two languages in one mind: What bilinguals tell us about language processing and its consequences for cognition. In B. Ross (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation (Vol. 56, pp. 229–262). San Diego: Academic Press.
Linck, J. A., Osthus, P., Koeth, J. T., & Bunting, M. F.
(2014) Working memory and second language comprehension: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21, 861–883. Crossref link
Lu, C. H., & Proctor, R. W.
(1995) The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 174–207. Crossref link
Luk, G. & Bialystok, E.
(2013) Bilingualism is not a categorical variable: Interaction between language proficiency and usage. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 605–621. Crossref link
Luo, L., Craik, F. I. M., Moreno, S., & Bialystok, E.
(2013) Bilingualism interacts with domain in a working memory task: Evidence from aging. Psychology and Aging, 28(1), 28–34. Crossref link
May, C., Hasher, L., & Kane, M. J.
(1999) The role of interference in memory span. Memory & Cognition, 27, 759–767. Crossref link
Oberauer, K., Süss, H. M., Schulze, R., Wilhelm, O., & Wittman, W. W.
(2000) Working memory capacity- facets of a cognitive ability construct. Personality and Individual Differences, 29, 1017–1045. Crossref link
Shipley, W. C., Gruber, C. P., Martin, T. A., & Klein, A. M.
(2009) Shipley-2 Manual. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.
van Merrienboer, J., & Sweller, J.
(2005) Cognitive load theory and complex learning: Recent developments and future directions. Educational Psychology Review, 17, 147–177. Crossref link
Wodniecka, Z., Craik, F. I. M., Luo, L., & Bialystok, E.
(2010) Does bilingualism help memory? Competing effects of verbal ability and executive control. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 13 (5), 575–595. Crossref link