References (55)
References
Aksu-Koç, A., Ketrez, F.N., Laalo, K. & Pfeiler, B. 2007. Agglutinating languages: Turkish, Finnish, and Yucatec Maya. In Typological Perspectives on the Acquisition of Noun and Verb Morphology [Antwerp Papers in Linguistics 112], S. Laaha & S. Gillis (eds), 47–59. Antwerp: Universiteit Antwerpen.Google Scholar
Bergqvist, H. 2008. Temporal reference in Lakandon-Maya: Speaker and event perspectives. PhD dissertation, University of London.Google Scholar
Bohnemeyer, J. 2002. Activity nouns, unaccusativity, and argument marking in Yukatekan. Paper presented at the SSILA meeting; Special Session: Denominal Verbs in the Languages of the Americas; LSA; San Francisco; January 4.
. 2004. Split intransitivity, linking, and lexical representation: The case of Yukatek Maya. Linguistics 42(1): 67–107. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bowerman, M. 1994. From universal to language-specific in early grammatical development. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 346: 34–45. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bricker, V., Po’ot Yah, E. & Dzul de Po’ot, O. 1998. A Dictionary of the Maya Language as Spoken in Hocabá, Yucatán. Salt Lake City UT: University of Utah Press.Google Scholar
Brown, P. 1998. Children's first verbs in Tzeltal: Evidence for an early verb category. Linguistics 36(4): 713–53. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brown, P., Pfeiler, B., De León, L. & Pye C. 2013. The acquisition of agreement in four Mayan languages. In The Acquisition of Ergativity [Trends in Language Acquisition 9], E. Bavin & S. Stoll (eds), 271–306. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Choi, S. 1998. Verbs in early lexical and syntactic development in Korean. Linguistics 36: 755–780. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Choi, S. & Gopnik, A. 1995. Early acquisition of verbs in Korean: A crosslinguistic study. Journal of Child Language 22(3): 497–529. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, E.V. 1993. The Lexicon in Acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De León, L. 1998. Raíces verbales tempranas en tzotzil: Input materno vs. restricciones cognoscitivas. Función 18: 147–174.Google Scholar
. 1999a. Verb roots and caregiver speech in early Tzotzil acquisition. In Cognition and Function in Language, B.A. Fox, D. Jurafsky & L.A. Michaelis (eds), 99–119. Stanford CA: CSLI.Google Scholar
. 1999b. Verb roots in Tzotzil early syntactic development. International Journal of Bilingualism 3: 219–240. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2001. Finding the richest path: Language and cognition in the acquisition of verticality in Tzotzil (Mayan). In Language Acquisition and Conceptual Development, M. Bowerman & S.C. Levinson (eds), 544–565. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2007. A preliminary view at Ch’ol (Mayan) early lexicon: The role of language and cultural context. In Learning Indigenous Languages: Child language Acquisition in Mesoamerica, B. Pfeiler (ed), 85–103. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dressler, W.U. 2004. Degrees of grammatical productivity in inflectional morphology. Italian Journal of Linguistics 15: 31–62.Google Scholar
Dromi, E. 2009. Old data - New eyes. Theories on word meaning acquisition. In Routes to Language: Studies in Honor of Melissa Bowerman, V. Gathercole (ed), 39–63. New York NY.: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Gelman, S.A. & Tardif, T. 1998. Acquisition of nouns and verbs in Mandarin and English. In Proceedings of the 29th Annual Child Language Research Forum 1997, E.V. Clark (ed), 27–36. Stanford CA: CSLI.Google Scholar
Gentner, D. 1982. Why nouns are learned before verbs: Linguistic relativity versus natural partitioning. In Language Development: Language, Thought and Culture, S.A. Kuczaj (ed), 301–334. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Gentner, D. & Boroditsky, L. 2001. Individuation, relativity, and early word learning. In Language Acquisition and Conceptual Development, M. Bowerman & S. Levinson (eds), 215–256. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2009. Early acquisition of nouns and verbs: Evidence from Navajo. In Routes to Language: Studies in Honor of Melissa Bowerman, V.C. Gathercole (ed), 5–36. New York NY: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Goldfield, B.A. 1993. Why nouns before verbs? A view from pragmatics. In BUCLD 22: Proceedings of the 22nd Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, A. Greenhill, M. Hughes, H. Littlefield & H. Walsh (eds), 281–292. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Gómez López, P. 2007. Acquisition of referential and relational words in Huichol: From 16 to 24 months of age. In Learning Indigenous Languages: Child Language Acquisition in Mesoamerica, B. Pfeiler (ed), 103–119. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, A. & Choi, S. 1995. Names, relational words, and cognitive development in English and Korean speakers: Nouns are not always learned before verbs. In Beyond Names for Things: Young Children’s Acquisition of Verbs, M. Tomasello & W.E. Merriman (eds), 63–80. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Haryu, E., Imai, M., Okada, H., Li, L., Meyer, M.A., Hirsh-Pasek, K. & Golinkoff, R. Michnick. 2005. Noun bias in Chinese children: Novel noun and verb learning in Chinese, Japanese and English preschoolers. In BUCLD 29: Proceedings of the 29th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, A. Brugos, M. Clark-Cotton & S. Ha (eds), 272–283. Somerville MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Hopper, P.J. & Thompson, S.A. 1984. The discourse basis for lexical categories in universal grammar. Language 60(4): 703–752. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaufman, Terrence. 1990. Algunos rasgos estructurales de los idiomas Mayances con Referencia Especial al Kíiche. In Lecturas sobre la Linguistica Maya, N.C. England & S.R. Elliot (eds), 59–116. Wellfleet MA: Plumsock Mesoamerican Studies.Google Scholar
Kaufman, T. 1991. Notes on the structure of Yukateko and other Yukatekan languages (Nov-Dec 1991), PDLMA Seminar handout.Google Scholar
Kovačević, M., Blaha Pfeiler, B. & Palmović, M. 2008. Red thread of Croatian and Yukatek (Maya): What could be common in the two languages? Península II( 2, otoño de 2007): 33–50.Google Scholar
Küntay, A.C. & Slobin, D.I. 1999. Discourse behavior of lexical categories in Turkish child-directed speech: Nouns vs. verbs. Paper presented at the VIIIth IASCL Congress , San Sebastian-Donostia, July 14.
Lois, X. & Vapnarsky, V. 2003. Polyvalence of Root Classes in Yukatekan Mayan Languages. Munich: Lincom.Google Scholar
. 2006. Lexical Categories and Root Classes in Amerinidian languages. Frankfurt: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Lois, X., Vapnarsky, V., Becquey, C. & Monod Becquelin, A. 2010. Polycategoriality across Mayan languages: Action nouns and ergative splits. Ms, August 2010.Google Scholar
Mcnamara, J. 1982. Names for things: A Study of Human Learning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Parisse, C. & Le Normand, M.T. 2000. How children build their morphosyntax: The case of French. Journal of Child Language 27: 267–292. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pfeiler, B. (2002). Noun and verb acquisition in Yucatec Maya. In Pre- and Protomorphology: Early Phases of Morphological Development in Nouns and Verbs, M.D. Voeikova & W.U. Dressler (eds), 75–83. Munich: Lincom.Google Scholar
. 2003. Early acquisition of the verbal complex in Yucatec Maya. In Development of Verb Inflection in First Language Acquistion. A Cross–Linguistic Perspective, D. Bittner, W.U. Dressler & M. Kilani–Schoch (eds), 379–401. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2006. Polyvalence in the acquisition of early lexicon in Yucatec Maya. In Lexical Categories and Root Classes in Amerinidian Languages, X. Lois & V. Vapnarsky (eds), 319–341. Bern: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
. 2007. "Lo oye, lo repite y lo piensa". The contribution of prompting to the socialization and language acquisition in Yukatek Maya toddler. In Learning Indigenous Languages: Child Language Acquisition in Mesoamerica, B. Pfeiler (ed), 183–202. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pfeiler, B & Martín Briceño, E. 1997. Early verb inflection in Yucatec Maya. Papers and Studies in Contrastive Linguistics 33: 117–125.Google Scholar
Pfeiler, B. & Martín Briceño, E. 1998. La adquisición de la transitividad en el maya yucateco. Función 18: 97–120.Google Scholar
Pine, J., Lieven, E. & Rowland, C. 1997. Stylistic variation at the ‘Single-word’ stage: Relations between maternal speech characteristics and children’s vocabulary composition and usage. Child Development 68: 807–819. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pye, C. 1983. Mayan telegraphese: Intonational determinants of inflectional development in Quiché Mayan. Language 59: 583–604. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 1992. The acquisition of K’iche’. In The Crosslinguistic Study of Language Acquisition, Vol. 3, D. Slobin (ed), 221–308. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Pye, C., Pfeiler, B., de León, L., Brown, P. & Mateo P. 2007. Roots or edges? Explaining variation in children’s early verb forms across five Mayan languages. In Learning Indigenous Languages: Child Language Acquisition in Mesoamerica, B. Pfeiler (ed), 15–47. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pye, C., Pfeiler, B. & Mateo, P. 2013. The acquisition of extended ergativity in Mam, Q’anjob’al and Yucatec. In The Acquisition of Ergativity [Trends in Language Acquisition Research 9], E. Bavin & S. Stoll (eds), 307–335. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smiley, P. & Huttenlocher, J. 1995. Conceptual development and the child’s early words for events, objects and persons. In Beyond Names for Things: Young Children’s Acquisition of Verbs, M. Tomasello & W. E. Merriman (eds), 21–61. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Tardif, T. 1996. Nouns are not always learned before verbs: Evidence from Mandarin speaker’s early vocabularies. Developmental Psychology 32: 492–504. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tardif, T., Shatz, M. & Naigles, L.G. 1997. Caregiver speech and children’s use of nouns versus verbs: A comparison of English, Italian, and Mandarin. Journal of Child Language 24: 535–565. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tardif, T., Gelman, S.A. & Xu, F. 1999. Putting the ‘Noun Bias’ in context: A comparison of English and Mandarin. Child Development 70: 620–635. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. & Olguin, R. 1993. Twenty-three-month-old children have a grammatical category of noun. Cognitive Development 8: 451–464. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., Akhtar, N., Dodson, K. & Rekau, L. 1997. Differential productivity in young children’s use of nouns and verbs. Journal of Child Language 24: 373–387. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vapnarsky, V. & Lois, X. 2009. Morphosyntactic and semantic issues on polycategoriality in Yucatecan Mayan languages. Paper presented at the ALT 8 Conference , Berkeley, July.
Vapnarsky, V. 2013. Is Yucatec Maya an omnipredicative language? Predication, the copula and focus constructions. STUF – Language Typology and Universals 66(1): 40–86. DOI logo