Article published in:
Scientific Study of Literature
Vol. 7:2 (2017) ► pp. 177202
References

References

Baggett, P.
(1979) Structurally equivalent stories in movie and text and the effect of the medium on recall. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 18(3), 333–356. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Batson, C. D., Early, S., & Salvarani, G.
(1997) Perspective taking: Imagining how another feels versus imagining how you would feel. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 751–751. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Booth, W. C.
(1961) The rhetoric of fiction. Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
Bordwell, D.
(1985) Narration in the fiction film. Madison: University of Wisconsin Press.Google Scholar
Bordwell, D., & Thompson, K.
(2016) Film art: An introduction. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Boyd, R. L., & Pennebaker, J. W.
(2015) Did Shakespeare write double falsehood? Identifying an individual’s mental world with text analysis. Psychological Science. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Buck, R.
(1984) The communication of emotion. New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Buhrmester, M., Kwang, T., & Gosling, S. D.
(2011) Amazon’s Mechanical Turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet high quality, data? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(1), 3–5. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, J. M., & Russell, J. A.
(1996) Do facial expressions signal specific emotions? Judging emotion from the face in context. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 2015–218. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
[ p. 200 ]
(1997) Facial expressions in Hollywood’s Portrayal of Emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 164–176. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Coburn, C. E., Penuel, W. R., & Geil, K. E.
(2013) Research-practice partnerships: A strategy for leveraging research for educational improvement in school districts. Retrieved from http://​wtgrantfoundation​.org​/focusareas#research​-practice​-partnerships
Cutting, J. E.
(1987) Rigidity in cinema seen from the front row, side aisle. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13(3), 323–334.Google Scholar
Cutting, J. E., Brunick, K. L., & Candan, A.
(2012) Perceiving event dynamics and parsing Hollywood films. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(6), 1476–1490.Google Scholar
Cutting, J., & Iricinschi, C.
(2015) Re-presentations of space in Hollywood movies: An event-indexing analysis. Cognitive Science, 39, 434–434. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
de Vega, M., & Leon, I., & Diaz, J. M.
(1996) The representation of changing emotions in reading comprehension. Cognition and Emotion, 10, 303–321. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P.
(1992) An argument for basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 169–200. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P., Friesen, W. W., & Ellsworth, P.
(1982) What are the relative contributions of facial behavior and contextual information to the judgment of emotion? In P. Ekman (Ed.), Emotions in the human face (2nd ed, pp. 111–127). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fletcher, C. R., & Bloom, C. P.
(1988) Causal reasoning in the comprehension of simple narrative texts. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 235–244. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gernsbacher, M. A., Hallada, B. M., & Robertson, R. R. W.
(1998) How automatically do readers infer fictional characters’ emotional states? Scientific Studies of Reading, 2(3), 271–300. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gerrig, R. J.
(1993) Experiencing narrative worlds: On the psychological activities of reading. New Haven, CT: Yale.Google Scholar
Gerrig, R. J., & Jacovina, M. E.
(2009) Reader participation in the experience of narrative. In B. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 51, pp. 223–254). New York, NY: Elsevier. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Graesser, A. C., Bowers, C., Olde, B., & Pomeroy, V.
(1999) Who said what? Source memory for narrator and character agents in literary short stories. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91(2), 284–300. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hickenlooper, G. Director & Hell, K.
(Executive Producer) (1994) Some folks call it a sling blade [Motion Picture]. USA: Vanguard Cinema.Google Scholar
Kintsch, W.
(1988) The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: A construction-integration model. Psychological Review, 95(2), 163–182. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Komeda, H., & Kusumi, T.
(2006) The effect of protagonist’s emotional shift on situation model construction. Memory & Cognition, 34(7), 1548–1556. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Larsen, J. T., McGraw, A. P., & Caccioppo, J. T.
(2001) Can people feel happy and sad at the same time? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 684–696. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
[ p. 201 ]
Long, D., & Golding, J.
(1993) Superordinate goal inferences: Are they automatically generated during comprehension? Discourse Processes, 16, 55–74. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Magliano, J. P., Dijkstra, K., & Zwaan, R. A.
(1996) Predictive inferences in movies. Discourse Processes, 22, 199–224. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Magliano, J. P., Miller, J., & Zwaan, R. A.
(2001) Indexing space and time in film understanding. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 15(5), 533–545. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Magliano, J. P., Taylor, H. A., & Kim, H. J.
(2005) When goals collide: Monitoring the goals of multiple characters. Memory & Cognition, 33(8), 1357–1367. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Magliano, J. P., & Zacks, J. M.
(2011) The impact of continuity editing in narrative film on event segmentation. Cognitive Science, 35, 1489–1489. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Miall, D. S.
(1989) Beyond the schema given: Affective comprehension of literary narratives. Cognition & Emotion, 3, 55–78. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mobbs, D., Weiskopf, N., Lau, H. C., Featherstone, E., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D.
(2006) The Kuleshov effect: The influence of contextual framing on emotional attributions. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1, 95–95. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Myers, J. L., & O’Brien, E. J.
(1998) Accessing the discourse representation during reading. Discourse Processes, 26, 131–157. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Özyürek, A., & Trabasso, T.
(1997) Evaluation during the understanding of narratives. Discourse Processes, 23, 305–335. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pennebaker, J. W., Booth, R. J., Boyd, R. L., & Francis, M. E.
(2015) Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count: LIWC 2015. www​.liwc​.net
Pennebaker, J. W., & Seagal, J. D.
(1999) Forming a story. The health benefits of narrative. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 55, 1243–1254. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Prince, S., & Hensley, W. E.
(1992) The Kuleshove effect: Recreating the classic experiment. Cinema Journal, 31, 59–75. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pudovkin, V. I.
(1929/1970) Film technique and film acting. New York, NY: Grove.Google Scholar
(1974) Naturshchik vmesto aktera. Sobranie Sochinenii, 1, 184.Google Scholar
Rabinowitz, P.
(1987) Before reading: Narrative conventions and the politics of interpretation. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Rapp, D. N., Komeda, H., & Hinze, S. R.
(2011) Vivifications literary investigation. Scientific Study of Literature, 1(1), 123–135. Crossref.Google Scholar
Reichle, E. D., Pollatsek, A., Fisher, D. L., Rayner, K.
(1998) Toward a model of eye movement control in reading. Psychological Review. 105, 125–57. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Russell, J. A., & Carroll, J. M.
(1999) On the bipolarity of positive and negative emotions. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 3–30. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Suh, S., & Trabasso, T.
(1993) Inferences during reading: Converging evidence from discourse analysis, talk-aloud protocols and recognition priming. Journal of Memory & Language, 32, 279–300. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tan, E. S.
(1996) Emotion and the structure of narrative film: Film as an emotion machine. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Thornton, B. B. Director & Meistrich, L.
(Executive Producer) (1996) Sling blade [Motion Picture]. USA: Miramax.Google Scholar
[ p. 202 ]
Tompkins, S. S.
(1962) Affect, imagery, consciousness (Vol. 1). New York, NY: Springer.Google Scholar
Tomkins, S. S., & McCarter, R.
(1964) What and where are the primary affects: Some evidence for a theory. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 18, 119–158. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Trabasso, T., van den Broek, P., & Suh, S.
(1989) Logical necessity and transitivity of causal relations in stories. Discourse Processes, 12, 1–25. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tversky, B., & Hard, B. M.
(2009) Embodied and disembodied cognition: Spatial perspective-taking. Cognition, 110, 124–124. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wallbott, H.
(1988) In and out of context: Influences of facial expression and context information on emotion attributions. British Journal of Social Psychology, 27, 327–369. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Zacks, J. M., Speer, N. K., & Reynolds, J. R.
(2009) Segmentation in reading and film comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138(2), 307–327. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Zillmann, D.
(1995) Mechanisms of emotional involvement with drama. Poetics, 23, 33–51. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Zillmann, D., & Cantor, J. R.
(1977) Affective responses to the emotions of the protagonist. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13(2), 155–165. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Zwickel, J.
(2009) Agency attribution and visuospatial perspective taking. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(6), 1089–1093. CrossrefGoogle Scholar