Article published in:
Pragmatic Development in First Language Acquisition
Edited by Danielle Matthews
[Trends in Language Acquisition Research 10] 2014
► pp. 112
References
Allen, S.E.M.
(2000) A discourse-pragmatic explanation for argument representation in child Inuktitut. Linguistics , 38(3), 483–521. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Allen, S.E.M., Skarabela, B., & Hughes, M.
(2008) Using corpora to examine discourse effects in syntax. In H. Behrens (Ed.), Corpora in Language Acquisition Research: Finding Structure in Data (pp. 99–137). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Apperly, I.
(2011)  Mindreaders: The Cognitive Basis of ‘Theory of Mind’ . Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Ariel, M.
(2010)  Defining Pragmatics . Cambridge: CUP. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bergelson, E., & Swingley, D.
(2012) At 6–9 months, human infants know the meanings of many common nouns. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences , 109(9), 3253–3258. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Berger, F., & Höhle, B.
(2012) Restrictions on addition: children’s interpretation of the focus particles auch ‘also’ and nur ‘only’ in German. Journal of Child Language , 39(02), 383–410. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Csibra, G.
(2010) Recognizing communicative intentions in infancy. Mind and Language , 25(2), 141–168. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Csibra, G., & Gergely, G.
(2011) Natural pedagogy as evolutionary adaptation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences , 366(1567), 1149–1157. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Daum, M.M., Ulber, J., & Gredebäck, G.
in press). The development of pointing comprehension in infancy: Effects of communicative verbal cues on covert shifts of attention. Developmental Psychology .
de Marchena, A., Eigsti, I.-M., Worek, A., Ono, K.E., & Snedeker, J.
(2011) Mutual exclusivity in autism spectrum disorders: Testing the pragmatic hypothesis. Cognition , 119(1), 96–113. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Diesendruck, G., & Markson, L.
(2011) Children’s assumption of the conventionality of culture. Child Development Perspectives , 5(3), 189–195. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Evans, N., & Levinson, S.C.
(2009) The myth of language universals: Language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences , 32(05), 429–448. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Frank, M.C., & Goodman, N.D.
(2012) Predicting pragmatic reasoning in language games. Science , 336, 998. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gegerly, G., & Csibra, G.
(2006) Sylvia’s recipe: The role of imitation and pedagogy in the transmission of cultural knowledge. In N.J. Enfield & S.C. Levinson (Eds.), Roots of Human Sociality: Culture, Cognition and Interaction . Oxford: Wenner-Gren Foundation.Google Scholar
Gomez, J.C.
(2007) Pointing behaviors in apes and human infants: A balanced interpretation. Child Development , 78(3), 729–734. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grice, H.P.
(1975) Logic and conversation. In P. Cole & J. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and Semantics 3, Speech Acts (pp. 41–58). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Henrich, J., Heine, S.J., & Norenzayan, A.
(2010) Most people are not WEIRD. Nature , 466(7302), 29–29.Google Scholar
Kalish, C.W., & Sabbagh, M.A.
(2007) Conventionality and cognitive development: Learning to think the right way. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development , 2007(115), 1–9. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A.
(1998) Development itself is the key to understanding developmental disorders. Trends in Cognitive Sciences , 2(10), 389–398. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Krahmer, E., & van Deemter, K.
(2011) Computational generation of referring expressions: A survey. Computational Linguistics , 38(1), 173–218. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Levinson, S.C.
(1983)  Pragmatics . Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
(2006) On the human “interaction engine”. In N.J. Enfield & S.C. Levinson (Eds.), Roots of Human Sociality: Culture, Cognition and Interaction . Oxford: The Wenner-Gren Foundation.Google Scholar
Ninio, A., & Snow, C.E.
(1996)  Pragmatic Development . Boulder, CO: Westview Press.Google Scholar
Norbury, C.F., & Sparks, A.
(2013) Difference or disorder? Cultural issues in understanding neurodevelopmental disorders. Developmental Psychology , 49(1), 45–58. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ochs, E., & Schieffelin, B.
(1979)  Developmental Pragmatics . New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Parise, E., & Csibra, G.
(2012) Electrophysiological evidence for the understanding of maternal speech by 9-month-old infants. Psychological Science , 23(7), 728–733. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Potts, C.
in press). Pragmatics. In R. Mitkov (Ed.) Oxford Handbook of Computational Linguistics 2nd Ed. Oxford OUP.
Rakoczy, H., Brosche, N., Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M.
(2009) Young children’s understanding of the context-relativity of normative rules in conventional games. British Journal of Developmental Psychology , 27(2), 445–456. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sarilar, A., Matthews, D.E., & Kuntay, A.
in press). Hearing relative clauses boosts relative clause usage (and referential clarity) in young Turkish language learners. Applied Psycholinguistics .
Saygin, A.P., & Cicekli, I.
(2002) Pragmatics in human-computer conversations. Journal of Pragmatics , 34(3), 227–258. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Serratrice, L.
(2004) Crosslinguistic influence at the syntax-pragmatics interface: Subjects and objects in English-Italian bilingual and monolingual acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition , 7(3), 183–205. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Serratrice, L., Sorace, A., Filiaci, F., & Baldo, M.
(2011) Pronominal objects in English-­Italian and Spanish-Italian bilingual children. Applied Psycholinguistics , 33, 725–751. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Siegal, M., & Peterson, C.C.
(1998) Preschoolers’ understanding of lies and innocent and negligent mistakes. Developmental psychology , 34(2), 332–341. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sperber, D., & Wilson, D.
(1995)  Relevance: Communication and Cognition (2nd Ed.). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Talwar, V., & Lee, K.
(2008) Social and cognitive correlates of children’s lying behavior. Child Development , 79(4), 866–881. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M.
(2003)  Constructing a Language: A Usage-Based Theory of Language Acquisition . Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
(2008)  Origins of Human Communication . Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H.
(2005) Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences , 28, 675–690. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Turing, A.M.
(1950) I. – Computing machinery and intelligence. Mind , LIX(236), 433–460. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
van Deemter, K., Gatt, A., van Gompel, R.P.G., & Krahmer, E.
(2012) Toward a computational psycholinguistics of reference production. Topics in Cognitive Science , 4(2), 166–183. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wells, B.
(2010) Tonal repetition and tonal contrast in English carer-child interaction. In D. Barth-Weingarten, E. Reber & M. Selting (Eds.), Prosody in Interaction (pp. 243–262). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wells, B., & Local, J.
(2009) Prosody as an interactional resource: A clinical linguistic perspective. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology , 11(4), 321–325. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wittgenstein, L.
(1958)  Philosophical investigations . (G.E.M. Anscombe, Trans.). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Wyman, E., Rakoczy, H., & Tomasello, M.
(2009) Normativity and context in young children’s pretend play. Cognitive Development , 24(2), 146–155. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 2 other publications

Clark, Eve V.
2018.  In The Encyclopedia of Applied Linguistics,  pp. 1 ff. Crossref logo
Schulze, Cornelia
2020.  In The Encyclopedia of Child and Adolescent Development,  pp. 1 ff. Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 26 february 2021. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.