Part of
The Acquisition of Reference
Edited by Ludovica Serratrice and Shanley E.M. Allen
[Trends in Language Acquisition Research 15] 2015
► pp. 105121
References
Acredolo, L., Goodwyn, S., & Abrams, D
(2002) Baby signs (3rd edition). New York, NY: McGraw Hill.Google Scholar
Aksu-Koç, A., & Slobin, D.I
(1985) The acquisition of Turkish. In D.I. Slobin (Ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition (Vol. 1., pp. 839–878). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bates, E
(1976) Language and context: The acquisition of pragmatics. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V
(1979) The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Thal, D., Whitesell, K., Fenson, L., & Oakes, L
(1989) Integrating language and gesture in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 25(6), 1004–1019. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J.S
(1975) The ontogenesis of speech acts. Journal of Child Language, 2(1), 1–19. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Butcher, C., & Goldin-Meadow, S
(2000) Gesture and the transition from one- to two-word speech: When hand and mouth come together. In D. McNeill (Ed.), Language and gesture (pp. 235–258). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Capirci, O., Contaldo, A., Caselli, M.C., & Volterra, V
Capone, N.C., & McGregor, K.K
(2004) Gesture development: A review for clinical and research practices. Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research, 47(1), 173–186. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clancy, P.M
(1993) Preferred argument structure in Korean acquisition. In E.V. Clark (Ed.), Proceedings of the Twenty-Fifth Annual Child Language Research Forum. Stanford, CA: CSLI.Google Scholar
Demir, Ö. E
(2009) A tale of two hands: Development of narrative structure in children’s speech and gesture and its relation to later reading skill (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Chicago, Chicago, IL.Google Scholar
Demir, Ö. E., Fisher, J.A., Goldin-Meadow, S., & Levine, S.C
(2014) Narrative processing in typically developing children and children with early unilateral brain injury: Seeing gesture matters. Developmental Psychology, 50(3), 815–828. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Demir, Ö. E., Levine, S.C., & Goldin-Meadow, S
2014). A tale of two hands: Children’s early gesture use in narrative production predicts later narrative structure in speech. Journal of Child Language, 42(3), 662–681. DOI: 10.1017/S0305000914000415.
Demir, Ö. E., So, W.C., Özyürek, A., & Goldin-Meadow, S
(2012) Turkish-and English-speaking children display sensitivity to perceptual context in the referring expressions they produce in speech and gesture. Language and Cognitive Processes, 27(6), 844–867. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Furman, R., Küntay, A.C., & Özyürek, A
(2014) Early language-specificity of children’s event encoding in speech and gesture: Evidence from caused motion in Turkish. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29(5), 620–634. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Genesee, F., Nicoladis, E., & Paradis, J
(1995) Language differentiation in early bilingual development. Journal of Child Language, 22(3), 611–631. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gentner, D
(1982) Why nouns are learned before verbs: Linguistic relativity versus natural partitioning. In S.A. Kuczaj (Ed.), Language development: Language, thought, and culture (Vol. 2, pp. 301–334). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Gentner, D., & Boroditsky, L
(2001) Individuation, relational relativity and early word learning. In M. Bowerman & S. Levinson (Eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development (pp. 215–256). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gliga, T., & Csibra, G
(2009) One-year-old infants appreciate the referential nature of deictic gestures and words. Psychological Science, 20(3), 347–353. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S
(1997) When gestures and words speak differently. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 6(5), 138–143. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldin‐Meadow, S
(2007) Pointing sets the stage for learning language – and creating language. Child Development, 78(3), 741–745. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., & Butcher, C
(2003) Pointing toward two-word speech in young children. In S. Kita (Ed.), Pointing: Where language, culture, and cognition meet (pp. 85–107). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Goldin‐Meadow, S., Goodrich, W., Sauer, E., & Iverson, J
(2007) Young children use their hands to tell their mothers what to say. Developmental Science, 10(6), 778–785. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., Alibali, M.W., & Church, R.B
(1993) Transitions in concept acquisition: Using the hand to read the mind. Psychological Review, 100(2), 279–279. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Golinkoff, R.M
(1986) ‘I beg your pardon?’: The preverbal negotiation of failed messages. Journal of Child Language, 13(3), 455–476. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Greenfield, P.M., & Smith, J.H
(1976) Structure of communication in early language development. New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Gürcanli, Ö., Nakipoğlu, M., & Özyürek, A
(2007) Shared information and argument omission in Turkish. In H. Caunt-Nulton, S. Kulatilake, & I. Woo (Eds.), Proceedings of the 31st Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 267–273). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Iverson, J.M., & Goldin-Meadow, S
(2005) Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychological Science, 16(5), 367–371. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Iverson, J.M., Capirci, O., Volterra, V., & Goldin-Meadow, S
(2008) Learning to talk in a gesture-rich world: Early communication in Italian vs. American children. First Language, 28(2), 164–181. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Iverson, J.M., & Thelen, E
(1999) Hand, mouth and brain: The dynamic emergence of speech and gesture. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6(11–12), 19–40.Google Scholar
Kelly, S.D
(2001) Broadening the units of analysis in communication: Speech and nonverbal behaviours in pragmatic comprehension. Journal of Child Language, 28(2), 325–349. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kendon, A., & Versante, L
(2003) Pointing by hand in Neapolitan. In S. Kita (Ed.), Pointing: Where language, culture, and cognition meet (pp. 109–137). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Liszkowski, U., Brown, P., Callaghan, T., Takada, A., & de Vos, C
(2012) A prelinguistic gestural universal of human communication. Cognitive Science, 36(4), 698–713. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., Striano, T., & Tomasello, M
(2006) 12-and 18-month-olds point to provide information for others. Journal of Cognition and Development, 7(2), 173–187. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Masur, E.F
(1982) Mothers’ responses to infants’ object-related gestures: Influences on lexical development. Journal of Child Language, 9(1), 23–30. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Matthews, D., Lieven, E., Theakston, A., & Tomasello, M
(2006) The effect of perceptual availability and prior discourse on young children’s use of referring expressions. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27(3), 403–422. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mayberry, R.I., Jaques, J., & DeDe, G
(1998) What stuttering reveals about the development of the gesture-speech relationship. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 1998(79), 77–88. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D
(1992) Hand and mind: What gestures reveal about thought. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
McNeil, N.M., Alibali, M.W., & Evans, J.L
(2000) The role of gesture in children’s comprehension of spoken language: Now they need it, now they don’t. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24(2), 131–150. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morford, M., & Goldin-Meadow, S
(1992) Comprehension and production of gesture in combination with speech in one-word speakers. Journal of Child Language, 19(3), 559–580. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Namy, L.L., & Waxman, S.R
(1998) Words and gestures: Infants’ interpretations of different forms of symbolic reference. Child Development, 69(2), 295–308. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nicoladis, E
(2002) Some gestures develop in conjunction with spoken language development and others don’t: Evidence from bilingual preschoolers. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 26(4), 241–266. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007) The effect of bilingualism on the use of manual gestures. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28(3), 441–454. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nicoladis, E., Mayberry, R.I., & Genesee, F
(1999) Gesture and early bilingual development. Developmental Psychology, 35(2), 514–526. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Özçalışkan, S., Gentner, D., & Goldin-Meadow, S
(2014) Do iconic gestures pave the way for children’s early verbs? Applied Psycholinguistics, 35(6), 1143–1162. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Özçalışkan, Ş., & Goldin-Meadow, S
(2005) Gesture is at the cutting edge of early language development. Cognition, 96(3), B101–B113. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011) Is there an iconic gesture spurt at 26 months? In G. Stam & M. Ishino (Eds.), Integrating gestures: The interdisciplinary nature of gesture (pp. 163–174). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pizzuto, E., & Capobianco, M
Rowe, M.L., & Goldin-Meadow, S
(2009) Differences in early gesture explain SES disparities in child vocabulary size at school entry. Science, 323(5916), 951–953. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sauer, E., Levine, S.C., & Goldin-Meadow, S
(2010) Early gesture predicts language delay in children with pre- or perinatal brain lesions. Child Development, 81(2), 528–539. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
So, W.C., Demir, Ö. E., & Goldin-Meadow, S
(2010) When speech is ambiguous, gesture steps in: Sensitivity to discourse-pragmatic principles in early childhood. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31(01), 209–224. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
So, W.C., Kita, S., & Goldin-Meadow, S
(2009) Using the hands to identify who does what to whom: Gesture and speech go hand-in-hand. Cognitive Science, 33(1), 115–125. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
So, W.C., Lim, J., & Tan, S.H
(2014) Sensitivity to information status in discourse: Gesture precedes speech in unbalanced bilinguals. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35(01), 71–95. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilkins, D
(2003) Why pointing with the index finger is not a universal (in sociocultural and semiotic terms). In S. Kita (Ed.), Pointing: Where language, culture and cognition meet (pp. 171–215). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 2 other publications

Salazar-Orvig, Anne, Geneviève de Weck, Rouba Hassan & Annie Rialland
Salazar-Orvig, Anne, Geneviève de Weck, Rouba Hassan & Annie Rialland

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 21 march 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.