Part of
Semantics in Language Acquisition
Edited by Kristen Syrett and Sudha Arunachalam
[Trends in Language Acquisition Research 24] 2018
► pp. 124150
References
Arunachalam, S., & Kothari, A.
(2010) Telicity and event culmination in Hindi perfectives. In P. M. Bertinetto, A. Korhonen, A. Lenci, A. Melinger, S. Schulte im Walde, & A. Villavicencio (Eds.), Proceedings of the 2nd interdisciplinary Workshop on the Identification and Representation of Verb Features (pp. 16–19). Pisa: University of Pisa.Google Scholar
Behrens, H.
(1998) How difficult are complex verbs? Evidence from German, Dutch and English. Linguistics, 36, 679–712.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M.
(1997) Uncommon understanding. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Botting, N., & Adams, C.
(2005) Semantic and inferencing abilities in children with communication disorders. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 40, 49–66.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Conti-Ramsden, G., & Botting, N.
(2006) Specific Language Impairment. In K. Brown (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics (pp. 629–632). Oxford: Elsevier.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crain, S., & McKee, C.
(1986) Acquisition of structural restrictions on anaphora. In S. Berman, J. W. Choe, & J. McDonough (Eds.), Proceedings of the Northeastern Linguistic Society. Amherst, MA: GLSA.Google Scholar
Demirdache, H., & Martin, F.
(2015) Agent control over non culminating events. In E. Barrajón López, J. L. Cifuentes Honrubia, & S. Rodríguez Rosique (Eds.), Verb classes and aspect, (pp.185–217) Amsterdam: John Benjamins.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Villiers, J. G.
(2003) Defining SLI: A linguistic perspective. In Y. Levy & J. Schaeffer (Eds.), Language competence across populations: Toward a definition of specific language impairment (pp. 425–447). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Dowty, D. R.
(1979) Word meaning and Montague grammar. Dordrecht: Reidel.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Filip, H.
(2008) Events and maximalization: The case of telicity and perfectivity. In S. Rothstein (Ed.), Theoretical and crosslinguistic approaches to the semantics of aspect, (pp. 217–256). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014) Weak and strong telicity via maximization Delimit Event. Presentation atWorkshop on Identification and Delimitation of Events, Université Paris 7, March, 2014.
Friedmann, N., & Novogrodsky, R.
(2008) Subtypes of SLI: SySLI, PhoSLI, LeSLI, and PraSLI. In A. Gavarró & M. J. Freitas (Eds.), Language acquisition and development (pp. 205–217). Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars.Google Scholar
Gentner, D.
(1978) On relational meaning: The acquisition of verb meaning. Child Development, 49, 988–998.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gentner, D.
(1982) Why nouns are learned before verbs: Linguistic relativity versus natural partitioning. In S. Kuczaj (Ed.), Language development, Vol. 2: Language, thought, and culture. (pp. 301–334). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Grinstead, J., McCurley, D., Pratt, T., Obregon, P, & Flores, B.
(2013) The semantics of the tense deficit in child Spanish SLI. In M. Becker, J. Grinstead, & J. Rothman (Eds.), Generative linguistics and acquisition: Studies in honor of Nina M. Hyams (pp. 107–128). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gropen, J., Pinker, S., Hollander, M. & Goldberg, R.
(1991) Syntax and semantics in the acquisition of locative verbs. Journal of Child Language, 18, 115–151.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hamann, C.
(2015) Language impairment in German children. In A. Stavrakaki (Ed.), Specific Language Impairment. Current trends in research (pp. 215–251). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hay, J., Kennedy, C. & Levin, B.
(1999) Scale structure underlies telicity in ‘degree achievements’. In T. Matthews & D. Strolovitch (Eds.), Proceedings of Semantics and Linguistic Theory 9 (pp. 127–144). Ithaca, NY: CLC Publications.Google Scholar
van Hout, A.
(1996) Event semantics of verb frame alternation. A case study of Dutch and its acquisition (Doctoral dissertation). University of Tilburg. (Published in 1998 by Garland, NY.)Google Scholar
(1998) On the role of direct objects and particles in learning telicity in Dutch and English. In A. Greenhill, M. Hughes, H. Littlefield, & H. Walsh (Eds.), BUCLD 22: Proceedings of the 22nd annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 397–408). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
(2000) Event semantics in the lexicon-syntax interface: Verb frame alternations in Dutch and their acquisition. In C. L. Tenny & J. Pustejovsky (Eds.), Events as grammatical objects. The converging perspectives of lexical semantics and syntax (pp. 239–282). Stanford CA: CSLI.Google Scholar
(2008) Acquiring telicity cross-linguistically: On the acquisition of telicity entailments associated with transitivity. In M. Bowerman & P. Brown (Eds.), Crosslinguistic perspectives on argument structure: Implications for learnability (pp. 255–278). New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
2018On the acquisition of event culmination. In K. Syrett & S. Arunachalam (Eds.), Semantics in Language Acquisition (pp. 95–121). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ingham, R., Fletcher, P., Schelletter, C., & Sinka, I.
(1998) Resultative VPs and specific language impairment. Language Acquisition, 7, 87–111.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jackendoff, R.
(1996) The proper treatment of measuring out, telicity, and perhaps even quantification in English. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 14, 305–354.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jeschull, L.
(2007) The pragmatics of telicity and what children make of it. In A. Belikova, L. Meroni, & M. Umeda (Eds.), Proceedings of the 2nd Conference on Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition North America (pp. 180–187). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Kauschke. C.
(2000) Der Erwerb des frühkindlichen Lexikons–eine empirische Studie zur Entwicklung des Wortschatzes im Deutschen. Tübingen: Narr.Google Scholar
Kelly, D., & Rice, M.
(1994) Preferences for verb interpretation in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37, 182–192.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kieburg, A., & Schulz, P.
(2010) Input factors in early verb acquisition: Do word order variability and word frequency of verbs matter? In M. Anderssen, K. Berentzen, & M. Westergaard (Eds.), Optionality in the input. Papers from the GLOW 30 Workshop (pp. 95–127). Dordrecht: Springer.Google Scholar
Kiese-Himmel, C., & Kozielski, P. M.
(1996) Aktiver Wortschatztest für drei- bis sechsjährige Kinder. AWST 3–6. (2nd ed.). Weinheim: Beltz.Google Scholar
Krifka, M.
(1989) Nominal reference, temporal constitution, and quantification in event semantics. In R. Bartsch, J. van Benthem, & P. van Emde Boas (Eds.), Semantics and Contextual Expression, (pp. 75–115). Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
(1998) The origins of telicity. In S. Rothstein (Ed.), Events and grammar (pp. 197–235). Dordrecht: Kluwer.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L. P.
(2014) Children with Specific Language Impairment. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Levin, B., & Rappaport Hovav, M.
(1995) Unaccusativity: At the syntax-lexical semantics interface. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Lakusta, L., & DiFabrizio, S.
(2016) And, the winner is…a visual preference for end points over starting points in infants’ motion event representations. Infancy, 22, 323–343.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lakusta, L., Muentener, P., Petrillo, L., Mullanaphy, N., & Muniz, L.
(2016) Does making something move matter? Representations of goals and sources in causal motion events. Cognitive Science, 41, 814–826.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Maguire, M. J., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., Imai, M., Haryu, E., Vanegas, S. Okada, H., Pulverman, R., & Sanchez-Davis, B.
(2010) A developmental shift from similar to language specific strategies in verb acquisition: A comparison of English, Spanish, and Japanese. Cognition, 114, 299–319.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Penner, Z., Schulz, P., & Wymann, K.
(2003) Learning the meaning of verbs: What distinguishes language impaired from normally developing children? Linguistics, 41, 289–319.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pustejovsky, J.
(1995) The generative lexicon. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Roeper, T.
(2004) Diagnosing language variations: Underlying principles for syntactic assessment. Seminars in Speech and Language, 25, 41–55.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schulz, P.
(2002) Relational Word Inventory (RWI) (Unpublished manuscript). Humboldt University, Berlin.Google Scholar
(2005, July). Are children’s first event expressions telic? Evidence from child German. Paper presented at 10th International Congress for the Study of Child Language, Berlin.
(2010) Some notes on semantics and SLI. In A. Castro, J. Costa, M. Lobo, & F. Pratas (Eds.), Language acquisition and development. Proceedings of Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition 2009 (pp. 391–406). Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars.Google Scholar
Schulz, P., & Kiese-Himmel, C.
(2006) Verbverstehen und expressiver Wortschatzumfang bei sprech-/sprachentwicklungsgestörten Kindern. L.O.G.O.S. Interdisziplinär, 4, 244–252.Google Scholar
Schulz, P., & Ose, J.
(2008, February). Semantics and pragmatics in the acquisition of telicity. Paper presented at 29. Jahrestagung der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Sprachwissenschaft (DGfS), Bamberg.
Schulz, P., & Penner, Z.
(2002) How you can eat the apple and have it too: Evidence from the acquisition of telicity in German. In J. Costa & M. J. Freitas (Eds.), Proceedings of the GALA’ 2001 Conference on Language Acquisition (pp. 239–246). Lisboa: Associação Portuguesa de Linguística, Lisboa.Google Scholar
Schulz, P., Penner, Z., & Wymann, K.
(2002) Comprehension of resultative verbs in normally developing and language impaired children. In F. Windsor, M. L. Kelly, & N. Hewlett (Eds.), Investigations in clinical phonetics and linguistics (pp. 115–129). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Schulz, P., & Roeper, T.
(2011) Acquisition of exhaustivity in wh-questions: A semantic dimension of SLI? Lingua, 121(3), 383–407.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schulz, P., & Tracy, R.
(2011) Linguistische Sprachstandserhebung – Deutsch als Zweitsprache (LiSe-DaZ). Göttingen: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Schulz, P., & Wenzel, R.
(2005, July). Acquisition of compositional telicity by German-speaking children with specific language impairment. Paper presented at the Symposium ‘Crosslinguistic acquisition of telicity’, 10th International Congress for the Study of Child Language, Berlin.
Schulz, P., & Wittek, A.
(2003) Opening doors and sweeping floors: What children with specific language impairment know about telic and atelic verbs. In B. Beachley, A. Brown, & F. Colin (Eds.), BUCLD 27: Proceedings of the 27th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (Vol. 2, pp. 727–738). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Schulz, P., Wymann, K., & Penner, Z.
(2001) The early acquisition of verb meaning in German by normally developing and language impaired children. Brain and Language, 77, 407–418.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Talmy, L.
(1991) Path to realization: A typology of event integration. In L. A. Sutton, C. Johnson, & R. Shields (Eds.), Proceedings of the 17th Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society: General session and parasession on the grammar of event structure (pp. 480–519). Available online: <[URL]>
van der Lely, H. K. J.
(2005) Domain-specific cognitive systems: Insight from grammatical specific language impairment. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 53–59.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vendler, Z.
(1957) Verbs and times. The Philosophical Review, 66, 143–160.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wagner, L., & Lakusta, L.
(2009) Using language to navigate the infant mind. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 177–184.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Walkková, M.
(2013) The aspectual function of particles in phrasal verbs (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Groningen.Google Scholar
Watkins, R., & Rice, M.
(1991) Verb particle and preposition acquisition in language-impaired preschoolers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 1130–1141.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wittek, A.
(2002) Learning the meaning of change-of-state verbs: A case study of German child language. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 2 other publications

Horvath, Sabrina, Leslie Rescorla & Sudha Arunachalam
2018. Chapter 4. Semantic features of early verb vocabularies. In Semantics in Language Acquisition [Trends in Language Acquisition Research, 24],  pp. 68 ff. DOI logo
van Hout, Angeliek
2018. Chapter 5. On the acquisition of event culmination. In Semantics in Language Acquisition [Trends in Language Acquisition Research, 24],  pp. 96 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 22 march 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.