AAP
(2020) Remote housing most important NT issue. NITV. Retrieved on 14 June 2021 from [URL]
Ackerman, F., Blevins, J. P., & Malouf, R.
(2009) Parts and wholes: Implicative patterns in inflectional paradigms. In J. P. Blevins & J. Blevins (Eds.), Analogy in grammar. Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ackerman, F., & Malouf, R.
(2013) Morphological organization: The low conditional entropy conjecture. Language, 89(3), 429–464. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Aguado-Orea, J.
(2004) The acquisition of morpho-syntax in Spanish: Implications for current theories of development (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). The University of Nottingham.Google Scholar
Aguado-Orea, J., & Pine, J. M.
(2015) Comparing different models of the development of verb inflection in early child Spanish. PLOS ONE, 10(3). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Aguirre, C.
(2003) Early verb development in one Spanish-speaking child. In D. Bittner, W. U. Dressler, & M. Kilani-Schoch (Eds.), Development of verb inflection in first language acquisition: A cross-linguistic perspective (pp. 1–25). Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Aksu-Koç, A., & Ketrez, F. N.
(2003) Early verbal morphology in Turkish: Emergence of inflections. In D. Bittner, W. U. Dressler, & M. Kilani-Schoch (Eds.), Development of verb inflection in first language acquisition: A cross-linguistic perspective (pp. 27–52). Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Aksu-Koç, A., & Slobin, D. I.
(1985) The acquisition of Turkish. In D. I. Slobin (Ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, Vol. 1: The data (pp. 839–878). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Albright, A., & Hayes, B.
(2003) Rules vs. Analogy in English past tenses: A computational/experimental study. Cognition, 90(2), 119–161. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Allen, S.
(2017) Polysynthesis in the acquisition of Inuit languages. In M. Fortescue, M. Mithun, & N. Evans (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of polysynthesis (pp. 449–472). Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Allen, S. E. M., & Crago, M. B.
(1996) Early passive acquisition in Inuktitut. Journal of Child Language, 23(1), 129–155. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ambridge, B., Kidd, E., Rowland, C. F., & Theakston, A. L.
(2015) The ubiquity of frequency effects in first language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 42(2), 239–273. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ambridge, B., & Lieven, E.
(2011) Child language acquisition: Contrasting theoretical approaches. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Argus, R., & Kazakovskaya, V.
(2013) Acquisition of compounds in Estonian and Russian: Frequency, productivity, transparency and simplicity effect. Eesti Rakenduslingvistika Ühingu Aastaraamat / Estonian Papers in Applied Linguistics, 9, 23–42. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arkadiev, P., & Gardani, F.
(Eds.) (2020) Complexities of morphology. Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Aronsson, K., & Thorell, M.
(1999) Family politics in children’s play directives. Journal of Pragmatics, 31(1), 25–48. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Astington, J. W.
(1988) Children’s production of commissive speech acts. Journal of Child Language, 15(02), 411–423. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Austin, J. L.
(1962) How to do things with words. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Australian Institute of Health and Welfare
(2011) Ear and hearing health of Indigenous children in the Northern Territory. AIHW.Google Scholar
Baayen, R. H.
(2008) Analyzing linguistic data: A practical introduction to statistics using R. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baker, M.
(1985) The mirror principle and morphosyntactic explanation. Linguistic Inquiry, 16(3), 373–415.Google Scholar
Barone-Nugent, B.
(2008) Event classification: A cognitive semantic analysis of two Murrinh-Patha event categories (Unpublished honours thesis). University of Melbourne.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V.
(1975) The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer quarterly of behavior and development, 21(3), 205–226.Google Scholar
(1976) Sensorimotor performatives. In E. Bates (Ed.), Language and context: Studies in the acquisition of pragmatics. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Behrens, H.
(1998) How difficult are complex verbs? Evidence from German, Dutch and English. Linguistics, 36(4), 679–712. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009) Usage-based and emergentist approaches to language acquisition. Linguistics, 47(2), 383–411. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Berko, J.
(1958) The child’s learning of English morphology. Word, 14, 150–177. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Berman, R. A.
(2011) Children’s acquisition of compound constructions. In R. Lieber & P. Štekauer (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of compounding (pp. 298–322). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bernicot, J., & Laval, V.
(2004) Speech acts in children: The example of promises. In I. A. Noveck & D. Sperber (Eds.), Experimental pragmatics (pp. 207–227). Palgrave Macmillan. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bittner, D.
(2003) The emergence of verb inflection in two German-speaking children. In D. Bittner, W. U. Dressler, & M. Kilani-Schoch (Eds.), Development of verb inflection in first language acquisition: A cross-linguistic perspective (pp. 53–88). Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bittner, D., Dressler, W. U., & Kilani-Schoch, M.
(Eds.) (2003a) Development of verb inflection in first language acquisition: A cross-linguistic perspective. Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2003b) Introduction. In D. Bittner, W. U. Dressler, & M. Kilani-Schoch (Eds.), Development of verb inflection in first language acquisition: A cross-linguistic perspective (pp. vii–xxxvii). Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Blevins, J. P., Ackerman, F., & Malouf, R.
(2018) Word and paradigm morphology. In J. Audring & F. Masini (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of morphological theory (pp. 264–284). Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bloom, L.
(1991) Language development from two to three. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Blythe, J.
(2009) Doing referring in Murriny Patha conversation (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Sydney.Google Scholar
(2010) From ethical datives to number markers in Murriny Patha. In R. Hendrey & J. Hendriks (Eds.), Grammatical change: Theory and description (pp. 157–187). Pacific Linguistics.Google Scholar
(2013) Preference organization driving structuration: Evidence from Australian Aboriginal interaction for pragmatically motivated grammaticalization. Language, 89(4), 883–919. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Blythe, J., Nordlinger, R., Street, C., Forshaw, W., & Mansfield, J.
n.d.). Murrinhpatha Toolbox database.
Blythe, J., Reid, N. J., & Nordlinger, R.
(2007) Murriny Patha finite verb paradigms.Google Scholar
Bottari, P., Cipriani, P., & Chilosi, A. M.
(1993) Protosyntactic devices in the acquisition of Italian free morphology. Language Acquisition, 3(4), 327–369. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bowerman, M.
(2011) Linguistic typology and first language acquisition. In J. J. Song (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of linguistic typology (pp. 591–617). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Brown, P.
(1997) Isolating the CVC root in Tzeltal Mayan: A study of children’s first verbs. In E. V. Clark (Ed.), Proceedings of the 28th annual Child Language Research Forum (pp. 41–52). CSLI.Google Scholar
(1998) Children’s first verbs in Tzeltal: Evidence for an early verb category. Linguistics, 36(4), 713–753. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brown, R.
(1973) A first language: The early stages. Harvard University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bunduck, D. F., & Ward, T.
(2017) The program at Wadeye, past and present. In B. Devlin, S. Disbray & N. Devlin (Eds.), History of bilingual education in the Northern Territory (pp. 285–292). Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bunduck, D. X.
(2018) Kardu palngun murrinh niyith-niyith peningime. Wadeye Literature Production Centre.Google Scholar
Bybee, J. L.
(1995) Regular morphology and the lexicon. Language and Cognitive Processes, 10(5), 425–455. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J. L., & Moder, C. L.
(1983) Morphological classes as natural categories. Language, 2, 251–270. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cameron-Faulkner, T.
(2014) The development of speech acts. In D. Matthews (Ed.), Pragmatic development in first language acquisition (pp. 37–52). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cameron-Faulkner, T., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M.
(2003) A construction based analysis of child directed speech. Cognitive Science, 27(6), 843–873. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cazden, C. B.
(1968) The acquisition of noun and verb inflections. Child Development, 39(2), 433–448. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chee, M.
(2017) A longitudinal cross-sectional study on the acquisition of Navajo verbs in children aged 4 years 7 months through 11 years 2 months (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). The University of New Mexico.Google Scholar
Ciesielski, S.
(2015) Language development and socialisation in Sherpa (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Melbourne.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H.
(1999) Lexical entries and rules of language: A multidisciplinary study of German inflection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 991–1060. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2006a) Dual-mechanism morphology. In K. Brown (Ed.), Encyclopaedia of language and linguistics. Elsevier. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2006b) Linguistic perspectives on morphological processing. In D. Wunderlich (Ed.), Advances in the theory of the lexicon (pp. 355–388). Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V.
(1978) Discovering what words can do. In D. Farkas, W. M. Jacobsen, & K. W. Todrys (Eds.), Papers from the parasession on the lexicon, Chicago Linguistics Society April 14–15, 1978 (pp. 34–57). Chicago Linguistic Society, University of Chicago.Google Scholar
(1981) Lexical innovations: How children learn to create new words. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 18.Google Scholar
(1993) The lexicon in acquisition. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V., & Berman, R. A.
(1987) Types of linguistic knowledge: Interpreting and producing compound nouns. Journal of Child Language, 14(03), 547–567. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V., Frant Hecht, B., & Mulford, R. C.
(1986) Coining complex compounds in English: Affixes and word order in acquisition. Linguistics, 24(1), 7–30. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V., Gelman, S. A., & Lane, N. M.
(1985) Compound nouns and category structure in young children. Child Development, 56(1), 84–94. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clemens, M.
(2013) Verbal stress patterns in a morphologically complex language: Murrinh-Patha (Unpublished honours thesis). University of Melbourne.Google Scholar
Courtney, E. H., & Saville-Troike, M.
(2002) Learning to construct verbs in Navajo and Quechua. Journal of Child Language, 29(03), 623–654. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Couzos, S., Metcalf, S., & Murray, R.
(2001) Systematic review of existing evidence and primary care guidelines on the management of Otitis Media in Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander populations. Office for Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander Health Services, Commonwealth Department of Health and Family Service. [URL]
Crago, M. B., & Allen, S. E. M.
(2001) Early finiteness in Inuktitut: The role of language structure. Language Acquisition, 9(1), 59–111. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crago, M. B., Allen, S. E. M., & Pesco, D.
(1998) Issues of complexity in Inuktitut and English child directed speech. In E. V. Clark (Ed.), Proceedings of the 29th annual Child Language Research Forum (pp. 37–46). CSLI.Google Scholar
Cummings, L.
(2013) Pragmatics: A multidisciplinary perspective. Taylor and Francis. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dąbrowska, E.
(2001) Learning a morphological system without a default: The Polish genitive. Journal of Child Language, 28(3), 545–574. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2004) Rules or schemas? Evidence from Polish. Language and Cognitive Processes, 19(2), 225–271. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Davidson, L.
(2018) Allies and adversaries (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Melbourne.Google Scholar
Deen, K. U.
(2005) The acquisition of Swahili. John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009) The morphosyntax interface. In E. L. Bavin (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of child language (pp. 259–280). Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Demuth, K.
(1989) Maturation and the acquisition of the Sesotho passive. Language, 65(1), 56–80. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1992) Acquisition of Sesotho. In D. I. Slobin (Ed.), The cross-linguistic study of language acquisition (Vol. 3, pp. 557–638). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
(1996a) Collecting spontaneous production data. In Methods for assessing children’s syntax (pp. 3–22). The MIT Press.Google Scholar
(1996b) The prosodic structure of early words. In J. Morgan & K. Demuth (Eds.), Signal to syntax: Bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition (pp. 171–184). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
(2001) Prosodic constraints on morphological development. In J. Weissenborn & B. Höhle (Eds.), Approaches to Bootstrapping (pp. 3–22). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2006) Crosslinguistic perspectives on the development of prosodic words. Language & Speech, 49(2), 129–135. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014) Prosodic licensing and the development of phonological and morphological representations. In A. Farris-Trimble & J. Barlow (Eds.), Perspectives on phonological theory and development. In honor of Daniel A. Dinnsen. (pp. 11–24). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2018) The development of prosodic phonology. In S.-A. Rueschemeyer & M. G. Gaskell (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 674–689). Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Demuth, K., & Fee, E. J.
(1995) Minimal prosodic words in early phonological development. (Unpublished manuscript). Brown University and Dalhousie University.Google Scholar
Devlin, B.
(2005) Two-way learning in the NT: Some research-based recommendations (Report prepared for NT DEET).Google Scholar
Disbray, S.
(2008) More than one way to catch a frog: A study of children’s discourse in an Australian contact language (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Melbourne.Google Scholar
Dobrin, L. M., & Berson, J.
(2011) Speakers and language documentation. In J. Sallabank & P. K. Austin (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of endangered languages (pp. 187–211). Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dressler, W. U.
(1997) Introduction. Papers and Studies in Contrastive Linguistics, 33, 9–14. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007a) Introduction. Antwerp Papers in Linguistics: Typological Perspectives on the Acquisition of Noun and Verb Morphology, 112, 3–9.Google Scholar
(2007b) Productivity in word formation. In G. Jarema & G. Libben (Eds.), The mental lexicon: Core perspectives (pp. 159–183). Elsevier. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012) On the acquisition of inflectional morphology: Introduction. Morphology, 22(1), 1–8. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dressler, W. U., & Karpf, A.
(1995) Theoretical relevance of Pre- and Protomorphology in language acquisition. In G. Booij & J. van Marle (Eds.), Yearbook of morphology (pp. 99–122). Kluwer.Google Scholar
Dressler, W. U., Ketrez, F. N., & Kilani-Schoch, M.
(Eds.) (2017) Nominal compound acquisition. John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dressler, W. U., Lettner, L. E., & Korecky-Kröll, K.
(2010) First language acquisition of compounds. In S. Scalise & I. Vogel (Eds.), Cross-Disciplinary Issues in Compounding (pp. 323–344). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dressler, W. U., Sommer-Lolei, S., Korecky-Kröll, K., Argus, R., Dabašinskienė, I., Kamandulytė-Merfeldienė, L., Ijäs, J. J., Kazakovskaya, V. V., Laalo, K., & Thomadaki, E.
(2019) First-language acquisition of synthetic compounds in Estonian, Finnish, German, Greek, Lithuanian, Russian and Saami. Morphology, 29(3), 409–429. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Edwards, D., & Goodwin, R.
(1986) Action words and pragmatic function in early language. In S. A. Kuczaj & M. D. Barrett (Eds.), The development of word meaning (pp. 257–273). Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eickelkamp, U.
(2010) Children and youth in Aboriginal Australia: An overview of the literature. Anthropological Forum, 20(2), 147–166. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ely, R., & Gleason, J. B.
(2006) I’m sorry I said that: Apologies in young children’s discourse. Journal of Child Language, 33(03), 599–620. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Engelmann, F., Granlund, S., Kolak, J., Szreder, M., Ambridge, B., Pine, J., Theakston, A., & Lieven, E.
(2019) How the input shapes the acquisition of verb morphology: Elicited production and computational modelling in two highly inflected languages. Cognitive Psychology, 110, 30–69. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S.
(1986) The development of requests. In R. L. Schiefelbusch (Ed.), Communicative competence: Acquisition and intervention (pp. 295–312). College Hills Press.Google Scholar
Evans, N.
(2004) Experiencer objects in Iwaidjan languages (Australia). In P. Bhaskararao & K. V. Subbarao (Eds.), Non-nominative subjeccts (pp. 169–192). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Evans, N., & Levinson, S. C.
(2009) The myth of language universals: Language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32(5), 429–448; discussion 448–494. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Evans, N., & Sasse, H. J.
(2002) Introduction: Problems of polysynthesis. In Problems of polysynthesis (pp. 1–13). Akademie Verlag. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Family, N.
(2009) Lighten up: The acquisition of light verb constructions in Persian. In J. Chandlee, M. Francini, S. Lord, & G.-M. Rheiner (Eds.), BUCLD 33: Proceedings of the 33rd annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 139–150). Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Fasoli, L., Maypilama, L., Lowell, A., Yunipingu, M., & Farmer, R.
(2018) We’re still being dragged to be white’: Learning from Yolŋu growing up their children in two worlds. In A. Pence & J. Harvel (Eds.), Pedagogies for diverse contexts (pp. 78–94). Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fietz, P.
(2008) Socialisation and the shaping of youth identity at Docker River. In G. Robinson, U. Eickelkamp, J. Goodnow, & I. Katz (Eds.), Contexts of child development: Culture, policy and intervention (pp. 49–58). Charles Darwin University Press.Google Scholar
Fikkert, P.
(1994) On the acquisition of prosodic structure (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Radboud University.Google Scholar
Forshaw, W.
(2011) A continuum of incorporation: Noun incorporation in Murrinh-Patha (Unpublished honours thesis). University of Melbourne.Google Scholar
(2014) Little kids, big paradigms: Murrinhpatha dual subject verbs (Poster). 13th International Congress for the Study of Child Language, Amsterdam.
(2016) Little kids, big verbs: The acquisition of Murrinhpatha bipartite stem verbs (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Melbourne. [URL]
Forshaw, W., Davidson, L., Kelly, B., Nordlinger, R., Wigglesworth, G., & Blythe, J.
(2017) The acquisition of Murrinhpatha. In M. Fortescue, M. Mithun, & N. Evans (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of polysynthesis. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Forshaw, W., Nordlinger, R., & Blythe, J.
(2012) Complexity in use: Implications for learning Murrinh-Patha. 43rd Annual Meeting of the Australian Linguistic Society.
Freudenthal, D., Pine, J., Jones, G., & Gobet, F.
(2015) Simulating the cross-linguistic pattern of optional infinitive errors in children’s declaratives and Wh- questions. Cognition, 143, 61–76. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gentner, D.
(2006) Why verbs are hard to learn. In K. Hirsh-Pasek & R. M. Golinkoff (Eds.) Action meets word: How children learn verbs (pp. 544–564). Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Georgalidou, M.
(2008) The contextual parameters of linguistic choice: Greek children’s preferences for the formation of directive speech acts. Journal of Pragmatics, 40(1), 72–94. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gerken, L.
(1987a) Function morphemes in young children’s speech perception and production (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Columbia University.Google Scholar
(1987b) Telegraphic speaking does not imply telegraphic listening. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 26, 48–55.Google Scholar
(1991) The metrical basis for children’s subjectless sentences. Journal of Memory and Language, 30(4), 431–451. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1996) Prosodic structure in young children’s language production. Language, 72(4), 683–712. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gerken, L., Landau, B., & Remez, R. E.
(1990) Function morphemes in young children’s speech perception and production. Developmental Psychology, 26(2), 204–216. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gerken, L., & McIntosh, B. J.
(1993) Interplay of function morphemes and prosody in early language. Developmental Psychology, 29(3), 448–457. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Granlund, S., Kolak, J., Vihman, V., Engelmann, F., Lieven, E. V. M., Pine, J. M., Theakston, A. L., & Ambridge, B.
(2019) Language-general and language-specific phenomena in the acquisition of inflectional noun morphology: A cross-linguistic elicited-production study of Polish, Finnish and Estonian. Journal of Memory and Language, 107, 169–194. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Green, I.
(1989) Marrithiyel: A language of the Daly River region of Australia’s Northern Territory (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Australian National University.Google Scholar
(2003) The genetic status of Murrinh-Patha. In N. Evans (Ed.), The non-Pama-Nyungan languages of Northern Australia (pp. 280–310). Pacific Linguistics.Google Scholar
Greif, E. B., & Gleason, J. B.
(1980) Hi, thanks, and goodbye: More routine information. Language in Society, 9(02), 159–166. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hamilton, A.
(1981) Nature and nurture: Aboriginal child-rearing in north-central Arnhem Land. Australian Institute of Aboriginal Studies.Google Scholar
Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M.
(2006) Action meets word: How children learn verbs. Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hoekstra, T., & Hyams, N.
(1998) Aspects of root infinitives. Lingua, 106(1–4), 81–112. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hollos, M., & Beeman, W.
(1978) The development of directives among Norwegian and Hungarian children: An example of communicative style in culture. Language in Society, 7(3), 345–355. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Smiley, P., & Charney, R.
(1983) The emergence of action categories in the child: Evidence from verb meaning. Psychological Review, 90, 72–93. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows (20.0)
(2011) (Computer software). IBM Corp.Google Scholar
Imai, M., Haryu, E., Okada, H., Lianjing, L., & Shigematsu, J.
(2006) Revisiting the noun-verb debate: A cross-linguistic comparison of novel noun and verb learning in English-, Japanese-, and Chinese-speaking children. In K. Hirsh-Pasek & R. M. Golinkoff (Eds.), Action meets word: How children learn verbs (pp. 450–476). Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Imedadze, N., & Tuite, K.
(1992) The acquisition of Georgian. In D. I. Slobin (Ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition (Vol. 3, pp. 39–109). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Katičić, A.
(2003) Early verb development in one Croatian-speaking child. In D. Bittner, W. U. Dressler, & M. Kilani-Schoch (Eds.), Development of verb inflection in first language acquisition (pp. 239–268). Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kelly, B., & Davidson, L.
(2015) “I talk … She copy me”: Carer language-learning beliefs in Murrinhpatha. The 46th Annual Conference of the Australian Linguistic Society, Western Sydney University.
Kelly, B., Forshaw, W., Nordlinger, R., & Wigglesworth, G.
(2015) Linguistic diversity in first language acquisition research: Moving beyond the challenges. First Language, 35(4–5), 286–304. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kelly, B., Nordlinger, R., & Wigglesworth, G.
(2009) Indigenous perspectives on the vitality of Murrinh-Patha. In Y. Treis & R. D. Busser (Eds.), Selected Papers from the 2009 conference of the Australian Linguistic Society (pp. 1–21).Google Scholar
Kelly, B., Wigglesworth, G., Nordlinger, R., & Blythe, J.
(2014) The acquisition of polysynthetic languages. Language and Linguistics Compass, 8(2), 51–64. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kidd, E., & Donnelly, S.
(2020) Individual differences in first language acquisition. Annual Review of Linguistics, 6(1), 319–340. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kilani-Schoch, M.
(2003) Early verb inflection in French: An investigation of two corpora. In D. Bittner, W. U. Dressler, & M. Kilani-Schoch (Eds.), Development of verb inflection in first language acquisition: A cross-linguistic perspective (pp. 269–296). Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kim, J., Pinker, S., Prince, A., & Prasada, S.
(1991) Why no mere mortal has ever flown out to center field. Cognitive Science, 15, 173–218. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Koch, H., & Nordlinger, R.
(2014) The languages of Australia in linguistic research: Context and issues. In H. Koch & R. Nordlinger (Eds.), The languages and linguistics of Australia: A comprehensive guide (pp. 3–21). Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
König, E., & Siemund, P.
(2007) Speech act distinctions in grammar. In T. Shopen (Ed.), Language typology and syntactic description. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Köpcke, K. M.
(1998) The acquisition of plural marking in English and German revisited: Schemata versus rules. Journal of Child Language, 25(2), 293–319. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krajewski, G., Lieven, E., & Theakston, A. L.
(2012) Productivity of a Polish child’s inflectional noun morphology: A naturalistic study. Morphology, 22(1), 9–34. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krott, A., & Nicoladis, E.
(2005) Large constituent families help children parse compounds. Journal of Child Language, 32(01), 139–158. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Küntay, A. C., Nakamura, N., & Ateş Şen, B.
(2014) Crosslinguistic and crosscultural approaches to pragmatic development. In D. Matthews (Ed.), Pragmatic development in first language acquisition (pp. 317–341). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Küntay, A. C., & Slobin, D. I.
(1996) Listening to a Turkish mother: Some puzzles for acquisition. In Social interaction, social context, and language (pp. 265–286). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Laaha, S., & Gillis, S.
(Eds.) (2007) Typological perspectives on the acquisition of noun and verb morphology. University of Antwerp.Google Scholar
Laalo, K.
(2003) Early verb development in Finnish: A preliminary approach to miniparadigms. In D. Bittner, W. U. Dressler, & M. Kilani-Schoch (Eds.), Development of verb inflection in first language acquisition: A cross-linguistic perspective (pp. 323–350). Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leonard, W. Y.
(2018) Reflections on (de)colonialism in language documentation. In B. McDonnell, A. Berez-Kroeker, & G. Holton (Eds.), Reflections on language documentation 20 years after Himmelmann 1998 (pp. 1–8). University of Hawai’i Press. Retrieved on 14 June 2021 from [URL]
Levinson, S. C.
(1983) Pragmatics. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lieven, E.
(2010) Input and first language acquisition: Evaluating the role of frequency. Lingua, 120(11), 2546–2556. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lieven, E., & Stoll, S.
(2010) Language. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), The handbook of cross-cultural developmental science (pp. 143–160). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Lignos, C., & Yang, C.
(2016) Morphology and language acquisition. In A. Hippisley & G. Stump (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of morphology. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lleó, C.
(2006) The acquisition of prosodic word structures in Spanish by monolingual and Spanish-German bilingual children. Language & Speech, 49(2), 205–229. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lleó, C., & Demuth, K.
(1999) Prosodic constraints on the emergence of grammatical morphemes: Crosslinguistic evidence from Germanic and Romance languages. In A. Greenhill, H. Littlefield, & C. Tano (Eds.), BUCLD 23: Proceedings of the 23rd annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 407–418). Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Long, C.
(2010) Apology in Japanese gratitude situations: The negotiation of interlocutor role-relations. Journal of Pragmatics, 42(4), 1060–1075. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B.
(1978) The acquisition of morphophonology. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 43(1/2), 1–123. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Malvern, D., Richards, B., Chipere, N., & Duran, P.
(2004) Lexical diversity and language development: Quantification and assessment. Palgrave Macmillan. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mansfield, J.
(2013) The social organisation of Wadeye’s heavy metal mobs. The Australian Journal of Anthropology, 24(2), 148–165. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014) Polysynthetic sociolinguistics: The language and culture of Murrinh Patha youth (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Australian National University.Google Scholar
(2015) Morphotactic variation, prosodic domains and the changing structure of the Murrinhpatha verb. Asia-Pacific Language Variation, 1(2), 163–189. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016a) Borrowed verbs and the expansion of light verb phrases in Murrinhpatha. In F. Meakins & C. O’Shannessy (Eds.), Loss and renewal: Australian Indigneous languages since colonisation (pp. 397–424). Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016b) Intersecting formatives and inflectional predictability: How do speakers and learners predict the correct form of Murrinhpatha verbs? Word Structure, 9(2), 183–214. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017) Prosodic words in cyclic derivation: The strange case of Murrinhpatha compound verbs. Morphology, 27(3), 359–382. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2019a) Murrinhpatha morphology and phonology. De Gruyter Mouton. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2019b) Epistemic authority and sociolinguistic stance in an Australian Aboriginal language. Open Linguistics, 5, 25–48. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mansfield, J., & Nordlinger, R.
(2020) Demorphologization and deepening complexity in Murrinhpatha. In P. Arkadiev & F. Gardani (Eds.), The complexities of morphology (pp. 52–80). Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marchman, V. A.
(1997) Children’s productivity in the English past tense: The role of frequency, phonology, and neighborhood structure. Cognitive Science, 21(3), 283–304. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marcus, G. F.
(2000) Children’s overregularization and its implications for cognition. In P. Broeder & J. Murre (Eds.), Models of language acquisition (pp. 154–176). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Martin, D. F.
(1993) Autonomy and relatedness: An ethnography of Wik people at Aurukun, Western Cape York Peninsula (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Australian National University.Google Scholar
Mateo Pedro, P.
Matthews, D.
McClelland, J. L., & Patterson
(2002) Rules or connections in past-tense inflections: What does the evidence rule out? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(11), 465–472. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McGregor, W.
(2002) Verb classification in Australian languages. Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014) Language contact varieties. In H. Koch & R. Nordlinger (Eds.), The languages and linguistics of Australia: A comprehensive guide (pp. 365–416). Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mellenius, I.
(1997) The acquisition of nominal compounding in Swedish. Lund University. Retrived on 14 June 2021 from [URL]
Minitab 17 Statistical Software
(2010) Minitab Inc.Google Scholar
Mirković, J., Seidenberg, M. S., & Joanisse, M. F.
(2011) Rules versus statistics: Insights from a highly inflected language. Cognitive Science, 35(4), 638–681. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mithun, M.
(1989) The acquisition of polysynthesis. Journal of Child Language, 16, 285–312. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2010) The search for regularity in irregularity: Defectiveness and its implications for our knowledge of words. In M. Baerman, G. Corbett, & D. Brown (Eds.), Defective paradigms: Missing forms and what they tell us (pp. 125–149). British Academy and Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morris, P. S., Leach, A. J., Silberberg, P., Mellon, G., Wilson, C., Hamilton, E., & Beissbarth, J.
(2005) Otitis media in young Aboriginal children from remote communities in Northern and Central Australia: A cross-sectional survey. BMC Paediatrics, 5, 27. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Moses, K.
(2009) How do dinosaurs hug in the Kimberley? The use of questions by Aboriginal caregivers and children in a Walmajarri community (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Melbourne.Google Scholar
Naigles, L. R., Hoff, E., & Vear, D.
(2009) Flexibility in early verb use: Evidence from a multiple-n diary study. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 74(2), 1–143.Google Scholar
Nakamura, K.
(1999) The development of requests in young Japanese children. 8th International Congress for the Study of Child Language, San Sebastian-Donostia, Spain.
National Indigenous Languages Report
(2020) Department of Infrastructure, Transport, Regional Development and Communications. Retrieved on 14 June 2021 from [URL]
Nespor, M., & Vogel, I.
(1986) Prosodic phonology. Foris.Google Scholar
Nganbe, T., & McCormack, D.
(2009) Education in Wadeye and the Thamarrurr region: Challenges & responsibilities across the generations (Unpublished manuscript).Google Scholar
Nicoladis, E.
(2002) What’s the difference between ‘toilet paper’ and ‘paper toilet’? French-English bilingual children’s crosslinguistic transfer in compound nouns. Journal of Child Language, 29(04), 843–863. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007) Preschool children’s acquisition of compounds. In G. Libben & G. Jarema (Eds.), The representation and processing of compound words (pp. 96–124). Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nicoladis, E., & Krott, A.
(2007) Word family size and French-speaking children’s segmentation of existing compounds. Language Learning, 57(2), 201–228. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ninio, A.
(1999a) Model learning in syntactic development: Intransitive verbs. International Journal of Bilingualism, 3(2–3), 111–130. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1999b) Pathbreaking verbs in syntactic development and the question of prototypical transitivity. Journal of Child Language, 26(3), 619–653. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nordlinger, R.
(2010) Verbal morphology in Murrinh-Patha: Evidence for templates. Morphology, 20(2), 321–341. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011) Transitivity in Murrinh-Patha. Studies in Language, 35(3), 702–734. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012a) Complex predicates in Murrinh-Patha: Problems for analysis. 43rd Annual Meeting of the Australian Linguistic Society.
(2012b) Number marking in the Daly languages. In M. Butt & T. H. King (Eds.), Proceedings of the LFG12 Conference (pp. 422–439). CSLI.Google Scholar
(2015) Inflection in Murrinh-Patha. In M. Baerman (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of inflection. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
(2017) The languages of the Daly River region (Northern Australia). In M. Fortescue, M. Mithun, & N. Evans (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of polysynthesis. Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nordlinger, R., & Caudal, P.
(2012) The tense, aspect and modality system in Murrinh-Patha. Australian Journal of Linguistics, 32(1), 73–113. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ochs, E.
(1988) Culture and language development: Language acquisition and language socialization in a Samoan village. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ogura, T., Dale, P. S., Yamashita, Y., Murase, T., & Mahieu, A.
(2006) The use of nouns and verbs by Japanese children and their caregivers in book-reading and toy-playing contexts. Journal of Child Language, 33(1), 1–29. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
O’Shannessy, C.
(2006) Language contact and children’s bilingual acquisition: Learning a mixed language and Warlpiri in northern Australia (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Sydney.Google Scholar
Peters, A. M.
(1985) Language segmentation: Operating principles for the perception and analysis of language. In D. I. Slobin (Ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition (Vol. 2, pp. 1029–1067). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
(2001) Filler syllables: What is their status in emerging grammar? Journal of Child Language, 28(1), 229–242. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pfeiler, B.
(2003) Early acquisition of the verbal complex in Yucatec Maya. In D. Bittner, W. U. Dressler, & M. Kilani-Schoch (Eds.), Development of verb inflection in first language acquisition: A cross-linguistic perspective (pp. 379–399). Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S.
(1984) Language learnability and language development. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
(1989) Review of Slobin, D. I., Crosslinguistic evidence for the language-making capacity. Journal of Child Language, 16, 456–460.Google Scholar
(2006) Whatever happened to the past tense debate? In Wondering at the natural fecundity of things: Essays in honor of Alan Prince (pp. 221–238). Linguistics Research Center.Google Scholar
Pinker, S., & Prince, A.
(1991) Regular and irregular morphology and the psychological status of rules of grammar. Proceedings of the Seventeenth Annual Meeting of the Berkley Linguistics Society: General Session and Parasession on the Grammar of Event Structure, 230–251. BLS. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S., & Ullman, M.
(2002) The past and future of the past tense. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(11), 456–463. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Poeppel, D., & Wexler, K.
(1993) The full competence hypothesis of clause structure in early German. Language, 69(1), 1–33. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Prasada, S., & Pinker, S.
(1993) Generalisation of regular and irregular morphological patterns. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8(1), 1–56. eric. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pye, Bro. J.
(1972) The Port Keats story. Colemans.Google Scholar
Pye, C.
(1980) The acquisition of grammatical morphemes in Quiche Mayan (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Pittsburgh.Google Scholar
(1983) Mayan telegraphese: Intonational determinants of inflectional development in Quiche Mayan. Language, 59, 583–604. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pye, C., Pfeiler, B., de León, L., Brown, P., & Mateo Pedro, P.
(2007) Roots or edges? Explaining variation in children’s early verb forms across five Mayan languages. In B. Pfeiler (Ed.), Learning indigenous languages: Child language acquisition in Mesoamerica (pp. 15–46). Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Reid, N. J.
(2011) Ngan’gityemerri: A language of the Daly River region, Northern Territory of Australia. Lincom.Google Scholar
Rhydwen, M.
(2003) Kriol: The creation of a written language and a tool of colonisation. In M. Walsh & C. Yallop (Eds.), Language and Culture in Aboriginal Australia (pp. 155–168). Aboriginal Studies Press.Google Scholar
Rizzi, L.
(1993) Some notes on linguistic theory and language development: The case of root infinitives. Language Acquisition, 3(4), 371–393. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roche, G.
(2020) Abandoning endangered languages: Ethical loneliness, language oppression, and social justice. American Anthropologist, 122(1), 164–169. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Romaine, S.
(2017) Language endangerment and language death. Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rose, Y., & Brittain, J.
(2011) Grammar matters: Evidence from phonological and morphological development in Northern East Cree. In M. Pirvulescu (Ed.) Selected proceedings of the 4th conference on Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition North America (pp. 193–208). Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Rumsey, A.
(1993) Language and territoriality in Aboriginal Australia. In Language and culture in Aboriginal Australia (pp. 191–206). Aboriginal Studies Press.Google Scholar
Ryckebusch, C., & Marcos, H.
(2004) Speech acts, social context and parent-toddler play between the ages of 1;5 and 2;3. Journal of Pragmatics, 36(5), 883–897. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sadock, J. M., & Zwicky, A. M.
(1985) Speech act distinctions in syntax. In T. Shopen (Ed.), Language typology and syntactic description (pp. 155–196). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Salustri, M., & Hyams, N.
(2003) Is there an analogue to the RI stage in the null subject languages? In B. Beachley, A. Brown, & F. Conlin (Eds.), BUCLD 27: Proceedings of the 27th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, 692–703. Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
(2006) Looking for the universal core of the RI stage. In V. Torrens & L. Escobar (Eds.), The Acquisition of Syntax in Romance Languages (pp. 159–182). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sandefur, J.
(1986) Kriol of North Australia: A language coming of age. SIL.Google Scholar
Savičiūtė, E., Ambridge, B., & Pine, J. M.
(2018) The roles of word-form frequency and phonological neighbourhood density in the acquisition of Lithuanian noun morphology. Journal of Child Language, 45(3), 641–672. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schultze-Berndt, E.
(2000) Simple and complex verbs in Jaminjung: A study of event categorisation in an Australian language. MPI. Retrieved on 14 June from [URL]
Schütze, C. T., & Wexler, K.
(1996) Subject case licensing and English root infinitives. In A. Stringfellow, D. Cahana-Amitay, E. Hughes, & A. Zukowski (Eds.), BUCLD 20: Proceedings of the 20th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 670–681). Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Searle, J. R.
(1969) Speech acts: An essay in the philosophy of language. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1976) A classification of illocutionary acts. Language in Society, 5(1), 1–23. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Seiss, M.
(2012) A rule-based morphological analyzer for Murrinh-Patha. In N. Calzolari, K. Choukri, T. Declerck, M. Uğur Doğan, B. Maegaard, J. Mariani, J. Odijk, & S. Piperidis (Eds.), Proceedings of the Eighth International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation (pp. 751–758). European Language Resources Association.Google Scholar
Selkirk, E.
(1984) Phonology and syntax: The relation between sound and structure. The MIT Press.Google Scholar
(1986) On derived domains in sentence phonology. Phonology Yearbook, 3, 371–405. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shannon, C. E.
(1948) A mathematical theory of communication. Bell System Technical Journal, 27(3), 379–423. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Simpson, J., & Wigglesworth, G.
(Eds.) (2008) Children’s language and multilingualism: Indigenous language use at home and school. Bloomsbury Academic.Google Scholar
Singer, R.
(2020) Why don’t linguists talk about politics? Moving forward on language endangerment – together. American Anthropologist, 122(1), 177–178. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Skarabela, B., & Allen, S. E. M.
(2004) The contexts of non-affixal verb forms in child Inuktitut. In A. Brugos, L. Micciulla, & C. E. Smith, (Eds.), BUCLD 28: Proceedings of the 28th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development. Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I.
(1982) Universal and particular in the acquisition of language. In L. Gleitman & E. Wanner (Eds.), Language acquisition: The state of the art (pp. 128–170). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
(1985a) Crosslinguistic evidence for the language-making capacity. In D. I. Slobin (Ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, Vol. 2: Theoretical issues (pp. 1157–1256). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
(1985b) Introduction: Why study language acquisition crosslinguistically? In D. I. Slobin (Ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, Vol. 1: The data (pp. 3–24). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
(Ed.) (1985c) The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, Vol. 2: Theoretical issues. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
(Ed.) (1992) The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, Vol. 3. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
(Ed.) (1997a) The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, Vol. 4. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
(Ed.) (1997b) The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, Vol. 5: Expanding the contexts. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Sloetjes, H., & Wittenberg, P.
(2008) Annotation by category – ELAN and ISO DCR. In Proceedings of the 6th International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation. European Language Resources Association.Google Scholar
Smiley, P., & Huttenlocher, J.
(1995) Conceptual development and the child’s early words for events, objects, and persons. In M. Tomasello & W. E. Merriman (Eds.), Beyond names for things: Young children’s acquisition of verbs (pp. 21–61). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Stoll, S.
(2015) Inflectional morphology in language acquisition. In M. Baerman (Ed.), The oxford handbook of inflection (pp. 351–374). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Stoll, S., & Bickel, B.
(2013) Capturing diversity in language acquisition research. In B. Bickel, L. A. Grenoble, D. A. Peterson, & A. Timberlake (Eds.), Language typology and historical contingency: In honor of Johanna Nichols (pp. 1–22). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stoll, S., Bickel, B., Lieven, E., Paudyal, N. P., Banjade, G., Bhatta, T. N., Gaenszle, M., Pettigrew, J., Rai, I. P., Rai, M., & Rai, N. K.
(2012) Nouns and verbs in Chintang: Children’s usage and surrounding adult speech. Journal of Child Language, 39(2), 284–321. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stoll, S., Bickel, B., & Mažara, J.
(2017) The Acquisition of polysynthetic verb forms in Chintang. In M. Fortescue, M. Mithun, & N. Evans (Eds.) The Oxford handbook of polysynthesis. Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stoll, S., & Lieven, E.
(2013) Studying language acquisition cross-linguistically. In H. Winskel & P. Padakannaya (Eds.), South and Southeast Asian psycholinguistics (pp. 19–35). Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Street, C.
(1987) Introduction to the language and culture of the Murrinh-Patha. SIL.Google Scholar
(2012) Murrinhpatha to English dictionary (July 2012 revision). Wadeye Literature Production Centre.Google Scholar
Street, C., & Mollinjin, G. P.
(1981) The phonology of Murinbata. Work Papers of SIL-AAB, 5(A), 183–244.Google Scholar
Stump, G., & Finkel, R. A.
(2013) Morphological typology: From word to paradigm. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Swift, M. D., & Allen, S. E. M.
(2002) Contexts of verbal inflection dropping in Inuktitut child speech. In B. Skarabela, S. Fish, & A. H.-J. Do (Eds.), BUCLD 26: Proceedings of the 26th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 689–700). Cascadilla.Google Scholar
Taylor, J.
(2010) Demography as destiny: Schooling, work and Aboriginal population change at Wadeye. CAEPR Working Paper, 64.Google Scholar
Taylor, J., & Ivory, B.
(2013) Back on TRAAC: Community-led social change in the Thamarrurr region: A Report to the Board of the Thamarrurr Regional Authority Aboriginal Corporation.Google Scholar
Terry, K.
(2010) The emergence of intransitive verb inflection in Northern East Cree: A case study (Unpublished masters thesis). Memorial University of Newfoundland.Google Scholar
Theakston, A. L., Lieven, E. V. M., Pine, J. M., & Rowland, C. F.
(2004) Semantic generality, input frequency and the acquisition of syntax. Journal of Child Language, 31(1), 61–99. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Merriman, W. E.
(Eds.) (1995) Beyond names for things: Young children’s acquisition of verbs. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Stahl, D.
(2004) Sampling children’s spontaneous speech: How much is enough? Journal of Child Language, 31(1), 101–121. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tweedie, F. J., & Baayen, R. H.
(1998) How variable may a constant be? Measures of lexical richness in perspective. Computers and the Humanities, 32(5), 323–352. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Veneziano, E., & Sinclair, H.
(2000) The changing status of ‘filler syllables’ on the way to grammatical morphemes. Journal of Child Language, 27(3), 461–500. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wadeye – Thamarrurr Development Corporation
n.d.). Retrieved on 14 April 2020 from [URL]
Wadrill, V., Wavehill, B., Dodd, T. & Meakins, F.
(2019) Karu: Growing up Gurindji. Spinifex Press.Google Scholar
Walsh, M.
(1995) Body parts in Murrinh-Patha: Incorporation, grammar and metaphor. In H. Chappell & W. McGregor (Eds.), The grammar of inalienability: A typological perspective on body part terms and the part-whole relation (pp. 327–380). Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
(1997) Noun classes, nominal classification and generics in Murrinhpatha. In Nominal classification in Aboriginal Australia (Vol. 37, pp. 255–292). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011) The Murinypata language of north-west Australia. Lincom.Google Scholar
Walsh, M. J.
(1987) The impersonal verb construction in Australian languages. In R. Steele & T. Threadgold (Eds.), Language Topics: Essays in honour of Michael Halliday (pp. 425–438). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ward, T.
(2018) Kanhi-ka da i murrinh ngarra kardu Thamarrurr numi da pubemkardurdu: The countries and languages of peoples gathered together in Wadeye. Batchelor Institute Press.Google Scholar
Wells, G., & French, P.
(1980) Language in the transition from home to school: Final report to the Nuffield Foundation. Centre for the Study of Language and Communication, University of Bristol.Google Scholar
Weverink, M.
(1989) The subject in relation to inflection in child language (Unpublished masters thesis). University of Utrecht.Google Scholar
Wexler, K.
(1994) Optinal infinitives, head movement and economy of derivation. In N. Hornstein & D. Lightfoot (Eds.), Verb movement (pp. 305–350). Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1998) Very early parameter setting and the unique checking constraint: A new explanation of the optional infinitive stage. 106(1–4), 23–79.Google Scholar
Williams, C. J., & Jacobs, A. M.
(2009) The impact of otitis media on cognitive and educational outcomes. Medical Journal of Australia, 191(9), 69–72. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wood, M., Lantjin, D. M., Tipiloura, P. R., Nemarluk, M. M., & Tchinburur, T. S.
(2020) Murrinh niyith-niyith pumawathangime purru ‘We make stories together’: Place-based literacy in the Murrinhpatha classroom. 2020 Conference of the Australian Linguistic Society, Online.
Xanthos, A., & Gillis, S.
(2010) Quantifying the development of inflectional diversity. First Language, 30(2), 175–198. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Xanthos, A., Laaha, S., Gillis, S., Stephany, U., Aksu-Koc, A., Christofidou, A., Gagarina, N., Hrzica, G., Ketrez, F. N., Kilani-Schoch, M., Korecky-Kroll, K., Kovacevic, M., Laalo, K., Palmovic, M., Pfeiler, B., Voeikova, M. D., & Dressler, W. U.
(2011) On the role of morphological richness in the early development of noun and verb inflection. First Language, 31(4), 461–479. DOI logoGoogle Scholar