Tonal intelligibility within a paragraph: Analyzing Polish Mandarin learners’ tone production

Man-Ni Chu, Ewa ZajdlerHui-Wen Lin
Abstract

This study considers the performance of tone pronunciation and morpheme intelligibility by L2 Polish Mandarin learners. A mixed-effects ordinal regression model and Tukey’s HSD multiple test were used to examine the start-point, end-point, of syllables as a means of assessing their intelligibility by L2 Polish learners regarding the tones with the sequence T0, T1, T2, T4 > T3.and T0, T1> T2, T4 > T3, respectively. Students’ tone pronunciation at different stages of learning was compared, i.e. first at less than A1 level of proficiency and then after achieving an A2 level of proficiency according to the Common European Framework of Reference for Languages (CEFR), with the interval of a 22-week intensive Chinese class. The results suggest that in the case of the steady pitch produced for T0 and T1, L2 Polish learners found it easy to control the vibration speed of the vocal folds over two stages. Additionally, progress in successfully producing T4 was exhibited with improving control over the individual tonal register span, implying that competent tonal production develops with time. It remains unclear whether the reason why the low-range T3 is acquired last is down to the habitually high pitched range of spoken Polish, or a more universally observable phenomenon where T3 is liable to be confused with other tones in production.

Keywords:
Publication history
Table of contents

Chinese tones are usually analyzed by their pitch register and pitch contour, where the former denotes the speed of vibration of the vocal folds and the latter indicates the level of control over the speed of vibration of the vocal folds within a syllable and the production of a tone over time. Concerning pitch contour, the fundamental phonation frequency range during speech sees the voice move up or down within the register by 4 to 6 semitones (Whalen & Levitt, 1995, pp. 351–353). Such articulation seems complex to non-tonal language speakers, but simple to tonal language speakers.

Full-text access is restricted to subscribers. Log in to obtain additional credentials. For subscription information see Subscription & Price. Direct PDF access to this article can be purchased through our e-platform.

References

Braun, B., Galts, T., & Kabak, B.
(2014) Lexical encoding of L2 tones: The role of L1 stress, pitch accent and intonation. Second Language Research, 30 (3), 323–350. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Broselow, E., Hurtig, R., & Ringen, C.
(1987) The perception of second language prosody. In G. Ioup & S. Weinberger (Eds.), Interlanguage phonology: The acquisition of second language sound system (pp. 350–362). Rowley.Google Scholar
Cao, Jian-fen
(1986) 普通話輕聲音節特性分析 [Analysis of the characteristics of Mandarin neutral tones]. 應用聲學 [Applied Acoustics], 4 , 1–6.Google Scholar
Chao, Y. R.
(1922) A system of romanization of the national language. The Chinese Student’s Monthly 17, 3 , 194–200.Google Scholar
Chen, J. Y.
(1999) The representation and processing of tone in Mandarin Chinese: Evidence from slips of the tongue. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20 (2), 289–301. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chen, Q.
(1997) Toward a sequential approach for tonal error analysis. Journal Chinese Teacher Association, 32 , 21–39.Google Scholar
Deng, Dan
(2019) 普通話輕聲感知特性再分析 [Reanalysis of the perceptual characteristics of neutral tone in Mandarin]. 語言文字應用 [Language and Character Application] 2019(1), 66–75.Google Scholar
Deng, Dan, & Zhu, Lin
(2019) 二語學習者漢語普通話輕聲的感知與產出 [Second language learners’ perception and production of neutral tones in Mandarin Chinese]. 語言教學與研究 [Language Teaching and Research] 2019(5), 13–24.Google Scholar
Ding, Hongwei, R. Hoffmann, O. Jokisch
(2011) An Investigation of Tone Perception and Production in German Learners of Mandarin. Archives of Acoustics. 36. 3: 509–518.Google Scholar
Domahs, U., Knaus, J., Orzechowska, P., & Wiese, R.
(2012) Stress “deafness” in a language with fixed word stress: An ERP study on Polish. Frontiers in Psychology, 3 , 439. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gandour, J.
(1977) Counterfeit tones in the speech of Southern Thai bidialectals. Lingua, 41 (2), 125–143. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gandour, J., Dardarananda, R., Vibulsreth, S., & Buckingham, H.
(1982) Case study of a Thai transcortical motor aphasic. Language and Speech, 25 , 127–150. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gandour, J., Ponglorpisit, S., & Dardarananda, R.
(1992) Tonal disturbances in Thai after brain damage. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 7, 133–145. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Guo, Jun-hong, Liu, Hui-mei, Huang, Gui-jun, Wang, Xiao-chuan, & Cao, Fengming
(2010) 修改語音基頻曲線與低通濾波處理影響聽障學生國語聲調聽辨成效之研究 [A effect study on the modification of the fundamental frequency curve and low-pass filtering of hearing-impaired students in Mandarin tones]. 特殊教育研究學 [Research. Journal of Special Education Research], 35 (3), 77–101Google Scholar
Guo, L., & Tao, L.
(2008, April). Tone production in Mandarin Chinese by American students: A case study. In Proceedings of the 20th North American Conference on Chinese Linguistics (NACCL-20) (Vol. 1, pp. 123–138).Google Scholar
Gao, M.
(2016, November). Perception of lexical tones by Swedish learners of Mandarin. In Proceedings of the joint workshop on NLP for Computer Assisted Language Learning and NLP for Language Acquisition (pp. 33–40).Google Scholar
Hallé, P. A., Chang, Y. C., & Best, C. T.
(2004) Identification and discrimination of Mandarin Chinese tones by Mandarin Chinese vs. French listeners. Journal of phonetics, 32 (3), 395–421. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hojan, E., & Skrodzka, E.
(2005) Audiologiczne aspektu akustyki i psychoakustyki. Audiologia kliniczna. Łódź, 97–106. Retrievd 24 May 2022 from http://​livesound​.pl​/tutoriale​/artykuly​/4629​-zrozumialosc​-mowy
Hombert, J. M.
(1975, September). The perception of contour tones. In Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society (Vol. 1, pp. 221–232).Google Scholar
Huang, Jing-wen & Shi, Feng
(2019) 漢語輕聲音節韻律表現的多樣性 [The prosodic diversity of Chinese neutral tone]. 語言文字應用 [Language and Character Application] 2019(1):76–85.Google Scholar
Hsu, Hui-Chuan
(1989) Phonological acquisition of Taiwanese: A longitudinal case study (Unpublished master’s thesis). National Tsing Hua University, Taiwan
Leather, J.
(1990) Perceptual and productive learning of Chinese lexical tone by Dutch and English speakers. New Sounds, 90 , 72–97.Google Scholar
Li, C. N., & Thompson, S. A.
(1977) The acquisition of tone in Mandarin-speaking children. Journal of Child Language, 4 (2), 185–199. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ling, W., & Grüter, T.
(2022) From sounds to words: The relation between phonological and lexical processing of tone in L2 Mandarin. Second Language Research, 38 (2), 289–313. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miracle, W. C.
(1989) Tone production of American students of Chinese: A preliminary acoustic study. Journal of the Chinese Language Teachers Association, 24 (3), 49–65.Google Scholar
Naesear, M. A., & S. Chan
(1980) Case study of a Chinese aphasic with the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Exam. Neuropsychology, 18 , 389–410. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Newlin-Łukowicz, L.
(2012) Polish stress: Looking for phonetic evidence of a bidirectional system. Phonology, 29 (2), 271–329. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Packard, Jerome. L.
(1986) Tone production deficits in non-fluent aphasic Chinese speech. Brain and Language, 29 , 212–223. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Peperkamp, S., & Dupoux, E.
(2002) A typological study of stress ‘deafness’. Laboratory Phonology, 7 (2000), 203–240.Google Scholar
Peperkamp, S., Vendelin, I., & Dupoux, E.
(2010) Perception of predictable stress: A cross-linguistic investigation. Journal of Phonetics, 38 (3), 422–430. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shen, X. S.
(1989) Toward a register approach in teaching Mandarin tones. Journal of the Chinese Language Teachers Association, 24 (3), 27–47.Google Scholar
Tsao, F. M.
(2008) The effect of acoustical similarity on lexical-tone perception of one-year-old Mandarin-learning infants. Chinese Journal of Psychology, 50 (2), 111–124.Google Scholar
Wan, I. P.
(1999) Mandarin phonology: Evidence from speech errors (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). SUNY-Buffalo.
Wan, I. P., & Jaeger, J.
(1998) Speech errors and the representation of tone in Mandarin Chinese. Phonology, 15 (3), 417–461. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wan, I. P.
(2007) On the phonological organization of Mandarin tones. Lingua, 117 (10), 1715–1738. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2021) Interlanguage tone patterns in Thai pre-school children: A preliminary corpus analysis. Taiwan Journal of Chinese as a Second Language, 22 , 1–33.Google Scholar
Wang, Y., Spence, M. M., Jongman, A., & Sereno, J. A.
(1999) Training American listeners to perceive Mandarin tones. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 106 (6), 3649–3658. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wang, Y., Jongman, A., & Sereno, J. A.
(2003) Acoustic and perceptual evaluation of Mandarin tone productions before and after perceptual training. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 113 (2), 1033–1043. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wang, Y., Sereno, J., & Jongman, A.
(2006) L2 acquisition and processing of Mandarin tones. In P. Li, L. Tan, E. Bates, & O. Tzeng (eds.), The handbook of East Asian psycholinguistics (pp. 250–256). Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Whalen, D. H., A. G. Levitt
(1995) The universality of intrinsic f0 of vowels. Journal of phonetics. 23: 349–366.Google Scholar
Wu, X., & Lin, H.
(2008) Perception of Mandarin tones by Mandarin and English listeners. Journal of Chinese Language and Computing, 18 (4), 175–187.Google Scholar
Yue-Hashimoto, A. O.
(1986) Shengdiao jiaofa de Shangque. Diyijie Guoji Hanyu Jiaoxue Taolunhui Lunwenxuan, 229–235.Google Scholar
Zajdler, E., Marcin Jacoby, Qian Li-Piszczek
(2008) Współczesny język chiński. Mówić i pisać po chińsku (1), Wydawnictwo Akademickie Dialog, Warszawa, pp. 287.Google Scholar
Zajdler, E., & A. Stal
(2013) Umiejętności receptywne a realizacja tonemów języka chińskiego [ang. Perception skills and tonemes production in Chinese]. Lingwistyka Stosowana [Applied Linguistics], 8 , 153–164.Google Scholar
Zajdler, E., & Chu, M.
(2019) How Polish students develop Mandarin pronunciation through intensive training. Applied Linguistics Papers 26 (2), 103–115.Google Scholar