Attention and language

Maxim I. Stamenov
Table of contents

The topic of attention in relation to language has the potential for uniting the efforts of linguists and cognitive psychologists. Our presentation will be oriented toward their relationship at the level of simple sentence planning and comprehension, since this is a critical level of modeling (cf. Meyer et al. 2006). Modeling the dynamics of focusing in discourse processing (cf. Kendon 1992; Stevenson, Crawley & Kleinman 1994; Chafe 1994; van Dijk 1997; Tomlin 1997; Stamenov & Andonova 1998; Myachykov & Posner 2005) should have as its basis the relationships between the structure of attention, awareness and sentence structure. We will consider the functioning of attention in normal language use only, although there is also an extensive literature on different issues of the pathology of attention in language use (cf. Widlöcher & Hardy-Bayle 1989; Bates et al. 1995; Chapman & Ulatowska 1997; Meyer et al. 2006 for further orientation).

Full-text access is restricted to subscribers. Log in to obtain additional credentials. For subscription information see Subscription & Price.

References

Baddeley, A.
1989The uses of working memory. In P.R. Solomon et al. (eds.): 107–123. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1995Working memory. In M. Gazzaniga (ed.): The cognitive neurosciences: 755–764. MIT Press. DOI logo  BoPGoogle Scholar
2000The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4: 417–423. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, E., Ch Harris, V. Marchman, B. Wulfeck & M. Kritchevsky
1995Production of complex syntax in normal ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. Language and Cognitive Processes 10: 487–539. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Burzio, L.
1986Italian syntax. Reidel. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chafe, W.
1994Discourse, consciousness, and time. University of Chicago Press.  BoPGoogle Scholar
Chapman, S. & H. Ulatowska
1997Discourse in Dementia: Consideration of consciousness. In M.I. Stamenov (ed.): 155–188. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N.
1990Accessibility ‘in principle’. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13: 600–601. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1995The minimalist program. MIT Press. Google Scholar
2005Three factors in language design. Linguistic Inquiry 36: 1–22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cowan, N.
2001The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24: 87–114. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fauconnier, G. & M. Turner
2002The way we think: Conceptual blending and the mind’s hidden complexities. Basic Books.  BoPGoogle Scholar
Fernandez-Duque, D., G. Grossi, I.M. Thornton & H.J. Neville
2003Representation of change: Separate electrophysiological markers of attention, awareness, and implicit processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 15: 491–507. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jackendoff, R.
1990Semantic structures. MIT Press. Google Scholar
2002Foundations of language: Brain, meaning, grammar, evolution. Oxford University Press.  BoP DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jonides, J. & E.E. Smith
1997The architecture of working memory. In M. Rugg (ed.) Cognitive neuroscience: 243–276. Psychology Press. Google Scholar
Kahneman, D.
1973Attention and effort. Academic Press. Google Scholar
Kendon, A.
1992The negotiation of context in face-to-face interaction. In A. Duranti & Ch Goodwin (eds.) Rethinking context: Language as an interactive phenomenon: 326–334. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Koch, C. & N. Tsuchiya
2006The relationship between attention and consciousness Part I. Paper presented at the, 10th annual meeting of the Association for the Scientific Study of Consciousness, June 2006, St. Anne College, Oxford.
Langacker, R.
1987Foundations of cognitive grammar. Stanford University Press.  BoPGoogle Scholar
1995Conceptual grouping and constituency in cognitive grammar. In Linguistic Society of Korea (ed.) Linguistics in the morning calm 3: 149–172. Hanshin. Google Scholar
Levinson, S.C.
1995Three levels of meaning. In F.R. Palmer (ed.)90–115. Grammar and meaning: Cambridge University Press. Google Scholar
Mandler, G.
1985Cognitive psychology: An essay in cognitive science. Lawrence Erlbaum. Google Scholar
1989Memory: Conscious and unconscious. In P.R. Solomon et al. (eds.): 84–106. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2002Consciousness recovered: Psychological functions and origins of conscious thought. John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meyer, A., L. Wheeldon & A. Krott
(eds.) 2006Automaticity and control in language processing. Psychology Press. Google Scholar
Myachykov, A. & M.I. Posner
2005Attention in language. In L. Itti, G. Rees & J. Tsotsos (eds.) Neurobiology of attention: 324–329. Academic Press/Elsevier. Google Scholar
Parasuraman, R.
(ed.) 1998The attentive brain. MIT Press. Google Scholar
Pashler, H.E.
1997The psychology of attention. MIT Press. Google Scholar
Radford, A.
1997Syntactic theory and the structure of English: A minimalist approach. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sanford, A. & S. Garrod
1994Selective processing in text understanding. In M. Gernsbacher (ed.) Handbook of psycholinguistics: 699–719. Academic Press. Google Scholar
Solomon, P.R., G.R. Goethals, C.M. Kelley & B.R. Stephens
(eds.) 1989Memory: Interdisciplinary approaches. Springer-Verlag. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stamenov, M.I.
1997aGrammar, meaning and consciousness: What sentence structure can tell us about the structure of consciousness. In M.I. Stamenov (ed.): 277–342. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(eds.) 1997bLanguage structure, discourse and the access to consciousness. John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stamenov, M.I. & E. Andonova
1998Lexical access and coreference processing in Bulgarian. In D. Hillert (ed.) Sentence processing: A cross-linguistic perspective: 167–181. Academic Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stevenson, R., R.A. Crawley & D. Kleinman
1994Thematic roles, focus and the representation of events. Language and Cognitive Processes 9: 519–548. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Talmy, L.
1988Force dynamics in language and cognition. Cognitive Science 12: 49–100. DOI logo  BoPGoogle Scholar
1996The windowing of attention in language. In M. Shibatani & S. Thompson (eds.) Grammatical Constructions: Their Form and Meaning: 235–287. Oxford University Press. Google Scholar
Tomlin, R.S.
1997Mapping conceptual representations into linguistic representations: The role of attention in grammar. In J. Nuyts & E. Pederson (eds.) Language and Conceptualization: 162–189. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Dijk, T.A.
1997Cognitive context models and discourse. In M.I. Stamenov (ed.): 189–226. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vecchi, T. & G. Bottini
(eds.) 2006Imagery and spatial cognition. Methods models and cognitive assessment. John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Widlöcher, D. & M. -C. Hardy-Bayle
1989Cognition and control of action in psychopathology. Cahiers dePsychologie Cognitive 9: 583–615. Google Scholar