Attention and language

Maxim I. StamenovElena Andonova
Table of contents

The topic of attention in relation to language has the potential for uniting the efforts of linguists and cognitive psychologists. Our presentation will be oriented toward their relationship at the level of simple sentence planning and comprehension, since this is a critical level of modeling. Modeling the dynamics of focusing in discourse processing (cf. Kendon 1992; Stevenson, Crawley & Kleinman 1994; Chafe 1994; van Dijk 1997; Stamenov & Andonova 1998) should have as its basis the relationships between the structure of attention and sentence structure. We will consider the functioning of attention in normal language use only, while there is also an extensive literature on different issues of the pathology of attention in language use (cf. Widlöcher & Hardy-Bayle 1989; Bates et al. 1995; Chapman & Ulatowska 1997 for further orientation).

Full-text access is restricted to subscribers. Log in to obtain additional credentials. For subscription information see Subscription & Price.

References

Baddeley, A.
1989The uses of working memory. In Solomon et al.: 107–123.Google Scholar
1995Working memory. In M. Gazzaniga (ed.) The cognitive neurosciences: 755–764. MIT Press. DOI logo  BoPGoogle Scholar
Bates, E., Ch. Harris, V. Marchman, B. Wulfeck & M. Kritchevsky
1995Production of complex syntax in normal ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. Language and Cognitive Processes 10: 487–539. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Burzio, L.
1986Italian syntax. Reidel. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chafe, W.
1994Discourse, consciousness, and time. University of Chicago Press.  BoPGoogle Scholar
Chapman, S. & H. Ulatowska
1997Discourse in Dementia: Consideration of consciousness. In Stamenov (ed.): 155–188.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N.
1990Accessibility ‘in principle’. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13: 600–601. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1995The minimalist program. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R.
1987Consciousness and the computational mind. MIT Press.Google Scholar
1990Semantic structures. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jonides, J. & E.E. Smith
1997The architecture of working memory. In M. Rugg (ed.) Cognitive neuroscience: 243–276. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D.
1973Attention and effort. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kendon, A.
1992The negotiation of context in face-to-face interaction. In A. Duranti & Ch. Goodwin (eds.) Rethinking context: Language as an interactive phenomenon: 326–334. Cambridge University Press.  BoPGoogle Scholar
Langacker, R.
1987Foundations of cognitive grammar. Stanford University Press.  BoPGoogle Scholar
1995Conceptual grouping and constituency in cognitive grammar. In Linguistic Society of Korea (ed.) Linguistics in the morning calm 3: 149–172. Hanshin.Google Scholar
Levinson, S.C.
1995Three levels of meaning. In F.R. Palmer (ed.) Grammar and meaning: 90–115. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mandler, G.
1985Cognitive psychology: An essay in cognitive science. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
1989Memory: Conscious and unconscious. In Solomon et al. (eds.): 84–106.Google Scholar
Parasuraman, R.
(ed.) 1998The attentive brain. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Pashler, H.E.
1997The psychology of attention. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Radford, A.
1997Syntactic theory and the structure of English: A minimalist approach. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sanford, A. & S. Garrod
1994Selective processing in text understanding. In M. Gernsbacher (ed.) Handbook of psycholinguistics: 699–719. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Solomon, P.R., R. Paul et al..
(eds.) 1989Memory: Interdisciplinary approaches. Springer-Verlag. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stamenov, M.I.
1997aGrammar, meaning and consciousness: What sentence structure can tell us about the structure of consciousness. In Stamenov (ed.) 1997b: 277–342.Google Scholar
Stamenov, M.I
(ed.) 1997b Language structure, discourse and the access to consciousness. John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stamenov, M.I. & E. Andonova
1998Lexical access and coreference processing in Bulgarian. In D. Hillert (ed.) Sentence processing: A cross-linguistic perspective: 167–181. Academic Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Dijk, T.A.
1997Cognitive context models and discourse. In Stamenov (ed.): 189–226.Google Scholar
Stevenson, R., R.A. Crawley & D. Kleinman
1994Thematic roles, focus and the representation of events. Language and Cognitive Processes 9: 519–548. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Talmy, L.
1988Force dynamics in language and cognition. Cognitive Science 12: 49–100. DOI logo  BoPGoogle Scholar
1996The windowing of attention in language. In M. Shibatani & S. Thompson (eds.) Grammatical Constructions: Their Form and Meaning: 235–287. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Widlߞcher, D. & M.-C. Hardy-Bayle
1989Cognition and control of action in psychopathology. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive 9: 583–615.Google Scholar