Evolutionary pragmatics

Wolfgang Wildgen
Table of contents

Historically there is a link between Darwin’s theory of evolution (1859, applied to humans 1870) and discussions in the ‘Metaphysical Club’ at Harvard, which began meeting in 1872 and had William James and Charles Sanders Peirce as members. Their point of view was that pragmatics is Darwin’s theory of natural selection applied to philosophy. In a speech delivered in Harvard in 1872 Ch. S. Peirce sketched his ‘Pragmatism’ as a philosophy based on the practical consequences of intellectual operations. The term ‘pragmatic’ refers to Kant’s “Anthropologie in pragmatischer Absicht”. From its beginning, pragmatics had therefore a strong link to anthropology (cf. Kant) and evolutionary theory (cf. Darwin) with its central concept of adaptation (cf. Verschueren & Brisard 2002). Östman (1988) points to different levels of adaptation. One has to distinguish adaptive processes found in animals which shape instincts: “Instinct can be seen as a response to the pressures of natural selection, and it thus functions as a means of preservation of the species” (Östman 1988: 11), and adaptation can have different goals: adaptation to a changing ecology, adaptation to changes in the social organization and cultural context of populations and finally adaptation to new standards of language use. When Whorf describes the world-view of the Hopi, he can point to a divergence in human cultural adaptation, which results from a separation of the Amerindian population from the Europeans some 40 to 50,000 years ago. In sociolinguistics the separation of social classes creates different codes as modes of communication in different social networks (cf. the work of Bernstein and others). On a much shorter time scale, every conversation “creates meanings in addition and/or opposition to speaker’s intentions” (Östman 1988: 10). In the following sections, we will focus on the evolution of language and its pragmatic dimensions before the last out-of-Africa migration and the separation of the linguistic families.

Full-text access is restricted to subscribers. Log in to obtain additional credentials. For subscription information see Subscription & Price.

References

Albertazzi, L.
(ed.) 2001The Dawn of Cognitive Science. Early European Contributors. Kluwer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bax, M., B. Van Heusden & W. Wildgen
(eds.) 2004Semiotic Evolution and the Dynamics of Culture. Lang. Google Scholar
Becker, P. R.
1993Werkzeuggebrauch im Tierreich. Wie Tiere hämmern, bohren, streichen. Hirzel. Google Scholar
Bernstein, Basil
1962Social Class, Linguistic Codes and Grammatical Elements. in: Language and Speech 5: 221–240. Google Scholar
Boesch, C.
1993Aspects of Transmission of Tool-use in Wild Chimpanzees. In K. R. Gibson & T. Ingold (eds.): 171–183. Google Scholar
Boesch, C. & M. Tomasello
1998Chimpanzee and Human Cultures. Current Anthropology 39(5): 591–614. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bühler, K.
(1965) [1934]Sprachtheorie. Die Darstellungsfunktion der Sprache, 2nd ed. Fischer. Google Scholar
Buekens, F.
1995Pragmatism. In Handbook of Pragmatics. . DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cassirer, E.
1944An Essay on Man. An Introduction to a Philosophy of Human Culture. Yale University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1957) [1923–29]The Philosophy of Symbolic Forms, vol. 1 to 3. Yale University Press.  MetBibGoogle Scholar
Darwin, C.
(1969) [1872]The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals. Culture et Civilisation. Google Scholar
Davidson, I.
2002The Finished Artefact Fallacy: Acheulean Hand-axes and Language Origins. In A. Wray (ed.) The Transition to Language: 180–203. Oxford University Press. Google Scholar
Davidson, I. & W. Noble
1993Tools and Language in Human Evolution. In K. R. Gibson & T. Ingold (eds.): 363–388. Google Scholar
Dunbar, R.
1997Groups, Gossip, and the Evolution of Language. In A. Schmitt et al. (eds.) New Aspects of Humans Ethology: 77–90. Plenum Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2002Human Evolutionary Psychology.Palgrave Macmillan. Google Scholar
Fehr, E. & J. Henrich
2003Is Strong Reciprocity a Maladaptation? On the Evolutionary Foundation of Human Altruism. CESIFO Working Papers No. 859. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fitch, W. T., M. D. Hauser & N. Chomsky
2005The Evolution of the Language Faculty: Clarifications and Implications. Cognition 97: 179–210. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibson, J. J.
1966The Senses as Perceptual Systems. Houghten Mifflin. Google Scholar
Gibson, K. R. & T. Ingold
(eds.) 1993Tools, Language and Cognition in Human Evolution. Cambridge University Press. Google Scholar
Green, Richard E.
and many others 2010A draft sequence of the Neandertal genome. Science (7 May 2010) 328(5979: 710–722. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Habermas, J.
1982Theorie des kommunikativen Handelns, vol. 2: Zur Kritik der funktionalistischen Vernunft. Suhrkamp. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hewes, G. W.
1977Language Origin Theories. In D. M. Rumbaugh (ed.) Language Learning in Chimpanzee. The Lana Project: 2–53. Academic Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Immelmann, K.
1979Einführung in die Verhaltensforschung, 2nd ed. Parey. Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R. & S. Pinker
2005The Nature of the Language Faculty and its Implications for the Evolution of Language (Reply to Fitch, Hauser, and Chomsky). Cognition 97: 211–225. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jakobson, R.
1981Linguistics and Poetics. In R. Jakobson Selected Wrtitings III. Mouton. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G.
1987Women, Fire, and Dangerous Things. What Categories Reveal about the Mind. Chicago University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Östman, J.-O.
1988Adaptation, Variability, and Effect. IPrA Working Document 3: 5–40. Google Scholar
Pape, H
1999Pragmatismus. In H.-J. Sandkühler (ed.) Enzyklopädie der Philosophie: 1297–1301. Meiner. Google Scholar
Peirce, C. S.
(2002) [1885]Semiotische Schriften, vol. 1: 1865–1903. Suhrkamp. Google Scholar
Piveteau, J.
1991La main et l'hominisation.Masson. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Quiatt, D. & V. Reynolds
1993Primate Behaviour. Information, Social Knowledge, and the Evolution of Culture. Cambridge University Press. Google Scholar
Reynolds, P. C.
1983Ape Constructional Ability and the Origin of Linguistic Structure. In E. De Grolier (ed.) Glossogenetics. The Origin and Evolution of Language: 185–200. Harwood Academic Publishers. Google Scholar
Tembrok, G.
1977Grundlagen des Tierverhaltens. Vieweg. Google Scholar
Thom, R.
1983Mathematical Models of Morphogenesis.Horwood. Google Scholar
Tomasello, M.
1999The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Harvard University Press. Google Scholar
2001Primate communication. Handbook of pragmatics. John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Verschueren, J. & F. Brisard
2002Adaptability. Handbook of Pragmatics. John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wildgen, W.
1985Archetypensemantik. Grundlagen für eine dynamische Semantik auf der Basis der Katastrophentheorie. Narr. Google Scholar
1998Das kosmische Gedächtnis. Kosmologie, Semiotik und Gedächtnistheorie im Werke von Giordano Bruno (1548–1600). Lang. Google Scholar
1999Hand und Auge. Eine Studie zur Repräsentation und Selbstrepräsentation (kognitive und semantische Aspekte). Bremen U.P.(download from the homepage: http://​www​.fb10​.uni​-bremen​.de​/homepages​/wildgen​.htm).
2001Kurt Lewin and the Rise of “Cognitive Sciences” in Germany: Cassirer, Bühler, Reichenbach. In L. Albertazzi (ed.): 299–332. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2004The Evolution of Human Languages. Scenarios, Principles, and Cultural Dynamics. John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2008Semiotic Hypercycles Driving the Evolution of Language. Axiomathes 18(1): 91–116. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2009aMetarepresentation, Self-organization and Self-reference in the Visual Arts. In Wolfgang Wildgen and Barend Van Heusden (eds.) 2009 Meta-representation, Self-organization and Art, Lang, Bern: 173–199. Google Scholar
2009bSketch of an Evolutionary Grammar Based on Comparative Biolinguistics. In Röska-Hardy, Louise S. und Eva M. Neumann-Held (eds.). Learning from Animals? Examining the Nature of Human Uniqueness, Psychology Press, Hove and New York: 45–59.Google Scholar
in print 2010Thom's Theory of “saillance” and “prégnance” and Modern Evolutionary Linguistics. In Wolfgang Wildgen and Per Aage Brandt (eds.). Semiosis and Catastrophes. René Thom's Semiotic Heritage, Lang, Bern. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Plümacher, Martina and Wolfgang Wildgen
(eds.) 2009Prägnanter Inhalt–Prägnante Form. Thematic volume of: Zeitschrift für Semiotik, 31 (1–2), Stauffenburg, Tübingen. Google Scholar
Yates, F. A.
1966The Art of Memory. Routledge & Kegan. Google Scholar