Pragmatic acquisition

Sandrine Zufferey
Table of contents

In order to become proficient language users, children must develop an array of abilities ranging from the understanding of non-literal language uses, the ability to produce referring expressions containing the adequate degree of information depending on their addressee’s knowledge state, the use of adequate expressions depending on social relations with their addressee, to name but a few. Traditionally, in theoretical pragmatics, these competencies have been addressed in two different trends of studies. On the one hand, studies focusing on the appropriate use of language depending on social relations between speaker and hearer, that investigate phenomena such as politeness. On the other hand, studies aiming at explaining how hearers enrich the linguistically-encoded meaning of sentences to access the speaker’s meaning, that focus on phenomena such as reference resolution and implicatures.

Full-text access is restricted to subscribers. Log in to obtain additional credentials. For subscription information see Subscription & Price.

References

Ackerman, Brian
1983 “Form and function in children’s understanding of ironic utterances.” Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 35: 487–508. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Andersen, Elaine, Maquela Brizuela, Beatrice DuPuy and Laura Gonnerman
1999 “Cross-linguistic evidence for the early acquisition of discourse markers as register variables.” Journal of Pragmatics 31: 1339–1351. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ariel, Mira
1988 “Referring and accessibility.” Journal of Linguistics 24: 65–87. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Aronsson, Karin and Mia Thorell
1999 “Family politics in children’s play directives.” Journal of Pragmatics 31(1): 25–47. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Austin, John
1962How to Do Things with Words. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Bahtiyar, Sevda and Aylin Küntay
2008 “Integration of communicative partner’s visual perspective in patterns of referential requests.” Journal of Child Language 36(3): 529–555. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barner, David, Katherine Chow and Shu-Ju Yang
2009 “Finding one’s meaning: A test of the relation between quantifiers and integers in language development.” Cognitive Psychology 58: 195–219. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, Elizabeth
1976Language and Context: the Acquisition of Pragmatics. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Becker-Bryant, Judith
2009 “Pragmatic development.” In The Cambridge Handbook of Child Language, ed. by Edith Bavin, 339–354. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Begus, Katarina and Victoria Southgate
2012 “Infant pointing serves an interrogative function.” Developmental Science 15(5): 611–617. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bernicot, Josie
1992Les actes de langage chez l’enfant. Paris: P.U.F.Google Scholar
Bowerman, Melissa and Stephen C. Levinson
2001Language Acquisition and Conceptual Development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Boyce, W. Thomas
2004 “Social stratification, health, and violence in the very young.” Annuals of New York Academy of Science 1036: 47–68. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Burdelski, Matthew
2011 “Language socialization and politeness routines.” In Handbook of Language Socialization, ed. by Alessandro Duranti, Elinor Ochs and Bambi Schieffelin, 275–295. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bruner, Jerome
1975 “From communication to language: a psychological perspective.” Cognition 3(3): 255–287. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cameron-Faulkner, Thea
2014 “The development of speech acts.” Pragmatic Development in First Language Acquisition, ed. by Danielle Matthews, 37–53. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Campbell, Aimee, Patricia Brooks and Michael Tomasello
2000“Factors affecting young children’s use of pronouns as referring expressions.” Journal of Speech , Language, and Hearing Research 43: 1337–1349. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carrell, Patricia
1981 “Children’s understanding of indirect requests: Comparing child and adult comprehension.” Journal of Child Language 8: 320–345. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carruthers, Peter
2013 “Mindreading in infancy.” Mind & Language 28 (2): 141–172. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carston, Robyn
2002Thoughts and Utterances: The Pragmatics of Explicit Communication. Oxford: Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Charafeddine, Rawan, Hugo Mercier, Laurence Kaufman, Fabrice Clément, André Berchtold, Anne Reboul and Jean-Baptiste Van der Henst
2015 “How Preschoolers use cues of dominance to make sense of their social environment.” Journal of Cognition and Development 16(4): 587– 604. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, Eve
2003First Language Acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Coulthard, Malcolm
1977An Introduction to Discourse Analysis. London: Longman.Google Scholar
De Cat, Cécile
2015 “The cognitive underpinnings of referential abilities.” In The Acquisition of Reference, ed. by Ludovica Serratrice and Shanley Allen, 263–283. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Villiers, Jill
2007 “The interface of language and theory of mind.” Lingua 117: 1858–1878. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dews, Shelly, Ellen Winner, Joan Kaplan, Elizabeth Rosenblatt, Malia Hunt, Karen Lim, Angela McGovern, Alison Qualter and Bonnie Smarsh
1996 “Children’s understanding of the meaning and functions of verbal irony.” Child Development 67: 3071–3085. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dore, John
1975Holophrases, speech acts and language universals. Journal of Child Language 2: 21–40. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Duranti, Alessandro, Elinor Ochs and Bambi Schieffelin
2011Handbook of Language Socialization. Wiley-Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Egner, Inge
2006Intercultural aspects of the speech act of promising: Western and African practices. Intercultural Pragmatics 3(4): 443–464. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, Susan
1976 “Is Sybil there? The structure of some American English directives.” Language in Society 5: 25–66. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1977 “Wait for me, roller skate!” In Child Discourse, ed. by Susan Ervin-Tripp and Catherine Mitchell-Kernan, 165–188. New York: Academic Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, Susan and David Gordon
1986 “The development of children's requests.” In Communicative Competence: Assessment and intervention, ed. by Richard Schiefelbusch, 61–96. San Diego: College Hill Press.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, Susan, Jiansheng Guo and Martin Lampert
1990 “Politeness and persuasion in children’s control acts.” Journal of Pragmatics 14: 195–219. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, Susan, Mary O’Connor and Jarrett Rosenberg
1984 “Language and power in the family.” In Language and Power, ed. by. Cheris Kramarae, Muriel Schulz and William O’Barr, 116–135. Belmont: Sage Press.Google Scholar
Field, Margaret
2001 “Triadic directives in Navajo language socialization.” Language in Society 30(2): 249–263. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Filippova, Eva and Janet Astington
2008 “Further development in social reasoning revealed in discourse irony understanding.” Child Development 79: 126–138. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Georgalidou, Marianthi
2008 “The contextual parameters of linguistic choice: Greek children’s preferences for the formation of directive speech acts.” Journal of Pragmatics 40(1): 72–94. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Glenwright, Melanie, Jayanthi Parackel, Kristene Cheung and Elizabeth Nilsen
2013 “Intonation influences how children and adults interpret sarcasm.” Journal of Child Language 41(2): 472–484. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, Marjorie
1993 “Accomplishing social organization in girls’ play: patterns of competition and cooperation in an African-American working-class girls’ group.” In Feminist Theory and the Study of Folklore, ed. by. Susan Hollis, Linda Pershing, and M. Jane Young, 149–165. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Grice, Herbert Paul
1989 “Logic and conversation.” In Studies in the Way of Words, 22–40. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Guasti, Maria Teresa, Gennaro Chierchia, Stephen Crain, Francesca Foppolo, Andrea Gualmini, and Luisa Meroni
2005 “Why children and adults sometimes (but not always) compute implicatures.” Language and Cognitive Processes 20(5): 667–696. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gundel, Jeanette, Hedberg, Nancy and Zacharski, Ron
1993 “Cognitive status and the form of referring expressions in discourse.” Language 69: 274–307. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gundel, Jeanette and Johnson, Kaitlin
2013 “Children's use of referring expressions in spontaneous discourse: Implications for theory of mind development.” Journal of Pragmatics 56: 43–57. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gundel, Jeanette K., Dimitris Ntelitheos and Melinda Kowalsky
2007 “Children's use of referring expressions: some implications for theory of mind.” ZAS Papers in Linguistics 48: 1–21.Google Scholar
Hamlin, J. Kiley and Karen Wynn
2011 “Young infants prefer prosocial to antisocial others.” Cognitive Development 26(1): 30–39. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hancock, Jeffrey, Philipp Dunham and Kelly Purdy
2000 “Children’s comprehension of critical and complimentary forms of verbal irony.” Journal of Cognition and Development 12: 227–240. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Happé, Francesca
1993 “Communicative competence and theory of mind in autism: a test of relevance theory.” Cognition 48: 101–119. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hickmann, Maya
1995 “Discourse organization and the development of reference to person, space and time.” In The Handbook of Child Language, ed. by P. Fletcher and B. Mc. Whinney, 194–218. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Hurewitz, Felicia, Anna Papafragou, Lila Gleitman, and Rochel Gelman
2006 “Asymmetries in the acquisition of numbers and quantifiers.” Language Learning and Development 2: 77–96. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hyams, Nina and Kenneth Wexler
1993 “On the grammatical basis of null subjects in child language.” Linguistic Inquiry 24(3): 421–459.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, Annette
1979A Functional Approach to Child Language: A Study of Determiners and Reference. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Katsos, Napoleon
2009 “Neither default nor particularized: Scalar implicatures from a developmental perspective.” In Semantics and Pragmatics: From Experiment to Theory, ed. by Richard Breheny, Uli Sauerland and Kazuko Yatsushiro, 51–73. London: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Keenan, Thomas and Kathleen Quigley
1999 “Do young children use echoic information in their comprehension of sarcastic speech? A test of echoic mention theory.” British Journal of Developmental Psychology 17: 83–96. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Küntay, Aylin, Keiko Nakamura and Beysa Şen
2014 “Crosslinguistic and crosscultural approaches to pragmatic development.” In Pragmatic Development in First Language Acquisition, ed. by Danielle Matthews, 317–342. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ledbetter, Patricia and Cathy Dent
1988 “Young children’s sensitivity to direct and indirect request structure.” First Language 8: 227–245. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levinson, Stephen
2000Presumptive Meanings: The Theory of Generalized Conversational Implicatures. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Li, Duanduan
2008 “Pragmatic socialization.” In The Encyclopedia of Applied Linguistics, ed. by C. Chapelle, 71–83. Oxford: Wiley Online Library.Google Scholar
Liszkowski, Ulf, Malinda Carpenter and Michael Tomasello
2008 “Twelve-month-olds communicate helpfully and appropriately for knowledgeable and ignorant partners.” Cognition 108(3): 732–739. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Low, Jason and Joseph Perner
2012 “Implicit and explicit theory of mind. State of the art.” British Journal of Developmental Psychology 30: 1–13. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Maratsos, Michael
1976The Use of Definite and Indefinite Reference in Young Children: An Experimental Study of Semantic Acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Mascaro, Olivier and Gergely Csibra
2012 “Representation of stable social dominance relations by human infants.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 109(18): 6862–6867. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Matthews, Danielle
(ed.) 2014Pragmatic Development in First Language Acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Matthews, Danielle, Elena Lieven and Michael Tomasello
2007 “How toddlers and preschoolers learn to uniquely identify referents.” Child Development 78: 1744–1759. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nadig, Aparna and Julie Sedivy
2002 “Evidence of perspective-taking constraints in children’s online-reference resolution.” Psychological Science 13: 329–336. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nakamura, Keiko
2006 “The acquisition of linguistic politeness in Japanese.” In The Handbook of East Asian Psycholinguistics: Volume II Japanese, ed. by. Mineharu Nakayama, Reiko Mazuka and Yasuhiro Shirai, 110–115. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nakassis, Constantine and Jesse Snedeker
2002 “Beyond sarcasm: Intonation and context as relational cues in children’s recognition of irony.” In Proceedings of the Twenty-sixth Boston University Conference on Language Development, ed. by Barbora Skarabela, Sarah Fish and Anna Do, 429–440. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, Katherine
2007Young Minds in Social Worlds: Experience, Meaning, and Memory. Cambridge, MA.: Harvard University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Newcombe, N., and Zaslow, M
1981 “Do 2½ year-olds hint? A study of directive forms in the speech of 2½ year-olds to adults.” Discourse Processes 4(3): 239–252. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ninio, Anat and Catherine Snow
1996Pragmatic Development. Boulder: Westview Press.Google Scholar
Noveck, Ira
2001 “When children are more logical than adults: experimental investigations of scalar implicature.” Cognition 78(2): 165–188. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ochs, Elinor, and Schieffelin, Bambi
(eds.) 1979Developmental Pragmatics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ogiermann, Eva
2009 “Politeness and in-directness across cultures: a comparison of English, German, Polish and Swedish requests.” Journal of Politeness Research 5: 189–216. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
O’Neill, Daniela
1996 “Two-year-old children’s sensitivity to a parent’s knowledge state when making requests.” Child Development 67: 659–677. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
O’Neill, Daniela and Jane Topolovec
2001 “Two-year-old children’s sensitivity to the referential (in)efficacy of their own pointing gestures.” Journal of Child Language 28: 1–28. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ochs, Elinor and Bambi Schieffelin
1986Language Socialization across Cultures. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Origgi, Gloria and Dan Sperber
2000 “Evolution, communication and the proper function of language.” In Evolution and the Human Mind: Language, Modularity and Social Cognition, ed. by Peter Carruthers and Andrew Chamberlain, 140–169. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Papafragrou, A
2000 “Early communication: beyond speech-act theory.” Proceedings of the 24th Annual Boston University Conference on Child Development . Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
Papafragou, Anna
2006 “From scalar semantics to implicature: Children’s interpretation of aspectuals.” Journal of Child Language 33: 721–757. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Papafragou, Anna and Julien Musolino
2003 “Scalar implicatures: Experiments at the semantics/pragmatics interface.” Cognition 86: 253–282. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Papafragou, Anna and Dimitri Skordos
2016 “Scalar implicature.” In The Oxford Handbook of Developmental Linguistics, ed. by Jeffrey Lidz, William Snyder and Joe Pater, 611–632. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Papafragou, Anna and Nicky Tantalou
2004 “Children’s computation of implicatures.” Language Acquisition 12(1): 71–82. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Park, Eunjin
2006 “Grandparents, grandchildren, and heritage language use in Korean.” In Heritage Language Development: Focus on East Asian Immigrants, ed. by Kimi Kondo-Brown, 57–86. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pexman, Penny and Melanie Glenwright
2007 “How do typically developing children grasp the meaning of verbal irony?Journal of Neurolinguistics 20: 178–96. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pouscoulous, Nausicaa and Ira Noveck
2009 “Going beyond semantics: The development of pragmatic enrichment.” In Language Acquisition, ed. by Susan Foster-Cohen, 196–215. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pouscoulous, Nausicaa, Ira Noveck, Guy Politzer and Anne Bastide
2007 “A developmental investigation of processing costs in implicature production.” Language Acquisition 14: 347–376. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Read, Barbara and Louise Cherry
1978 “Preschool children’s production of directive forms.” Discourse Processes 1: 233–245. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Reinhardt, Tanja
2006Interface Strategies. Optimal and Costly Computations. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Rizzi, Luigi
2005 “Grammatically-based target inconsistencies in child language.” University of Connecticut Occasional Papers in Linguistics 4: 19–49.Google Scholar
Ryckebusch, Céline and Haydée Marcos
2004 “Speech acts, social context and parent-toddler play between the ages of 1;5 and 2;3.” Journal of Pragmatics 36(5): 883–897. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Saxton, Matthew
2010Child Language. Acquisition and Development. London: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Schneider, Klaus and Elly Ifantidou
to appear). Developmental and Clinical Pragmatics. Berlin/Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.
Schulze, Cornelia, Susanne Grassmann and Michael Tomasello
2013 “3-year-old children make relevance inferences in indirect verbal communication.” Child Development 84(6): 2079–2093. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Searle, John
1969Speech Acts. An Essay in the Philosophy of Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Serratrice, Ludovica and Shanley Allen
2015The Acquisition of Reference. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shatz, Marylin and Laura McCloskey
1984 “Answering appropriately: a developmental perspective on conversational knowledge.” In Discourse Development, ed. by Stan Kuczaj, 19–36. New York: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Siegal, Michael and Luca Surian
2009 “Conversational understanding in young children.” In Blackwell Handbook of Child Development, ed. by Erika Hoff and Marylin Shatz, 304–323. Malden: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Skarabela, Barbora, Shanley Allan and Thomas Scott-Phillips
2013 “Joint attention helps to explain why children omit new referents.” Journal of Pragmatics 56: 5–14. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sperber, Dan and Deirdre Wilson
2008 “A deflationary theory of metaphor.” In Cambridge Handbook of Metaphor and Thought, ed. by Raymond Gibbs, 84–105. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sullivan, Kate, Ellen Winner and Natalie Hopfield
1995 “How children tell a lie from a joke: the role of second-order mental state attributions.” British Journal of Developmental Psychology 13: 191–204. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thompson, Linda
(ed) 1997Children Talking. The Acquisition of Pragmatic Competence. Clevedon: Multilingual matters.Google Scholar
Tomasello, Michael
2004Constructing a Language: A usage-based Theory of Language Acquisition. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, Michael, Randi Strosberg and Nameera Akhtar
1996 “Eighteen-month-old children learn words in non-ostensive contexts.” Journal of Child Language 23: 157–176. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vázquez, Maria, Sarah Delisle and Megan Saylor
2012 “Four- and six-year-olds use pragmatic competence to guide word learning.” Journal of Child Language 40(2): 291–306. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vosniadou, Stella, Andrew Ortony, Ralph Reynolds and Paul Wilson
1984 “Sources of difficulty in children’s understanding of metaphorical language.” Child Development 55: 1588–1606. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wellman, Henry, David Cross and Julanne Watson
2001 “Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: the truth about false belief.” Child Development 72(3): 655–684. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilson, Deirdre
2006 “The pragmatics of verbal irony: echo or pretense?Lingua 116: 1722–1743. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2013 “Irony comprehension: a developmental perspective.” Journal of Pragmatics 59: 40–56. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilson, Deirdre and Robyn Carston
2007 “A unitary approach to lexical pragmatics: Relevance, inference and ad hoc concepts.” In Pragmatics ed. by Noel Burton-Roberts, 230–259. Basingstoke: Palgrave. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wimmer, Heinz and Jospeh Perner
1983 “Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception.” Cognition 13: 103–114. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Winner, Ellen
1988The Point of Words: Children’s Understanding of Metaphor and Irony. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Winner, Ellen and Sue Leekam
1991 “Distinguishing irony from deception: understanding the speaker’s second-order intention.” British Journal of Developmental Psychology 9: 257–270. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wittek, Angelika and Michael Tomasello
2005 “Young children’s sensitivity to listener knowledge and perceptual context in choosing referring expressions.” Applied Psycholinguistics 26: 541–558. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yule, George
1997Referential Communication Tasks. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Zufferey, Sandrine
2010Lexical Pragmatics and Theory of Mind. The Acquisition of Connectives. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2014Acquiring Pragmatics. Social and Cognitive Perspectives. London: Routledge.Google Scholar