Psycholinguistics

Gary D. Prideaux
Table of contents

Broadly speaking, psycholinguistics deals with the cognitive processes and representations involved in language comprehension, production, and acquisition. As such, it calls upon both the theories and methodologies of the two disciplines of linguistics and psychology to address a variety of areas involved in language processing. In the 1950s, a number of experimental studies within what could be called the psychology of language were carried out, following the influential studies on chunking in memory carried out by Miller and his associates early on in the decade. Some of these studies focused on the importance of ‘sets’, i.e. discourse topics, for the understanding and memory retention of a text.

Full-text access is restricted to subscribers. Log in to obtain additional credentials. For subscription information see Subscription & Price.

References

Atkinson, M.
1992Children’s syntax. Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bates, E. & B. Macwhinney
1982Functionalist approaches to grammar. In E. Wanner & L. Gleitman (eds.) Language acquisition: 173–218. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
1989Functionalism and the competition model. In B. Macwhinney & E. Bates (eds.): 3–73.Google Scholar
Bever, T. G.
1970The cognitive bases for linguistic structures. In J. R. Hayes (ed.) Cognition and the development of language: 279–362. John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Blakemore, D.
1992Understanding utterances. Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bransford, J. D. & J. J. Franks
1971The abstraction of linguistic ideas. Cognitive Psychology 2: 331–350. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Butterworth, B.
1992Lexical access in speech production. In W. D. Marslen-Wilson (ed.): 108–135.Google Scholar
Chafe, W. L.
1987Cognitive constraints on information flow. In R. S. Tomlin (ed.): 21–51. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N.
1957Syntactic structures. Mouton. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1965Aspects of the theory of syntax. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clark, H. H. & E. V. Clark
1977Psychology and language. Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Google Scholar
Crain, S. & M. Steedman
1985On not being led up the garden path. In D. Dowty et al. (eds.) Natural language parsing: 320–358. Cambridge University Press.. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Farrar, M. J.
1990Discourse and the acquisition of grammatical morphemes. Journal of Child Language 17: 607–624. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. A.
1983The modularity of mind. MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J.A
et al. 1974The psychology of language. McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. D.
1979Superstrategy. In W. E. Cooper & E. T. C. Walker (eds.) Sentence processing: 249–279. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
1991Sentence processing and the mental grammar. In P. Sells et al. (eds.) Foundational issues in natural language processing: 83–113. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Forster, K. I.
1992Basic issues in lexical processing. In W. D. Marslen-Wilson (ed.): 75–107.Google Scholar
Fox, B.
1987Morpho-syntactic markedness and discourse structure. Journal of Pragmatics 11: 359–375. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Frazier, L.
1979On comprehending sentences. Indiana University Linguistics Club.Google Scholar
1992Against lexical generation of syntax. In W. D. Marslen-Wilson (ed.): 505–528.  MetBibGoogle Scholar
Frazier, L. & J. D. Fodor
1978The sausage machine. Cognition 6: 291–325. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fromkin, V. A.
1993Speech production. In J. B. Gleason & N. B. Ratner (eds.): 271–300.Google Scholar
Gleason, J. B.
(ed.) 1993The development of language. Merrill.Google Scholar
Gleason, J. B. & N. B. Ratner
1993Language development in children. In J. B. Gleason & N. B. Ratner (eds.): 302–350. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(eds.) 1993Psycholinguistics. Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Google Scholar
Goodluck, H.
1991Language acquisition. Blackwell.. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Green, G. M.
1989Pragmatics and natural language understanding. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Grice, H. P.
1975Logic and conversation. In P. Cole & J. Morgan (eds.) Syntax and semantics, vol. 3: 41–58. Academic Press.Google Scholar
1989Studies in the way of words. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Kempson, R.
1988Mental representations. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kess, J. F.
1992Psycholinguistics. John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kimball, J.
1973Seven principles of surface structure parsing in natural language. Cognition 2: 15–47. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kintsch, W.
1970Memory and cognition. John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Leech, G. N.
1983Principles of pragmatics. Longman.Google Scholar
Levelt, W. J. M.
1989Speaking. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Macwhinney, B. & E. Bates
(eds.) 1989The crosslinguistic study of sentence processing. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W. D.
1987Functional parallelism in spoken word-recognition. Cognition 25: 71–102. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1992Access and integration. In W. D. Marslen-Wilson (ed.): 3–24.Google Scholar
(ed.) 1992Lexical representation and process. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W. D. & L. K. Tyler
1987Against modularity. In J. L. Garfield (ed.) Modularity in knowledge representation and natural language understanding: 37–62. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Mcclelland, J. L. & D. E. Rumelhart
1981An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception. Psychological Review 88: 375–407. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Prichett, B. L.
1988Garden path phenomena and the grammatical basis of language processing. Language 64: 539–576. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Prideaux, G. D.
1993Subordination and information distribution in oral and written narratives. Pragmatics and Cognition 1: 51–69. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Prideaux, G. D. & J. T. Hogan
1993Markedness as a discourse management device. Word 44: 397–411.. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Slobin, D. I.
1973Cognitive prerequisites for the development of language. In C. A. Ferguson & D. I. Slobin (eds.) Studies of child language development: 175–208. Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
(ed.) 1985A cross-linguistic study of language acquisition. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E.
1993Bilingualism and second language acquisition. In J. B. Gleason & N. B. Ratner (eds.): 392–416. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sperber, D. & D. Wilson
1986Relevance. Blackwell.Google Scholar
Stubbs, M.
1983Discourse analysis. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Tomlin, R. S.
1985Foreground-background information and the syntax of subordination. Text 5: 85–122. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(ed.) 1987Coherence and grounding in discourse. John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomlin, R. S. & M-M. Pu
1991Referential management in Mandarin. Cognitive Linguistics 2: 65–93. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tyler, L. K.
1992The role of lexical representations in language comprehension. In W. D. Marslen-Wilson (ed.): 439–462.Google Scholar
White, L.
1989Universal grammar and second language acquisition. John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wingfield, A.
1993Sentence processing. In J. B. Gleason & N. B. Ratner (eds.): 200–235.Google Scholar