Word

Marianne Mithun
Table of contents

We know that the size and content of words varies widely across languages; what can be said in a single word in some languages can be expressed only in a multi-word sentence in others. The word payugqetullrullinikiit, for example, used in a Central Alaskan Yup’ik (Eskimo) conversation, was translated into English as ‘they apparently used to bring food to her’. Some of the differences we find among languages in the content of words reflect differences in the ideas that are coded as basic concepts. The Yup’ik word above is based on the verb root payug- ‘to take food over to a friend, relative, or neighbor’. The root does not contain segments meaning ‘food’ or ‘relative’; it simply denotes a recognizable activity that constitutes a unitary cultural concept for speakers. Other cross-linguistic differences in the amount of information packaged in words are the result of differences in word structure. In polysynthetic languages like Yup’ik, words may have elaborate structures. The word above consists of seven meaningful parts: payug-qe-tu-llru-llini-k-iit. Following the root are a suffix -qe- ‘time after time’, a suffix -tu- ‘customarily’, a suffix -llru- for past tense, a suffix -llini- ‘apparently’, a suffix -k- for participial mood, and finally a pronominal suffix -iit ‘they/her’. The variation we find in the amount of information carried by words cross-linguistically raises some fundamental questions. First, are utterances like payugqetullrullinikiit really single words? Second, if they are, do such differences in the packaging of information matter?

Full-text access is restricted to subscribers. Log in to obtain additional credentials. For subscription information see Subscription & Price.

References

Anderson, S.R.
1992A-morphous morphology. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Aronoff, M.
1976Word formation in generative grammar. MIT Press. Google Scholar
Bybee, J.
1985Morphology: a study of the relation between meaning and form. John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Di Sciullo, A.M. & E. Williams
1987On the definition of word. MIT Press. Google Scholar
Jacobsen, S.
1984Yup’ik Eskimo dictionary. Alaska Native Language Center. Google Scholar
Mithun, M.
1989The acquisition of polysynthesis. Journal of Child Language 16: 285–312. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Scalise, S.
1984Generative morphology. Foris. Google Scholar
Selkirk, E.O.
1982The syntax of words. MIT Press. Google Scholar
Spencer, A.
1991Morphological theory: an introduction to word structure in generative grammar. Blackwell. Google Scholar
Zwicky, A.M.
1984Clitics and particles. In A. Zwicky & R. Wallace (eds.), Papers on morphology: 148–173. The Ohio State University Department of Linguistics Working Papers in Linguistics 29. Google Scholar