Inter-mind phenomena in child narrative discourse

Barbara Bokus

Abstract

A review of the current literature shows that by the age of two and a half (and probably earlier), children have already acquired a rich working knowledge of human intentionality and goal-directed action (Stein & Albro 1997: 7; Mandler 1998). The paper focuses on the ways in which children use this knowledge to tell stories from pictures. The story is the description of the actions performed by animate actors. We distinguish the main actors (protagonists in the narrative line) and the background actors (participants in the narrative field) who can observe and interpret what is going on in the main action. So the narrative text contains not only the action presented by the story-teller (landscape of action) but also how this action is interpreted by the story characters (landscape of consciousness). They are all thinking minds who can think similarly or differently about the plot. And the narrator uses characters’ minds to produce different representations of the story (Bokus 1998, 2000). The narrator can confront one interpretation with another, and a) makes choices of the “true” representation of the main action (in doing this the child plays the role of the omniscient and omnipresent story-teller who is directly in touch with the ontology of the story), or b) presents a possible but not a certain story reality (the listener is not told how things are but rather how they seem to be). Therefore we can speak about the interplay of the narrator’s mind and the minds of story characters in a kind of internal narrator’s dialogue. The storyteller creates different minds and alternative ways of interpreting the main action. Also shown are examples of such inter-mind phenomena in the stories told by preschool children.

Keywords:
Quick links
A browser-friendly version of this article is not yet available. View PDF
Abbeduto, L., and S. Rosenberg
(1985) Children’s knowledge of the presuppositions of know and other verbs. Journal of Child Language 12: 621-641. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Astington, J.W
(1990) Narrative and the child’s theory of mind. In B.K. Britton, & A.D. Pellegrini (eds.), Narrative thought and narrative language. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 151-171.Google Scholar
(1993) The child’s discovery of the mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bakhtin, M
(1984) Problems of Dostoevski’s poetics. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bamberg, M
(ed.) (1997) Narrative development: Six approaches. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.  BoPGoogle Scholar
Bamberg, M., and R. Damrad-Frye
(1991) On the ability to provide evaluative comments: Further explorations of children’s narrative competence. Journal of Child Language 18: 689-710. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bartsch, K., and H.M. Wellman
(1995) Children talk about the mind. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bokus, B
(1991) Children’s pragmatic knowledge of narrative tasks. In J. Verschueren (ed.), Pragmatics at issue. Vol. 1 (Selected papers of the International Pragmatics Conference, Antwerp – 1987). Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, pp. 13-28. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1992) Peer co-narration: Changes in structure of preschoolers’ participation. Journal of Narrative and Life History 2: 253-275. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1996a) Action and consciousness: Duality of children’s narrative construction. Paper presented at the VIIth International Congress for the Study of Child Language (Istanbul, July 14–19, 1996).
(1996b) Narrative space structuring at the preschool age (Findings on monologic and dialogic discourse. In C.E. Johnson & J.H.V. Gilbert (eds.), Children’s Language, vol. 9. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 197-207.Google Scholar
(1998) Action and its representation in the minds of story characters. Findings from children’s discourse. Psychology of Language and Communication 2.2: 63-77.Google Scholar
(2000) Światy fabuły w narracji dziecięcej (Story worlds in child narration). Warsaw: Energeia.Google Scholar
Britton, B.K., and A.D. Pellegrini
(eds.) (1990) Narrative thought and narrative language. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.  BoPGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J
(1986) Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
(1990) Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Donaldson, M
(1978) Children’s minds. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Eco, U
(1994) Lector in fabula. Warszawa: PIW.Google Scholar
Ely, R., A. McCabe, A. Wolf, and G. Melzi
(2000) The story behind the story: Gathering narrative data from children. In L. Menn, & N.B. Ratner (eds.), Methods for studying language production. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 249-269.Google Scholar
Flavell, J.H., and P.H. Miller
(1998) Social cognition. In D. Kuhn, & R. Siegler (eds.), Cognition, perception, and language. Vol. II of W. Damon (Gen.ed.), Handbook of child psychology. New York: Wiley, pp. 851-898.Google Scholar
Fox, R
(1991) Developing awareness of mind reflected in children’s narrative writing. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 9: 281-298. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, A., and A.N. Meltzoff
(1997) Words, thoughts, and theories. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.  BoPGoogle Scholar
Halliday, M.A.K., and R. Hasan
(1976) Cohesion in English. London: Longman.  BoPGoogle Scholar
Hermans, H.J.M
(1997) Self-narrative in the life course: A contextual approach. In M. Bamberg (ed.), Narrative development: Six approaches. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 223-264.Google Scholar
Hudson, J.A., and L.R. Shapiro
(1991) From kowing to telling: The development of children’s scripts, stories, and personal narratives. In A. McCabe, & C. Peterson (eds.), Developing narrative structure. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 89-136.Google Scholar
Kielar-Turska, M
(1999) The inner landscape of characters in stories told by children. Psychology of Language and Communication 3.2: 49-56.Google Scholar
Labov, W., and J. Waletzky
(1967) Narrative analysis: Oral versions of personal experience. In J. Helm (ed.), Essays on the verbal and visual arts. Seattle: University of Washington Press, pp. 12-44.Google Scholar
Lewis, C., N.H. Freeman, C. Hagestadt, and H. Douglas
(1994) Narrative access and production in preschoolers’ false belief reasoning. Cognitive Development 9: 397-424. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lucariello, J
(1990) Canonicality and consciousness in child narrative. In B.K. Britton, & A.D. Pellegrini (eds.), Narrative thought and narrative language. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 149.Google Scholar
Lyons, J
(1977) Semantics, vol. 2. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Macnamara, J., E. Baker, and C. Olson
(1976) Four-year-olds’ understanding of pretend, forget and know: Evidence for propositional operations. Child Development 47: 62-70. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mandler, J.M
(1998) Representation. In D. Kuhn, & R.S. Siegler (eds.), Cognition, perception, and language: Vol. 2, W. Damon (ed.), Handbook of child psychology . New York: Wiley, pp. 255-281.Google Scholar
Musatti, T
(1986) Early peer relations: The perspectives of Piaget and Vygotsky. In E.C. Mueller, & C.R. Cooper (eds.), Process and outcome in peer relationships. Orlando: Academic Press, pp. 25-53.Google Scholar
Nicolopoulou, A., and E.S. Richner
(1999a) From actors to characters to persons: The development of character representation in young children’s narratives. Poster presented at the 1999 Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development. Albuquerque (April 15-18, 1999), (work in progress), NM.
(1999b) The development of main character in young children’s narratives. Paper presented at the VIIIth Congress for the Study of Child Language, San Sebastian (July 12–16, 1999), NM.
Pillow, B.H
(1989) Early understanding of perception as a source of knowledge. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 47: 116-129. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pratt, C., and P. Bryant
(1990) Young children understand that looking leads to knowing. Child Development 61: 973-982. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Searle, J.R
(1983) Intentionality: An essay in the philosophy of mind. New York: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2002) Consciousness and language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shields, M
(1978) The child as psychologist: Construing the social world. In A. Lock (ed.), Action, gesture, and symbol: The emergence of language. London: Academic Press, pp. 529-561.Google Scholar
Shugar, G.W
(1976) Text-constructing with an adult. A form of child activity during early language acquisition. In D. Drachman (ed.), Akten des 1. Salzburger Kolloquiums über Kindersprache. Tübingen: Verlag Gunter Narr, pp. 343-356.Google Scholar
(1998) Entry into narration: Uses of operation of reference. Psychology of Language and Communication 2.2: 7-16.Google Scholar
Stein, N.L., and E.R. Albro
(1997) Building complexity and coherence: Children’s use of goal-structured knowledge in telling stories. In M. Bamberg (ed.), Narrative development: Six approaches. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 5-44.Google Scholar
Taylor, M
(1996) A theory of mind perspective on social development. In R. Gelman, & T.K.-F. Au (eds.), Perceptual and cognitive development. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, pp. 283-329. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M
(1999) The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tomaszewski, T
(1975) Człowiek i otoczenie (Man and environment). In T. Tomaszewski (ed.), Psychologia. Warsaw: PWN, pp. 13-36.Google Scholar
Wertsch, J.V
(1991) Voices of the mind. A sociocultural approach to mediated action. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar