Taboo vocatives in the language of London teenagers

Ignacio M. Palacios Martínez
University of Santiago de Compostela

Abstract

This study focusses on the use and functions of so-called taboo vocatives (e.g. dickhead, you bastard, bitch) in the language of London teenagers, based on the analysis of over 500 examples extracted from COLT (The Bergen Corpus of London Teenage Language) and LEC (London English Corpus). Findings illustrate a wide variety of items in this category, and show that these cannot be regarded as mere insults, since they often serve to reinforce the bonds between young speakers as well, and indeed can even carry affectionate connotations. The majority of these items are nouns and denote some kind of sexual reference, an abnormal or strange human condition, or a pejorative, animal-related allusion. There does not seem to have been any major changes in the use of these forms from the 1990s to the first decades of the current century, although many of them have broadened their meaning and can now be used with either male or female speakers.

Keywords:
Publication history
Table of contents

1.Introduction

Teen talk has been studied extensively in recent decades (Stenström et al. 2002Stenström, Anna-Brita, Gisle Andersen, and Ingrid Hasund 2002Trends in Teenage Talk: Corpus Compilation, Analysis and Findings. Amsterdam, Philadelphia: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Androutsopoulos and Georgakopoulos 2003Androutsopoulos, Jannis J., and Alexandra Georgakopoulos 2003Discourse Constructions of Youth Identities. John Benjamins: Amsterdam. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Stenström and Jørgensen 2009 eds. 2009Youngspeak in a Multilingual Perspective. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Spiegel and Gysin 2016Spiegel, Carmen, and Daniel Gysin (eds.) 2016Jugendsprache in Schule, Medien und Alltag (Sprache – Kommunikation – Kultur 19). Frankfurt am Main: Lang Verlag. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Tagliamonte 2016Tagliamonte, Sali 2016Teen Talk: The Language of Adolescents. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Drummond 2018Drummond, Rob 2018Researching Urban Youth Language and Identity. New York: Palgrave Macmillan. DOI logoGoogle Scholar), not least because teenagers are regarded as language innovators and precursors of language change (Eckert 1988Eckert, Penelope 1988 “Adolescent Social Structure and the Spread of Linguistic Change”. Language in Society 17: 183–207. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Tagliamonte 2016Tagliamonte, Sali 2016Teen Talk: The Language of Adolescents. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar). It is generally taken to be the case that teenagers differ from adults in the way they talk, not only in terms of lexis but also at other levels of the language, such as morpho-syntax and pragmatics (Stenström et al. 2002Stenström, Anna-Brita, Gisle Andersen, and Ingrid Hasund 2002Trends in Teenage Talk: Corpus Compilation, Analysis and Findings. Amsterdam, Philadelphia: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Tagliamonte 2016Tagliamonte, Sali 2016Teen Talk: The Language of Adolescents. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar).

A wide range of distinctive grammatical features have been identified as characteristic of London and British teenaged speaker (Stenström et al. 2002Stenström, Anna-Brita, Gisle Andersen, and Ingrid Hasund 2002Trends in Teenage Talk: Corpus Compilation, Analysis and Findings. Amsterdam, Philadelphia: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Cheshire et al. 2011Cheshire, Jenny, Paul Kerswill, Susan Fox, and Eivind Torgersen 2011 “Contact, the Feature Pool and the Speech Community: The Emergence of Multicultural London English”. Journal of Sociolinguistics 15 (2):151–196. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Palacios Martínez 2011aPalacios Martínez, Ignacio M. 2011a “The Language of British Teenagers. A Preliminary Study of its Main Grammatical Features”. Atlantis 33 (1): 105–126.Google Scholar; Tagliamonte 2016Tagliamonte, Sali 2016Teen Talk: The Language of Adolescents. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Drummond 2018Drummond, Rob 2018Researching Urban Youth Language and Identity. New York: Palgrave Macmillan. DOI logoGoogle Scholar): the use of intensifiers such as really, so, fucking to the detriment of the more standard very (Tagliamonte and Roberts 2005Tagliamonte, Sali, and Chris Roberts 2005 “ So weird; so cool; so innovative: The Use of Intensifiers in the Television Series Friends ”. American Speech 80 (3): 280–300. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Tagliamonte 2016Tagliamonte, Sali 2016Teen Talk: The Language of Adolescents. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Palacios Martínez and Núñez-Pertejo 2012Palacios Martínez, Ignacio M., and Paloma Núñez-Pertejo 2012 “ He’s absolutely massive. It’s a super day. Madonna, she is a wicked singer. Youth Language and Intensification. A Corpus-based Study”. Text and Talk 32 (6): 773–796. DOI logoGoogle Scholar); a high occurrence of vague terms (Cheshire 2007 2007 “Discourse, Variation, Grammaticalization and stuff like that ”. Journal of Sociolinguistics 11 (2): 155–193. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Tagliamonte and Denis 2010Tagliamonte, Sali, and Denis Derek 2010 “The stuff of Change: General Extenders in Toronto, Canada”. Journal of English Linguistics 38: 335–368. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Palacios Martínez and Núñez-Pertejo 2015 2015 “ Go up to Miss thingy. He’s probably like a whatsit or something. Placeholders in Focus. The Differences in Use between Teenagers and Adults in Spoken English”. Pragmatics 25 (3): 425–451. DOI logoGoogle Scholar); a variety of (invariant) tags and pragmatic markers such as innit, (do) you get me (Torgersen et. al. 2011Torgersen, Eivind N., Costas Gabrielatos, Sebastian Hoffmann, and Susan Fox 2011A Corpus-based Study of Pragmatic Markers in London English. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory 7 (1): 93–118. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Palacios Martínez 2015 2015 “Variation, Development and Pragmatic Uses of innit in the Language of British Adults and Teenagers”. English Language and Linguistics 19 (3): 383–405. DOI logoGoogle Scholar); a special quotative system including constructions with be (like), this is + pronoun (Tagliamonte and D’Arcy 2004Tagliamonte, Sali, and Alexandra D’Arcy 2004 “ He’s like She’s like. The Quotative System in Canadian Youth”. Journal of Sociolinguistics 8 (4): 493–514. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Fox 2012Fox, Susan 2012 “Performed Narrative: The Pragmatic Function of this is + Speaker and Other Quotatives in London Adolescent Speech”. In Quotatives: Cross-linguistic and Cross-disciplinary Perspectives. Converging Evidence in Language and Communication Research, ed. by Isabelle Buchstaller, and Ingrid van Alphen, 231–258. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar); the proliferation of vernacular negatives (Cheshire 1991Cheshire, Jenny 1991 “Variation in the Use of ain’t in an Urban British English Dialect”. In Dialects of English, ed. by Peter Trudgill, and J. K. Chambers, 54–73. London and New York: Longman.Google Scholar, 1999 1999 “English Negation from an International Perspective”. In Negation in the History of English, ed. by Ingrid Tieken-Boon van Ostade, Gunnel Tottie, and Wim van der Wurff, 29–53. Berlin and New York: Mouton. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Palacios Martínez 2011b 2011b “The Expression of Negation in British Teenagers’ Language: A Preliminary Study”. Journal of English Linguistics 39 (1): 4–35. DOI logoGoogle Scholar, 2016 2016 “ He don’t like football, does he? A Corpus-based Study of Third Person Singular don’t in the Language of British Teenagers”. In World Englishes: New Theoretical and Methodological Considerations, ed. by Elena Seoane, and Cristina Suárez-Gómez, 61–84. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar, 2017 2017 “Negative Concord in the Language of British Adults and Teenagers”. English World-Wide 38 (2): 153–181. DOI logoGoogle Scholar); a mode of expression crowded with address terms/vocatives familiarisers (man, brother, etc), taboo or offensive ones (bastard, dick) in particular (Leech 1999Leech, Geoffrey 1999 “The Distribution and Function of Vocatives in American and British English”. In Out of Corpora: Studies in Honour of Stig Johansson, ed. by Hasselgård, Hilde, and Signe Oksefjell, 107–120. Amsterdam: Rodopi.Google Scholar; Kiesling 2004Kiesling, Scott 2004 “Dude”. American Speech 79 (3): 281–305. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Rendle-Short 2010Rendle-Short, Johanna 2010 “ Mate as a Term of Address in Ordinary Interaction”. Journal of Pragmatics 42: 1201–1218. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Palacios Martínez 2018 2018 “ Help me move to that, blood. A Corpus-based Study of the Syntax and Pragmatics of Vocatives in the Language of British Teenagers”. Journal of Pragmatics 130: 33–50. DOI logoGoogle Scholar).

The latter will be the focus of the present study, which will be organised as follows: in Section 2 below, I will define the notion of ‘vocative’ with particular reference to taboo forms, followed by a review of the literature here, Section 3; Section 4 will set out the objectives and methodology of the study; findings will be described and discussed in Section 5; Section 6 will be concerned with the pragmatics of these terms, and the final section will offer some conclusions.

2.Taboo vocatives

Vocatives can be defined, broadly speaking, as a particular type of address term, specifically nouns (Braun 1988Braun, Friederike 1988Terms of Address. Mouton de Gruyter: Berlin, New York, Amsterdam. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; McCarthy and O’Keefe 2003McCarthy, Michael, and Anne O’Keefe 2003 “What’s in a Name? Vocatives in Casual Conversation and Radio Phone-in Calls”. In Corpus Analysis: Language Structure and Language Use, ed. by Pepi Leistna, and Charles Meyer, 153–185. Amsterdam: Rodopi. DOI logoGoogle Scholar), which are loosely integrated into the utterance (Leech 1999Leech, Geoffrey 1999 “The Distribution and Function of Vocatives in American and British English”. In Out of Corpora: Studies in Honour of Stig Johansson, ed. by Hasselgård, Hilde, and Signe Oksefjell, 107–120. Amsterdam: Rodopi.Google Scholar). That is, they are syntactically free forms outside the sentence construction (Braun 1988Braun, Friederike 1988Terms of Address. Mouton de Gruyter: Berlin, New York, Amsterdam. DOI logoGoogle Scholar, 11) and “denote the one or more persons to whom the sentence is addressed” (Quirk et al. 1985Quirk, Randolph, Sidney Greenbaum, Geoffrey Leech, and Jan Svartvik 1985A Comprehensive Grammar of the English Language. London: Longman.Google Scholar, 773).

(1)

I was like “Don’t drink it man ”. (LEC)11.Most of the examples included in the study have been extracted from the London English Corpus (LEC) and the Bergen Corpus of London Teenage Language (COLT). See Section 4 on aims and methodology for further details.

They very rarely take articles or any other determiners, and some of them may be preceded by a pronoun (you).

(2)

See you guys later. (COLT)

They can occur in different positions in the clause, be it initially, medially or finally, and in some cases they can even stand alone. They carry different pragmatic functions in discourse, and this may explain why at least some of them can be regarded as genuine pragmatic markers (McCarthy and O’Keefe 2003McCarthy, Michael, and Anne O’Keefe 2003 “What’s in a Name? Vocatives in Casual Conversation and Radio Phone-in Calls”. In Corpus Analysis: Language Structure and Language Use, ed. by Pepi Leistna, and Charles Meyer, 153–185. Amsterdam: Rodopi. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Clancy 2015Clancy, Brian 2015 “ Hurry up baby son all the boys is finished their breakfast. Examining the Use of Vocatives as Pragmatic Markers in Irish Traveller and Settled Family Discourse”. In Pragmatic Markers in Irish English, ed. by Carolina P. Amador-Moreno, Kevin McCafferty, and Elaine Vaughan, 229–247. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar).

Vocatives can be categorised into several groups, not only according to the meaning they convey and their formal features (Quirk et al. 1985Quirk, Randolph, Sidney Greenbaum, Geoffrey Leech, and Jan Svartvik 1985A Comprehensive Grammar of the English Language. London: Longman.Google Scholar; Leech 1999Leech, Geoffrey 1999 “The Distribution and Function of Vocatives in American and British English”. In Out of Corpora: Studies in Honour of Stig Johansson, ed. by Hasselgård, Hilde, and Signe Oksefjell, 107–120. Amsterdam: Rodopi.Google Scholar; Biber et al. 1999Biber, Douglas, Stig Johansson, Geoffrey Leech, Susan Conrad, and Edward Finegan 1999Longman Grammar of Spoken and Written English. Longman, London.Google Scholar; Huddleston, Pullum et al. 2002Huddleston, Rodney, Pullum, Geoffrey K. et al. 2002The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar), but also their function and context (Braun 1988Braun, Friederike 1988Terms of Address. Mouton de Gruyter: Berlin, New York, Amsterdam. DOI logoGoogle Scholar): terms of endearment (baby), family terms (mummy), names and titles (Mrs. Robinson, doctor), honorifics (sir), general plurals to address groups (folks), elaborate nominal clauses (those of you who want to come), familiarisers (man), and, finally, taboo vocatives, abusive or vocative terms of insult (Stenström 2006Stenström, Anna-Brita 2006 “Taboo Words in Teenage Talk”. Spanish in Context 3 (1): 115–138. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Stenström and Jørgensen 2008Stenström, Anna-Brita, and Annette Myre Jørgensen 2008 “A Matter of Politeness? A Contrastive Study of Phatic Talk in Teenage Conversation”. Pragmatics 18 (4): 635–657. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Rodríguez-González and Stenström 2011Rodríguez-González, Félix, and Anna-Brita Stenström 2011 “Expressive Devices in the Language of English and Spanish-speaking Youth”. Revista Alicantina de Estudios Ingleses 24: 235–256. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Fägersten 2012Fägersten, Kristy Beers 2012Who’s Swearing Now? The Social Aspects of Conversational Swearing. Newcastle: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.Google Scholar), such as bastard, dick(head), bitch, etc. The degree of acceptance or appropriateness of these address terms can vary according to the context and/or the individual speaker. Some words, such as twat, idiot and fool, are seen to be broadly acceptable, whereas others, such as bastard and motherfucker, may be of more restricted use, in that they are considered to be offensive to some extent. On these lines, it might be useful to bear in mind Anderson and Trudgill’s (1990Andersson, Lars, and Peter Trudgill 1990Bad Language. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar, 55) observation that no word is bad in itself, since quality of badness is something that the users of the language will themselves decide upon in a given context. For the purposes of the present study, a wide range of terms, encompassing various degrees of acceptability, have been included within the group of taboo vocatives, since they all express something pejorative and derogatory, and share a number of syntactic, semantic and pragmatic features, as will be discussed below. Note the following:

(3)

Shut up dickhead . (COLT)

Taboo vocatives, as with general ones, occur mainly after statements, although are also found directly after questions.

(4)

You know that, bitch ? (COLT)

They may also occur, and in fact often do, after imperatives and directives, and in such cases might have a mitigating or strengthening effect, depending on the context.

(5)

Shut up you prick . (LEC)

As with other vocatives, they may occur in any sentence position, although final position is by far the most common, as the examples above illustrate. In the following I will define taboo vocatives in more detail.

3.Literature review

Address terms have been explored in depth (Braun 1988Braun, Friederike 1988Terms of Address. Mouton de Gruyter: Berlin, New York, Amsterdam. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Leech 1999Leech, Geoffrey 1999 “The Distribution and Function of Vocatives in American and British English”. In Out of Corpora: Studies in Honour of Stig Johansson, ed. by Hasselgård, Hilde, and Signe Oksefjell, 107–120. Amsterdam: Rodopi.Google Scholar), often focusing on their role and behaviour at the discourse level, and looking at their degree of integration into the clause and their pragmatic functions. However, corpus analysis studies, using large data sets, are in fact quite scarce, although the following are available: Leech (1999)Leech, Geoffrey 1999 “The Distribution and Function of Vocatives in American and British English”. In Out of Corpora: Studies in Honour of Stig Johansson, ed. by Hasselgård, Hilde, and Signe Oksefjell, 107–120. Amsterdam: Rodopi.Google Scholar; Biber et al. (1999)Biber, Douglas, Stig Johansson, Geoffrey Leech, Susan Conrad, and Edward Finegan 1999Longman Grammar of Spoken and Written English. Longman, London.Google Scholar; Clancy (2015)Clancy, Brian 2015 “ Hurry up baby son all the boys is finished their breakfast. Examining the Use of Vocatives as Pragmatic Markers in Irish Traveller and Settled Family Discourse”. In Pragmatic Markers in Irish English, ed. by Carolina P. Amador-Moreno, Kevin McCafferty, and Elaine Vaughan, 229–247. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; McCarthy and O’Keefe (2003)McCarthy, Michael, and Anne O’Keefe 2003 “What’s in a Name? Vocatives in Casual Conversation and Radio Phone-in Calls”. In Corpus Analysis: Language Structure and Language Use, ed. by Pepi Leistna, and Charles Meyer, 153–185. Amsterdam: Rodopi. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Heyd (2010)Heyd, Theresa 2010 “ How you guys Doin? Staged Orality and Emerging Plural Address in the Television Series Friends ”. American Speech 85 (1): 33–66. DOI logoGoogle Scholar and Palacios Martínez (2018) 2018 “ Help me move to that, blood. A Corpus-based Study of the Syntax and Pragmatics of Vocatives in the Language of British Teenagers”. Journal of Pragmatics 130: 33–50. DOI logoGoogle Scholar.

The study by Brown and Ford (1961)Brown, Roger, and Marguerite Ford 1961 “Address Terms in American English”. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 62, 375–385. DOI logoGoogle Scholar is regarded as seminal in the examination of nominal address in American English. Leech (1999)Leech, Geoffrey 1999 “The Distribution and Function of Vocatives in American and British English”. In Out of Corpora: Studies in Honour of Stig Johansson, ed. by Hasselgård, Hilde, and Signe Oksefjell, 107–120. Amsterdam: Rodopi.Google Scholar compares the use of vocatives in British and American English, and concludes that it is in the latter that such terms appear most frequently in final position, thus contributing to greater familiarisation between speakers. In addition to these studies, other work has dealt with address terms in particular text types, such as telephone calls to radio programmes (McCarthy and O’Keefe 2003McCarthy, Michael, and Anne O’Keefe 2003 “What’s in a Name? Vocatives in Casual Conversation and Radio Phone-in Calls”. In Corpus Analysis: Language Structure and Language Use, ed. by Pepi Leistna, and Charles Meyer, 153–185. Amsterdam: Rodopi. DOI logoGoogle Scholar), family discourse (Wilson and Zeitlyn 1995Wilson, Andrew J., and David Zeitlyn 1995 “The Distribution of Person-referring Expressions in Natural Conversation”. Research on Language and Social Interaction 28 (1): 61–92. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Clancy 2015Clancy, Brian 2015 “ Hurry up baby son all the boys is finished their breakfast. Examining the Use of Vocatives as Pragmatic Markers in Irish Traveller and Settled Family Discourse”. In Pragmatic Markers in Irish English, ed. by Carolina P. Amador-Moreno, Kevin McCafferty, and Elaine Vaughan, 229–247. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar), political debates (Jaworski and Galasiński 2000Jaworski, Adam, and Dariusz Galasiński 2000 “Vocative Address Forms and Ideological Legitimisation in Political Debates”. Discourse Studies 2 (1): 35–53. DOI logoGoogle Scholar), and grime music (Adams 2018Adams, Zoe 2018 “ I don’t know why man’s calling me family all of a sudden: Address and Reference Terms in Grime Music.” Language and Communication 60: 11–27. DOI logoGoogle Scholar). All these have shown how the pragmatic function of vocatives changed according to discourse type. Thus, Clancy (2015)Clancy, Brian 2015 “ Hurry up baby son all the boys is finished their breakfast. Examining the Use of Vocatives as Pragmatic Markers in Irish Traveller and Settled Family Discourse”. In Pragmatic Markers in Irish English, ed. by Carolina P. Amador-Moreno, Kevin McCafferty, and Elaine Vaughan, 229–247. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar clearly illustrates that vocatives play an important pragmatic role in family discourse, and that they are indeed more important than traditional pragmatic markers. Meanwhile, Jaworski and Galasiński (2000)Jaworski, Adam, and Dariusz Galasiński 2000 “Vocative Address Forms and Ideological Legitimisation in Political Debates”. Discourse Studies 2 (1): 35–53. DOI logoGoogle Scholar examined political discourse, and showed that the role of vocatives is closely related to the image that political leaders seek to project.

Several further studies have looked in detail at a single item: Luckmann de Lopez (2013)Luckmann de Lopez, Kathrin 2013 “Clause-final man in Tyneside English”. In English Corpus Linguistics: Variation in Time, Space and Genre, ed. by Gisle Andersen, and Kristin Beach, 139–162. Amsterdam: Rodopi. DOI logoGoogle Scholar focuses on the vocative man in Tyneside English in its function as a distinctive feature of this northern variety of British English; Cheshire (2013) 2013 “Grammaticalisation in Social Context: The Emergence of a New English Pronoun”. Journal of Sociolinguistics 17 (5): 608–633. DOI logoGoogle Scholar does the same for Multicultural London English, providing evidence of the adoption of features typical of the pronoun category; Heyd (2010)Heyd, Theresa 2010 “ How you guys Doin? Staged Orality and Emerging Plural Address in the Television Series Friends ”. American Speech 85 (1): 33–66. DOI logoGoogle Scholar considers the address expression you guys in the television series Friends, Kiesling (2004)Kiesling, Scott 2004 “Dude”. American Speech 79 (3): 281–305. DOI logoGoogle Scholar looks at dude in the conversations of young American speakers, in which it serves as a means of expressing solidarity, and Rendle-Short (2010)Rendle-Short, Johanna 2010 “ Mate as a Term of Address in Ordinary Interaction”. Journal of Pragmatics 42: 1201–1218. DOI logoGoogle Scholar considers the use of mate in Australian English, which was found in the speech of young men and women.

Despite the extensive literature on general vocatives described above, taboo vocatives in particular have enjoyed far less attention. To my knowledge, there are no monographic studies here, with only brief references within more general studies on teen talk (Stenström et al. 2002Stenström, Anna-Brita, Gisle Andersen, and Ingrid Hasund 2002Trends in Teenage Talk: Corpus Compilation, Analysis and Findings. Amsterdam, Philadelphia: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Drummond 2018Drummond, Rob 2018Researching Urban Youth Language and Identity. New York: Palgrave Macmillan. DOI logoGoogle Scholar), phatic language (Stenströnm and Jørgensen 2008Stenström, Anna-Brita, and Annette Myre Jørgensen 2008 “A Matter of Politeness? A Contrastive Study of Phatic Talk in Teenage Conversation”. Pragmatics 18 (4): 635–657. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Rodríguez-González and Stenström 2011Rodríguez-González, Félix, and Anna-Brita Stenström 2011 “Expressive Devices in the Language of English and Spanish-speaking Youth”. Revista Alicantina de Estudios Ingleses 24: 235–256. DOI logoGoogle Scholar) and swearing and taboo words (Risch 1987Risch, Barbara 1987 “Women’s Derogatory Terms for Men: That’s Right, Dirty Words”. Language in Society 16: 353–358. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Anderson and Trudgill 1990Andersson, Lars, and Peter Trudgill 1990Bad Language. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar; De Klerk 1992De Klerk, Vivian 1992 “How Taboo Are Taboo Words for Girls?Language in Society 21: 277–289. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Stenström 2006Stenström, Anna-Brita 2006 “Taboo Words in Teenage Talk”. Spanish in Context 3 (1): 115–138. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Fägersten 2012Fägersten, Kristy Beers 2012Who’s Swearing Now? The Social Aspects of Conversational Swearing. Newcastle: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.Google Scholar; Mateo and Yus 2013Mateo, José, and Francisco Yus 2013 “Towards a Cross-cultural Pragmatic Taxonomy of Insults”. Journal of Language Aggression and Conflict 1 (1): 87–114. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Adams 2016Adams, Michael 2016In Praise of Profanity. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar; Bergen 2016Bergen, Benjamin K. 2016What the F: What Swearing Reveals about Our Language, Our Brains, and Ourselves. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar; Schweinberger 2018Schweinberger, Martin 2018 “Swearing in Irish English. A Corpus-based Quantitative Analysis of the Sociolinguistics of Swearing”. Lingua 209: 1–20. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Drummond 2019 2019 “Teenage Swearing in the UK”. English Worldwide 41 (1): 59–88. DOI logoGoogle Scholar). In what follows I will briefly describe the main studies in this area, although in the majority of cases taboo vocatives are dealt with only in passing.

Anderson and Trudgill (1990)Andersson, Lars, and Peter Trudgill 1990Bad Language. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar devote considerable space to swearing and slang, and they make some brief references to taboo address terms. Thus, they claim that vocatives of the bastard and bitch type have an abusive function since they are often derogatory, including here name-calling and curses (1990Andersson, Lars, and Peter Trudgill 1990Bad Language. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar, 61).

Rodríguez-González (2002Rodríguez-González, Félix 2002Comunicación y cultura juvenil. Barcelona: Ariel.Google Scholar, 48), in his study of Spanish youth language, notes that a number of taboo vocatives, such as cabrón “bastard” and maricón “homo/gay” are sometimes used with an affectionate tone rather than with their original offensive and insulting meaning. Stenström et al. (2002)Stenström, Anna-Brita, Gisle Andersen, and Ingrid Hasund 2002Trends in Teenage Talk: Corpus Compilation, Analysis and Findings. Amsterdam, Philadelphia: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar note that the use of such lexical items may vary according to the gender group of speakers.

In fact, the gender factor in the use of taboo words has been the central issue in a number of studies. It has been traditionally accepted that males tend to make a greater use of these terms than females, in that women have in general been considered as more socially and linguistically conservative, and more closely associated with the norms of the standard. However, evidence for this in some studies has been neither conclusive nor fully convincing. In this vein, Risch (1987)Risch, Barbara 1987 “Women’s Derogatory Terms for Men: That’s Right, Dirty Words”. Language in Society 16: 353–358. DOI logoGoogle Scholar discusses women’s use of derogatory terms, “dirty words”, to refer to men. The findings question the traditional assumption that women tend to stick to standard forms of speech, and also the fact that some of the terms cited were generally thought to refer to females rather than males. Five years later De Klerk (1992)De Klerk, Vivian 1992 “How Taboo Are Taboo Words for Girls?Language in Society 21: 277–289. DOI logoGoogle Scholar confirms Risch’s previous findings. Finally, Schweinberger (2018)Schweinberger, Martin 2018 “Swearing in Irish English. A Corpus-based Quantitative Analysis of the Sociolinguistics of Swearing”. Lingua 209: 1–20. DOI logoGoogle Scholar claims that in Irish English abusive cases such as you fucking bastard are substantially less frequent than other uses amounting to only 2.4% of the total in his data.

The issue of gender from a contrastive perspective is also of concern in Stenström and Jørgensen’s (2008)Stenström, Anna-Brita, and Annette Myre Jørgensen 2008 “A Matter of Politeness? A Contrastive Study of Phatic Talk in Teenage Conversation”. Pragmatics 18 (4): 635–657. DOI logoGoogle Scholar study on phatic talk in the language of Spanish and English teenagers. They find that taboo words prevail in male talk, are almost twice as frequent as in comparable female contexts, and are used as reaction signals, being more common than the actual interjections.

More recently, Rodríguez-González and Stenström (2011)Rodríguez-González, Félix, and Anna-Brita Stenström 2011 “Expressive Devices in the Language of English and Spanish-speaking Youth”. Revista Alicantina de Estudios Ingleses 24: 235–256. DOI logoGoogle Scholar note that the teenagers from Madrid tend to use far more vocatives in general, and taboo vocatives in particular, when addressing one another than do London teens.

The urban variety used by teenagers from Manchester has been the focus of an ethnographic study by Drummond (2019) 2019 “Teenage Swearing in the UK”. English Worldwide 41 (1): 59–88. DOI logoGoogle Scholar who focuses on 13 key swear words and finds that dick is the term which shows the most distinctive differences according to gender use, being preferred mainly by males over females, while the opposite is true for bastard, which is more typical of female talk.

In light of the previous overview, a gap in the literature in this area of taboo vocatives has been identified, which this study will try to fill.

4.Aims and methodology

The first aim of the present study is to identify the most common taboo vocatives in the language of British teenagers. Are they as frequent as familiarisers such as man, brother, guy, etc.? Do these speakers make use of a wide variety of them, or are they restricted to a small number, these being frequently repeated? In connection to this, what are the possible reasons or factors accounting for the high frequency of these terms in teen talk? Second, I will explore the extent to which differences exist in terms of the frequency and use of such terms between the speech of teenagers and adults. Third, I will look for any changes in the use of these vocatives in recent times, that is, from the 1990s to the first decade of the current century, since our data will be from these two periods. Fourth, I am also concerned with the meaning of these taboo vocatives and how they are actually used in spontaneous conversations, paying special attention to the type of referents, animate or inanimate, that they have. Finally, I will consider their position in the sentence and examine how far this position has a bearing on their pragmatics.

This paper forms part of a more extensive project on the study of the spoken language of London teenagers (13 to 20 years old). Both a corpus-based and a corpus-driven approach were adopted, analysing data drawn primarily from the London English Corpus (LEC), the Bergen Corpus of London Teenage Language (COLT) and the British National Corpus (BNC) (See Table 1) but also using as a starting-point material from previous studies together with the dictionaries OED and the Urban Dictionary.

Table 1.Corpora used in the analysis
Corpus Number of speakers Speakers’ ages Geographical area Number of words Collection method and material Compilation date
COLT (The Bergen Corpus of London Teenage Language)  33 13 to 17 Hackney, Tower Hamlets, Camden, Barnet, Havering, etc.  438,531

Subjects recorded themselves.

Spontaneous conversations

early 1990s
LEC (London English Corpus) young 149 12 to 20 Hackney and Havering 1,208,135 Sociolinguistic group interviews recorded by field workers 2004–2007
2007–2010
BNC (British National Corpus), adult sample 151 25 to 60+ London area  278,246 Oral recordings. Conversations 1990s (1991 onwards)
LEC (London English Corpus) adult 52 21–70 Hackney and Havering  460,022 Sociolinguistic group interviews recorded by field workers 2004–2007
2007–2010

The LEC was compiled by Cheshire and her team in London between 2004 and 2010 (Cheshire et al. 2011Cheshire, Jenny, Paul Kerswill, Susan Fox, and Eivind Torgersen 2011 “Contact, the Feature Pool and the Speech Community: The Emergence of Multicultural London English”. Journal of Sociolinguistics 15 (2):151–196. DOI logoGoogle Scholar) and is formed by the Linguistic Innovators Corpus (LIC) and the Multicultural London English Corpus (MLEC). The data for the former corpus was collected between 2004 and 2007 and both teenagers and adult speech are represented. MLEC, compiled between 2007 and 2010, contains data not only from young speakers but also from small children, as well as from young and elderly adults. In addition to LEC, I also analysed data extracted from COLT, compiled at the University of Bergen (Stenström et al. 2002Stenström, Anna-Brita, Gisle Andersen, and Ingrid Hasund 2002Trends in Teenage Talk: Corpus Compilation, Analysis and Findings. Amsterdam, Philadelphia: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar), and BNC. COLT contains some 438,531 words and is part of the BNC. It was collected in 1993 and consists of a total of 377 spontaneous conversations recorded by the teenagers themselves in the London area. Thus, the information here can be regarded as more natural than that provided by LEC, in which field workers were used to record mainly individual and group interviews. Data extracted from COLT will be contrasted with comparable samples taken from the spoken component of BNC itself, namely conversations by adult speakers of the London area, since these correspond to the same period and share a number of features, with the purpose of drawing correspondences between our findings and mainstream adult British English.

The different dates of compilation of the corpora used also give us an overview of the evolution of these vocatives, especially in terms of their frequency of use. However, for a correct interpretation of the data, differences in compilation methods across the different corpora should be borne in mind.

All tokens retrieved had to be filtered manually and, as a result, a large number of examples were discarded. The lack of access to the sound files was a problem, since intonation plays an important role in the use of vocatives and it is of particular importance when looking at their position in the sentence and their pragmatic value.

Moreover, it was sometimes difficult to determine the vocative position in the utterance, something that has been reported elsewhere (McCarthy and O’Keefe 2003McCarthy, Michael, and Anne O’Keefe 2003 “What’s in a Name? Vocatives in Casual Conversation and Radio Phone-in Calls”. In Corpus Analysis: Language Structure and Language Use, ed. by Pepi Leistna, and Charles Meyer, 153–185. Amsterdam: Rodopi. DOI logoGoogle Scholar, 158; Clancy 2015Clancy, Brian 2015 “ Hurry up baby son all the boys is finished their breakfast. Examining the Use of Vocatives as Pragmatic Markers in Irish Traveller and Settled Family Discourse”. In Pragmatic Markers in Irish English, ed. by Carolina P. Amador-Moreno, Kevin McCafferty, and Elaine Vaughan, 229–247. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar), and also to interpret the pragmatic meaning conveyed by some of these address terms, since the information deduced from the context is not always sufficient.

5.Findings

5.1Frequency

I found a wide variety of taboo vocatives in the data, a total of 59, most of these being present in both corpora. In a previous study on familiarisers (Palacios Martínez 2018 2018 “ Help me move to that, blood. A Corpus-based Study of the Syntax and Pragmatics of Vocatives in the Language of British Teenagers”. Journal of Pragmatics 130: 33–50. DOI logoGoogle Scholar) in the same mode of expression, I identified a total of 16, so the present finding of 59 taboo vocatives here is indeed very broad. They also occurred quite frequently, with an overall normalised frequency of 32.70 per 100,000 words. However, this proportion is considerably lower than that for familiarisers, and I can thus state that the latter are much more commonly used than taboo vocatives, although fewer in terms of members of the category.

As can be seen in the first column of Table 2, all these items are generally used as insult words in everyday language and they function as address terms in about 1/4 of the total, varying in their frequency according to each item. For example, in the case of (son of) a bitch, from a total of 193 recorded tokens, 66 are vocatives (34%); for bastard, from a total of 143, I find 52 uses (36%) as address terms.

Table 2.Frequency of taboo vocatives in each of the corpora studied
COLT
438,531 words
LEC young
1,208,135 words
Total
1,646,666 words
TN NF TV NF TN NF TV NF TN NF TV NF
ass/arsehole    18   4.10     6   1.36    12   0.99     1   0.08    30   1.82     7    0.42
bastard   116  26.45    43   9.80    27   2.23     9   0.74   143   8.68    52    3.15
batty     4   0.91     1   0.22    10   0.82     4   0.33    14   0.85     5    0.30
big/bean/crack/fish/peanuthead     3   0.68     3   0.68    34   2.81    10   0.82    37   2.24    13    0.78
black     2   0.45     2   0.45     3   0.24     2   0.16     5   0.30     4    0.24
bugger    18   4.10    15   3.42   –  –   –  –    18   1.09    15    0.91
chav   –  –   –  –     7   0.57     1   0.08     7   0.42     1    0.06
chicken     2   0.45     2   0.45     3   0.24   –  –     5   0.30     2    0.12
chopper     1   0.22     1   0.22   –  –   –  –     1   0.06     1    0.06
cow     6   1.36     2   0.45    11   0.91     3   0.24    17   1.03     5    0.34
crap    21   4.78     4   0.91    17   1.40   –  –    38   2.30     4    0.24
cunt    54  12.31    20   4.56    27   2.23    18   1.48    81   4.91    38    2.30
dick/cock(head) (arse)    91  20.75    18   4.10    48   3.97    28   2.31   139   8.44    46    2.79
div(vy)     7   1.59     1   0.22     1   0.08   –  –     8   0.48     1    0.06
dumb    18   4.10     1   0.22    58   4.80   –  –    76   4.61     1    0.06
fag(got)     2   0.45     1   0.22   –  –   –  –     2   0.12     1    0.06
fool    51  11.62    19   4.33    11   0.91     5   0.41    62   3.76    24    1.45
freak     2   0.45   –  –    28   2.31     5   0.41    30   1.82     5    0.30
geek   –  –   –  –     7   0.57     1   0.08     7   0.42     1    0.06
git    15   3.42     8   1.82     5   0.41     2   0.16    20   1.21    10    0.60
goon   –  –   –  –    24   1.98     2   0.16    24   1.45     2    0.12
hussy     1   0.22   –  –     1   0.08     1   0.08     2   0.12     1    0.06
idiot    25   5.70     9   2.05    71   5.87    12   0.99    96   5.82    21    1.27
joker     2   0.45   –  –    19   1.57     4   0.33    21   1.27     4    0.24
knob     8   1.82     2   0.45     9   0.74     2   0.16    17   1.03     4    0.24
liar    19   4.33     8   1.82    13   1.07     5   0.41    32   1.94    13    0.78
(mother)fucker    12   2.73     9   2.05     8   0.66     1   0.08    20   1.21    10    0.60
moron   –  –   –  –     2   0.16     2   0.16     2   0.12     1    0.06
mug     3   0.68   –  –    14   1.15     8   0.66    17   1.03     8    0.48
muppet   –  –   –  –     4   0.33     2   0.16     4   0.24     2    0.12
nigger    13   2.96     5   1.14     2   0.16   –  –    15   0.91     5    0.30
nutter     6   1.36     1   0.22     9   0.74     1   0.08    15   0.91     2    0.12
paedophile     1   0.22   –  –     6   0.49     2   0.16     7   0.43     2    0.12
paki     4   0.91     2   0.45     6   0.49     1   0.08    10   0.60     3    0.18
pervert    13   2.96     2   0.45    20   1.64     3   0.32    33   1.98     5    0.34
pig(head)     2   0.45     1   0.22     1   0.08   –  –     3   0.18     1    0.06
poof(utter)puff     5   1.14     4   0.91     3   0.24     1   0.08     8   0.48     5    0.30
prat    12   2.73     6   1.36     2   0.16     1   0.08    14   0.85     7    0.42
prick    14   3.19     3   0.68    33   2.73     7   0.57    47   2.85    10    0.60
pussy(hole)     3   0.68   –  –    22   1.82     2   0.16    25   1.51     2    0.12
scum     2   0.45     2   0.45    12   0.99     2   0.16    14   0.85     4    0.24
shit    24   5.47     4   0.91    31   2.56     6   0.48    55   3.34    10    0.60
slob   –  –   –  –     2   0.16     1   0.08     2   0.12     1    0.07
slag    14   3.19     6   1.36    35   2.89     1   0.08    49   2.97     7    0.42
slut     5   1.14   –  –     8   0.66     1   0.08    13   0.78     1    0.07
sod    24   5.47    18   4.10     2   0.16     2   0.16    26   1.57    20    1.21
(son of a)bitch   104  23.71    44  10.03    89   7.36    22   1.82   193  11.72    66  4
spud   –  –   –  –     1   0.08     1   0.08     1   0.06     1    0.06
stupid   215  49.02    34   7.75   196  16.22     2   0.16   411  24.95    36    2.18
tart    12   2.73     4   0.91    12   0.99     2   0.16    24   1.45     6    0.36
tit     1   0.22     1   0.22   –  –   –  –     1   0.06     1    0.06
tosser     7   1.59     1   0.22     2   0.16     1   0.08     9   0.54     2    0.12
tramp     2   0.45   –  –    24   1.98     3   0.24    26   1.57     3    0.18
twat     9   2.05     8   1.82     1   0.08     1   0.08    10   0.60     9    0.54
wanker    33   7.52    10   2.28    12   0.99     3   0.32    45   2.73    13    0.78
wasteman/wastegashgasman   –  –   –  –    19   1.57     3   0.24    19   1.15     3    0.18
whore     7   1.59     2   0.45    13   1.07     1   0.08    20   1.21     3    0.18
wimp   –  –   –  –     4   0.33     2   0.16     4   0.24     2    0.12
zombie   –  –   –  –     6   0.49     6   0.49     6   0.36     6    0.36
Total 1,033 235.55   333  75.92 1,027  86.64   206  17.11 2,080 126.29   538  32.7

TN: Total number; NF: Normalised frequency; TV: Taboo vocatives

As can be seen in Table 3, there are high degrees of coincidence across the two groups of teenagers regarding those taboo vocatives with the highest frequency. Overall, the 7 most common are, in this order: (son of a) bitch, bastard, dick(head), cunt, stupid, fool and idiot.

Table 3.Most frequent taboo vocatives in the language of London teenagers
COLT LEC young
(son of a) bitch dick(head)
bastard (son of a) bitch
stupid cunt
cunt idiot
fool bighead
dick bastard

5.2Evolution over time

Although the data show that taboo vocatives are far more common in COLT (75.92 per 100,000 words) than in LEC (17.11), I cannot assume from this that they were more frequent in the 1990s than in the first decade of the present century. As mentioned above, differences observed here may have to do with the research instruments used for our research, and more particularly with the method followed for the compilation of each of the corpora: whereas in COLT participants recorded themselves (spontaneous conversations), LEC is based on individual and group sociolinguistic interviews carried out by field-workers. This difference in the compilation method might itself have had a bearing on the data collected, and thus on any subsequent analysis across corpora.

When comparing the data from the two corpora, certain differences are observed. Some terms (bugger, chicken, chopper, crap, div, dumb, fag/got, nigger, pig(head, tit)) are found in COLT but not in LEC, and vice versa; chav, geek, goon, mup, muppet, tramp, wasteman and zombie, for example, are recorded only in LEC. However, no firm conclusions can be drawn from this since the numbers of tokens for most of these are quite limited.

5.3Meaning

All of the above terms can be regarded in their literal sense as offensive to a greater or lesser extent, although, as we will see below, there are cases in which they lose their power to insult and might even be used in an affectionate way.

The majority of them convey some kind of sexual reference, including male or female genitals and associated terms (dick(head), cunt) or sexual behaviour/identity with pejorative connotations (wanker, bitch, whore, slut, hussy, tart). Some also refer to some unusual forms of behaviour, with the meanings mad/bizarre (stupid, twat, nutter) or worthless (prat, wasteman). Animal references with a pejorative meaning are also quite common, as in bitch, cow, chicken; finally, some terms referring to race, often with clear racist overtones, are also recorded, these including nigger and paki.

Most of these terms have undergone semantic change in the sense of a widening or broadening of their meaning (Campbell 1998Campbell, Lyle 1998Historical Linguistics: An Introduction. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar, 216). As a result, the range of meanings of these vocatives has increased so that they can be used in more contexts than was the case before such a change. For example, bitch no longer refers to a female dog but to an unpleasant person, although it might also refer to a girlfriend, as in (6), although on this occasion it is a metalinguistic use rather than a fully spontaneous one. The same applies to prick and dick, which are used in their non-literal sense. Witness the following:

(6)

Interviewer.: ok what would be the informal name for a girlfriend?

Dave (17 years, male): girlfriend really

Int.: just girlfriend you haven’t got any other terms that you use?

Dave: bitch <laugh> that’s what you call them sometimes like “that’s my bitch ” or “that’s my girl” [Int: oh right] basically it’s my girl. (LEC)

5.4Referents of taboo vocatives

The effects of this broadening of meaning can also be observed in the personal references they denote. Note how speakers sometimes use bitch and cunt with male referents, whereas the contrary would be expected; something similar happens with dick and prick, which can be used to address female speakers. The following is an exchange between Dean and Chris. Note how Chris addresses Dean first as cunt and a few lines down the latter addresses Chris as dickhead.

(7)

Dean (17 years, male): I’ll class myself as mixed race as well then

Chris (17 years, male): #1 my dad is black you cunt so is my grandad /so I can say whatever I want/ #

Dean: #2 /so?. my godsister’s black /so I can say whatever I want #

Chris: #1 she’s mixed race /(name of person) is mixed race/ #

Dean: #2 /ok/ my godfather’s black #

Chris: that’s your godfather that’s not actually your blood

Dean: doesn’t matter. can say whatever I want then. you muggy dickhead. (LEC)

Also, the referents of address terms such as these can be a third person (8) or even a group of people, as in (9), where the referents are not present and do not participate in the conversation, although they may be involved directly or indirectly in the chat, either because the speakers are talking about them or are referring to something concerning them. Here the taboo vocative functions as a kind of aside or comment; in this respect it should probably not be considered as a prototypical example, but rather a less central one, in that it mainly represents the speaker’s thinking at the time, and the reference to the interlocutor is less direct. Consider the following:

(8)

Andrew (17 years): he opened the door # laugh # and. the fella said “come then” and the boy got in the car with them oh stupid dickhead /[ he got in there? ] / yeah [ why? ] he got in the car. (LEC)

(9)

cos we still looking for a club to get in. idiots wouldn’t let us in. terrible. terrible she was (LEC)

Although less common, these address terms can even have non-human or inanimate referents such as human body parts (10) or an object such as a telephone (11). These are mainly non-literal and tend to have a humorous touch, this derived from the personification of the inanimate referents.

(10)

i walk along brentwood high street with my friend and i hold her arm and i say do you want to turn this off i say “walk you bastard ” and she said “don’t say that” I’m talking to my legs [ yeah ] she says “people think your talking to me” # laugh # (LEC)

(11)

Chris (17): cos I’m quite powerful as everyone knows yeah my phone just flew and hit my table. hit all the chargers everything like that. picked it up i looked at it and i went. “ you bastard ”. cos all my numbers are saved on the phone. (LEC)

5.5Teenagers versus adults

Taboo vocatives are almost non-existent in the language of adults. Only 16 tokens were recorded, and these occurred in similar proportions in the two adult corpora (BNC subcorpus and LEC adult), together comprising over 730,000 words, with a normalised frequency per 100,000 words of 2.16. These terms are idiot (9), sod (3), stupid (2) cow (1), dick (1). So, taboo vocatives can be regarded as more typical of teen talk, and indeed are recorded in speakers as young as 12, that is, at an early age in the development of their interactional repertoires. However, the question that emerges here is exactly why so many more taboo vocatives are found in teen talk than in the language of adults?

Social relationships among peers and in-group identity are fundamental to teenagers. These speakers have a particularly urgent need to know that they form part of their own peer-group, in terms of being accepted by others and even by themselves, an issue that has been widely reported in the literature (Eckert 1988Eckert, Penelope 1988 “Adolescent Social Structure and the Spread of Linguistic Change”. Language in Society 17: 183–207. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Stenström et al. 2002Stenström, Anna-Brita, Gisle Andersen, and Ingrid Hasund 2002Trends in Teenage Talk: Corpus Compilation, Analysis and Findings. Amsterdam, Philadelphia: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Stenström and Jørgensen 2009 eds. 2009Youngspeak in a Multilingual Perspective. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Tagliamonte 2016Tagliamonte, Sali 2016Teen Talk: The Language of Adolescents. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Drummond 2018Drummond, Rob 2018Researching Urban Youth Language and Identity. New York: Palgrave Macmillan. DOI logoGoogle Scholar, 2019 2019 “Teenage Swearing in the UK”. English Worldwide 41 (1): 59–88. DOI logoGoogle Scholar). Taboo vocatives very often serve to establish, maintain and reinforce these relationships and can be considered a part of their special code or mode of expression. As Tagliamonte claims (2016Tagliamonte, Sali 2016Teen Talk: The Language of Adolescents. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar, 53), “the two most powerful forces that govern language use in adolescence [are]: (1) solidarity with peers and (2) separation from adults.” Furthermore, teenagers react more spontaneously than adults and there is an especially direct and tangible emotional function associated with such address terms. Moreover, these terms are very closely aligned with informal and colloquial registers.

5.6Taboo vocatives: Further features

In this section I will describe further features of the members of this category in view of our findings. As such, this description will complement the one presented in the introduction.

Taboo vocatives are used in the singular mainly, although a few cases are also recorded in the plural, as in the case of bastards, dickheads, idiots, pricks. They are also quite frequently used in reported speech; hence they are not reserved for any special uses or communicative conditions, and we can say that speakers consider them to be typical of their everyday expression.

(12)

i nearly/went mad i was like “you little bastard get out my house”. (LEC)

I also find some metalinguistic uses, such as (6) above, in which the speakers themselves talk about the meaning and use of these vocatives, whether they are really offensive or not, and who uses them most frequently.

Taboo vocatives may occur together with other common address terms (e.g. man, bruv) in the same turn unit (13). Note how in these cases the taboo vocative does not generally convey any offensive meaning, since the familiarizer cancels out or mitigates any offense conveyed by the term.

(13)

Dexter (male, 18 years): in your head but where’s that tough girl man? dickhead bruv that tough girl where is she? //Dexter kisses teeth// she’s not in there. (LEC)

They may even be found together with the referent’s personal name, this being a means by which the speaker singles out the addressee.

(14)

Shut up Miguel you stupid little motherfucking . (COLT)

Sometimes speakers make use of a number of these address terms together, often with an intensifying meaning, which itself derives from this accumulation of vocatives (15), or as a means of showing aggression to others, as in (16), in which the situation involves a bus driver closing the doors before they can get on, resulting in the terms uttered. This will be further discussed in Section 6 when I turn to pragmatics.

(15)

because of the fact that I know her ah fuck off.. cunt dickhead …. (LEC)

(16)

you’re running up the the thing yeah, as soon as you get there yeah, they just close the doors and go, just like when Mike, and Steve were here, and er, they are going wanker, fucking wanker , you cunt , you cunt . (COLT)

These terms are often modified by intensifying adverbs or adjectives, such as (mother)fucking, little, diddy, stupid, fat, dirty, silly, loud, rotten, sad, queer, dumb, two-faced, old, smelly, bloody, ugly, bad, poor; all of these, with the exception of little, carry pejorative connotations.

(17)

I thought “who you getting rude to you little fat cunt ?”. (LEC)

These are the main collocations found with each of them.

Table 4.Main collocations of the most frequent taboo vocatives in the language of London teenagers
fucking arsehole
Irish, little, mercenary, sad, fat, rotten, queer, silly, black, fucking bastard
fucking, silly, dumb, dirty, two-faced, stupid, little, lying, old, smally, fat, diddy, effing, sad bitch
bloody, right, stupid, boring old, dirty, little, nosey, greedy cow
fat, ugly, motherfucking, lying, little, flipping, dirty cunt
fucking, little, stupid, muggy dick(head)
poor, bad, little fuckers
damn, stupid, loud, little idiot
bloody, fucking, sod, stupid, silly, poor, lucky prick
fucking tosser/wanker

As mentioned above, they can be preceded by you, and in fact this is the case almost half of the time (46.01%) (see Table 5). For cow, bastard and idiot, this figure is even higher, at 80%, 71% and 52%, respectively, although the low number of tokens here means that firm conclusions cannot be drawn.

Table 5.Taboo vocatives preceded by you
COLT
438,531 words
LEC Young
(1,208,135 words)
Total
1,646,666 words
TN Preceded by you % TN Preceded by you % TN Preceded by you %
ass/arse(hole)   6   1    16.66%   1   1 100%   7   2    28.57%
bastard  43  30    69.76%   9   7    77.77%  52  37    71.15%
big/bean/crack/peanut/fish head   3   1    33.33%  10   4 40%  13   5    38.46%
black   2   1  50%   2   1 50%   4   2 50%
bugger  15   1     6.66%  –  –  –  15   1     6.66%
cow   2   2 100%   3   2    66.66%   5   4  80%
crap   4   1  25%  –  –  –   4   1  25%
cunt  20   8  40%  18  10    55.55%  38  18    47.36%
dick/cock (head) (arse)  18  14  77.77  28   7  25%  46  21    45.65%
dumb   1   1 100%  –  –  –   1   1 100%
fag(got)   1   1 100%  –  –  –   1   1 100%
fool  19   8    42.10%   5   2  40%  24  10    41.66%
freak  –  –  –   5   5 100%   5   5 100%
git   8   5   62.5%   2  –  –  10   5  50%
goon  –  –  –   2   1  50%   2   1  50%
idiot   9   6    66.66%  12   5    41.66%  21  11    52.38%
liar   8   2  25%   5   4  80%  13   6    46.15%
(mother)fucker   9   1    11.11%   1  –  –  10   1  10%
mug  –  –  –   8   5   62.5%   8   5   62.5%
muppet  –  –  –   2   2 100%   2   2 100%
nutter   1   1 100%   1   1 100%   2   2 100%
paedophile  –  –  –   2   2 100%   2   2 100%
pervert   2   1  50%   3   2    66.66%   5   3  60%
prat   6   5    83.33%   1  –  –   7   5    71.42%
prick   3   1    33.33%   7   4    57.14%  10   5  50%
pussy(hole)  –  –  –   2   2 100%   2   2 100%
shit   4  –  –   6   1    16.66%  10   1  10%
slag   6   4    66.66%   1  –  –   7   4    57.14%
slut  –  –  –   1   1 100%   1   1 100%
sod  18   1     5.55%   2   2 100%  20   3  15%
(son of a) bitch  44  13    29.54%  22  11  50%  66  24    36.36%
stupid  34  14    41.17%   2  –  –  36  14    38.88%
tart   4   2  50%   2  –  –   6   2    33.33%
tit   1   1 100%  –  –  –   1   1 100%
tramp  –  –  –   3   2    66.66%   3   2    66.66%
twat   8   5   62.5%   1  –  –   9   5    55.55%
wanker  10   6  60%   4   1  25%  14   7  50%
wasteman/wastegash  –  –  –   3   1    33.33%   3   1    33.33%
whore   2  –  –   1   1 100%   3   1    33.33%
wimp  –  –  –   1   1 100%   1   1 100%
Total 311 137    44.05% 178  88    49.40% 489 225    46.01%

When preceded by you, taboo vocatives are recorded mainly in final position. There seem to be two main roles for this pronoun when it comes to taboo vocatives: to single out or focus attention on the referent (18) or to intensify the message conveyed (19).

(18)

You look around ҂ ҂2 /look and they’re like “what you looking at you bitch ? (LEC)

(19)

Oh shut up you prick , you can’t do that. (LEC)

5.7Taboo Vocatives Position

As can be seen from Table 6 and Figure 1, my findings confirm previous studies on the sentence position of vocatives in general (Biber et al. 1999Biber, Douglas, Stig Johansson, Geoffrey Leech, Susan Conrad, and Edward Finegan 1999Longman Grammar of Spoken and Written English. Longman, London.Google Scholar; Leech 1999Leech, Geoffrey 1999 “The Distribution and Function of Vocatives in American and British English”. In Out of Corpora: Studies in Honour of Stig Johansson, ed. by Hasselgård, Hilde, and Signe Oksefjell, 107–120. Amsterdam: Rodopi.Google Scholar; McCarthy and O’Keefe 2003McCarthy, Michael, and Anne O’Keefe 2003 “What’s in a Name? Vocatives in Casual Conversation and Radio Phone-in Calls”. In Corpus Analysis: Language Structure and Language Use, ed. by Pepi Leistna, and Charles Meyer, 153–185. Amsterdam: Rodopi. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Clancy 2015Clancy, Brian 2015 “ Hurry up baby son all the boys is finished their breakfast. Examining the Use of Vocatives as Pragmatic Markers in Irish Traveller and Settled Family Discourse”. In Pragmatic Markers in Irish English, ed. by Carolina P. Amador-Moreno, Kevin McCafferty, and Elaine Vaughan, 229–247. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Palacios Martínez 2018 2018 “ Help me move to that, blood. A Corpus-based Study of the Syntax and Pragmatics of Vocatives in the Language of British Teenagers”. Journal of Pragmatics 130: 33–50. DOI logoGoogle Scholar). Final position is far more frequent than initial position and occurs with a shorter length than the latter. Initial position is associated mainly with gaining attention, whereas final position is more closely related to interpersonal issues and the maintenance and reinforcement of social relationships. When found in initial position, vocatives are frequently followed by a directive or a request.

(20)

You fool , turn it on now. (COLT) initial

(21)

Cos we were still looking for a club to get in. idiots wouldn’t let us in. (LEC) middle

(22)

Hey buggered off. Bastards ! (COLT) final

This pattern is reflected in all the taboo vocatives under investigation in the present study, with no exceptions. Fool is the item found with the highest proportion in initial position (29.1%).

Table 6.Clause position of the most common taboo vocatives
Initial Medial Final Total
(son of a) bitch 1/1.5%  –  65/98.5%  66
bastard 1/1.5%  –  51/98.5%  52
dick (head) 1/2.2% 3/6.6%  41/91.2%  45
cunt  – 1/2.6%  37/97.4%  38
stupid  –  –  36/100%  36
fool 7/29.1%  –  1770.9%  24
idiot  – 1/4.7%  20/95.3%  21
bugger  –  –  15/100%  15
liar  –  –  13/100%  13
wanker  –  –  13/100%  13
Total 10/3.1% 5/1.5% 308/95.4% 323

Figure 1 visually represents the different positions of the most frequent taboo vocatives in the present data.

Figure 1.Clause position of the five most common taboo vocatives
Figure 1.

6.Pragmatics

Taboo vocatives, like vocatives in general, are pragmatically loaded and have a multifunctional role in discourse. Although an exhaustive outline is difficult, two main broad groups of functions can be distinguished (McCarthy and O’Keefe 2003McCarthy, Michael, and Anne O’Keefe 2003 “What’s in a Name? Vocatives in Casual Conversation and Radio Phone-in Calls”. In Corpus Analysis: Language Structure and Language Use, ed. by Pepi Leistna, and Charles Meyer, 153–185. Amsterdam: Rodopi. DOI logoGoogle Scholar; Clancy 2015Clancy, Brian 2015 “ Hurry up baby son all the boys is finished their breakfast. Examining the Use of Vocatives as Pragmatic Markers in Irish Traveller and Settled Family Discourse”. In Pragmatic Markers in Irish English, ed. by Carolina P. Amador-Moreno, Kevin McCafferty, and Elaine Vaughan, 229–247. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar):

  • Those that serve to express interpersonal relationships between participants in a conversation: insult or offence, disagreement, humour, expression of contempt and envy, reinforcement of identity and peer relationship, etc.

  • Those that help to organise discourse in terms of engaging a conversation participant and managing topics (shift, expand, close) and turns (identify and interrupt speaker).

These two categories of functions are not mutually exclusive. That is, there are cases in which taboo address terms not only channel the expression of interpersonal relationships but also serve to organise discourse by closing the topic or interrupting the turn (see Section 6.2 below).

6.1Interpersonal relationships

6.1.1Offensive

These terms often function as straightforward insults (Sosa 2018Sosa, David 2018Bad Words: Philosophical Perspectives on Slurs. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar) and are used by teenagers with a clearly offensive purpose.

The following extracts are a good illustration of episodes in which these terms attain an offensive value through a reaction to some sort of disagreement with, or a lack of consideration shown by, the interlocutor. In (23) the speaker is reporting how he felt when a friend told him about the drowning of a family member (the person who drowned was one of his cousin’s sons). Note how in the account, in which there is alternation between direct and reported speech, the speaker uses the taboo vocative “silly old cow” to give more expressive force to his story.

(23)

he said to me “there’s so. a member one of you cousins or one of your family drowned” and when i come out i said to my mate “ silly old cow bleeding drowned no-ones drowned in my house” but it was it was my cousins little boy he got drowned on erm (LEC)

At times there is even an added value, in that these address terms, apart from being offensive, may also serve to reinforce the message (24) or even to engage the interlocutor’s attention abruptly (25). The two following extracts involve two exchanges where speakers argue about the recording they are doing and the materials they are using to do it. As noted above (cf. Section 4), participants in COLT recorded themselves with devices which they had been given. This situation led to a number of conflicts, and these were often discussed by them during the recordings.

(24)

Russel (male 15 years): Can you hear me?

Alistair (male, 15 years): You screwed up that tape you dick . (COLT)

(25)

Catriona (female, 16 years): I’ve only done two s= two tapes I’ve, where where are the other tapes?

Jess (female, 16 years): I don’t know.

Catriona: What on earth have I done with [them?]

Jess: [They’re] over here you dick . (COLT)

6.1.2Arguments and disagreements

It is also relatively common for these address terms to emerge over the course of arguments and disagreements between participants in a conversation. In the following extract, for example, the speakers maintain different views as to how good women can be at karate. At the end of the interaction, one of the speakers, Richard, responds with a taboo vocative (you prat), possibly because no further arguments occur to him in support of his views, but also because he is not interested in the discussion and seeks to close the topic and move onto another. Hence, what we find in the use of the vocative here is a combination of an interpersonal and discourse functions.

(26)

Ben (male, 13 years): What Emma’s gone on holiday?

Richard (male, 13 years): Yeah

Francais (female, 13 years): I, I’ve

Ben: Oh

Francais: Oi right erm, don’t tell her I told you this but she’d kill me <unclear> she <mimicking Japanese accent> the karate! </>, she’s beating me up

Richard: Women can’t do

Ben: I bet she is

Richard: karate you prat

Francais: some women

Ben: Why not? (COLT)

6.1.3To reinforce bonds and create solidarity and comradeship

In this respect, taboo terms are no different from familiarisers such as man, bro, dude, and this explains their high frequency in teen talk. In the following example, one of the speakers invites a boy to join a group that is chatting. The taboo vocative (you dickhead) in initial position serves to engage attention but also to prepare the ground and promote a positive atmosphere, so that he can accept the invitation and join them. Note the use of the familiarizer son in the following line which also serves to strengthen this feeling of comradeship and solidarity. The taboo vocatives here, then, serve to favour the conditions for his taking part in the conversation.

(27)

Sulema (female, 18 years): call (name) call (name=Will) call (name=Will)

Ryan (male, 18): (name=Will) won’t come in

Sulema: why?

Ryan: cos as soon as I say to him (name=Will) come in the room he says not again. guarantee you.

Sulema: go on say it

Ryan: was in here for like

Sulema: can you call him?..

Ryan: let me

Sulema: he’s been in here a few times

Ryan: that’s it I’ve only been in here once. Twice

Sulema: oh there he is. <shouts> (name) #1 (name).. (name=Will) #

Ryan: #2 (name=Will) /[Int.: is that (name)?]/ you dickhead you # fancy having (name=Will). wanna have a chat son ?. <laughter> I’ve never seen him look like that. ah.. right yeah (LEC)

6.1.4To express affection

This function is, clearly, the opposite of the offensive function. Here the taboo vocative reverses its meaning completely, conveying something positive rather than offensive or negative. In the following example, the speaker is telling an anecdote from the past, and is referring to a four-year-old boy, emphasising how “gorgeous” and “nice” he is, as in contrast to being “butters”, that is, “ugly”, the boy’s probable current status, which seems to denote the opposite. Note once again how the speaker alternates direct and reported speech.

(28)

Ahmed (male, 19): like four years he’s probably butters now anyway. people do change over time….he is gorgeous I’m telling you.first time I see him I just fell in love with him I was like “you’re fucking gorgeous you bastard ”.. he is nice ain’t he? (LEC)

6.1.5Expression of contempt and envy

Here the speaker conveys a feeling which can be described as a mixture of both contempt and envy, in that her interlocutor is going to be bought a car by her father. As a reaction to hearing this, she responds “Ah bitch”.

(29)

Kath (female, 17 years): Guess what my dad told me

Claire: (female, 17 years): What?

Kath: He says erm he said he might er might be able to get me a car for the Christmas holidays

Claire: Ah! Bitch . (COLT)

6.1.6Badinage (humorous and witty)

Humour is present in many conversations among teenagers, who are very fond of telling stories, dramatizing experiences, and talking about their peers or other members of their families and inner circle. This humour takes different forms: jokes, puns, amusing anecdotes and stories, playing with the language, mimicking and imitations of other accents plus exchanges where they make fun of each another, either directly or indirectly. Episode (30) is a good example of this. Jonathan and Rosh express different views as to the purpose of what they are recording, and they go on to engage in an exchange in which they pull each other’s legs by insulting each other, as if this were the most natural thing to do.

(30)

Ryan: Recording as we’re speaking English, we can learn to speak English again.

Jonathan (male, 17 years): <nv>laugh</nv>

Josh: <laughing>Oh yeah</>.

Jonathan: That’s the plot. That’s it, the secret’s out.

Rob: <nv>laugh</nv> …

Jonathan: Turn it on.

Rob: It is.

Jonathan: It’s on?

Rob: Jonathan’s a dancing queen.

Josh: <laughing>Yeah, <unclear></> you dickhead .

Rob: Jonathan’s a queen, period.

Jonathan: Yeah your mum’s a queen. (COLT)

6.2Organise discourse

Although the use of taboo vocatives as a resource to plan and organize discourse is not as commonplace as their use in relation to the expression of interpersonal relationships among speakers, I do find some examples in the data, particularly cases where a speaker is interested in closing the turn and changing the topic. This can be seen in (31) below, where the participants in the exchange have clearly differing views: Jonathan wants to go back to school, whereas none of the other speakers are attracted to this idea. After a brief and unproductive exchange of views in which several speakers intervene, John decides to stop the conversation by using the swear word fuck followed by the taboo vocative bitch. In this way he underlines the fact that he is not willing to continue arguing about this, seeking instead to change the topic.

(31)

Jonathan (male, 17 years): We gotta go back to school.

Ryan (male, 19 years): I won’t.

Jonathan: Have you got a course?

Ryan: We have an hour.

Jonathan: Oh.

Rob (male, 19 years): <unclear> fuck, off.

Jonathan: Fuck you I’ve gotta go back for chemistry.

Josh (male, 17 years): Fuck that bitch .

Jonathan: Yeah er. (COLT)

7.Conclusions

Taboo vocatives can be regarded as a typical feature of teen talk (a total of 59 types were recorded in the data), in that they are very common in the language of these speakers, in contrast to the very scant presence of such items in comparable data on adult speech. Most of these are nouns, although they may also function as adjectives. They are associated with sexual (dick, slut), inappropriate (sod, wanker) or bizarre behaviour (nutter, freak, twat), and some others also refer to animals pejoratively (bitch, cow, pig) or are directly or indirectly racist (nigger, paki).

While bearing in mind divergences in compilation methods used in the corpora here, the figures do seem to indicate that no major differences are found when comparing the most common taboo vocatives from the 1990s with those of the first decade of the current century, although some new terms seem to have emerged, including chav, geek, tramp and wasteman, and some others appear to be gradually becoming outdated, such as bugger and sod. Most items terms have suffered semantic change in the sense of a broadening or widening of their meaning (i.e. bitch, prick, dick), and some have come to allow both male and female referents, when originally, due to their meaning, they were more closely associated with a particular gender.

There are also some examples in which the referent is inanimate, a body part or a telephone for example, leading to the discourse acquiring a humorous tone derived from the personification of these referents. At times the referent of the taboo vocative may not itself be present. That is, a speaker might address a third person who does not participate in the conversation, yet is the topic of the discussion for some reason. In these cases, the taboo vocative functions an aside or indirect remark.

Taboo vocatives are found quite often in reported speech and may occur with other address terms, mainly man, mate and brother/bruv. Here the familiarizer tones down the possible offensive meaning of the taboo word. Taboo vocatives are very frequently modified by certain adverbs and adjectives: fucking, little, fat, etc. In 46.01% of cases they are preceded by you, this broadly in line with the frequency of other familiarisers (you guys).

Taboo address terms are found mainly in final position, as is the case with most vocatives. In terms of pragmatics they are multifunctional, in that they serve not only to express intersubjective relationships between the participants in the conversation but may also help to organize discourse. However, in this case intersubjective relationships clearly prevail over the discourse ones. Contrary to what might be expected, taboo vocatives are not always offensive or negative, and indeed can denote affection and badinage, this very often to reinforce the bonds between teenaged speakers through the underlining of their special code, thus helping them to reinforce their identity and to distinguish themselves from others through their use of language.

Funding

This research has been funded by the Spanish Ministry of Science, Innovation and Universities (PGC2018-093622-B-100), by the Conselleria de Cultura, Educación e Ordenación Universitaria, Xunta de Galicia (grant ED431B 2018/05) and by the European Regional Development Fund (PGC2018-093622 B-100).

Note

1.Most of the examples included in the study have been extracted from the London English Corpus (LEC) and the Bergen Corpus of London Teenage Language (COLT). See Section 4 on aims and methodology for further details.

References

Adams, Michael
2016In Praise of Profanity. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Adams, Zoe
2018 “ I don’t know why man’s calling me family all of a sudden: Address and Reference Terms in Grime Music.” Language and Communication 60: 11–27. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Andersson, Lars, and Peter Trudgill
1990Bad Language. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Androutsopoulos, Jannis J., and Alexandra Georgakopoulos
2003Discourse Constructions of Youth Identities. John Benjamins: Amsterdam. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bergen, Benjamin K.
2016What the F: What Swearing Reveals about Our Language, Our Brains, and Ourselves. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Biber, Douglas, Stig Johansson, Geoffrey Leech, Susan Conrad, and Edward Finegan
1999Longman Grammar of Spoken and Written English. Longman, London.Google Scholar
Braun, Friederike
1988Terms of Address. Mouton de Gruyter: Berlin, New York, Amsterdam. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brown, Roger, and Marguerite Ford
1961 “Address Terms in American English”. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 62, 375–385. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Campbell, Lyle
1998Historical Linguistics: An Introduction. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Cheshire, Jenny
1991 “Variation in the Use of ain’t in an Urban British English Dialect”. In Dialects of English, ed. by Peter Trudgill, and J. K. Chambers, 54–73. London and New York: Longman.Google Scholar
1999 “English Negation from an International Perspective”. In Negation in the History of English, ed. by Ingrid Tieken-Boon van Ostade, Gunnel Tottie, and Wim van der Wurff, 29–53. Berlin and New York: Mouton. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2007 “Discourse, Variation, Grammaticalization and stuff like that ”. Journal of Sociolinguistics 11 (2): 155–193. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2013 “Grammaticalisation in Social Context: The Emergence of a New English Pronoun”. Journal of Sociolinguistics 17 (5): 608–633. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cheshire, Jenny, Paul Kerswill, Susan Fox, and Eivind Torgersen
2011 “Contact, the Feature Pool and the Speech Community: The Emergence of Multicultural London English”. Journal of Sociolinguistics 15 (2):151–196. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clancy, Brian
2015 “ Hurry up baby son all the boys is finished their breakfast. Examining the Use of Vocatives as Pragmatic Markers in Irish Traveller and Settled Family Discourse”. In Pragmatic Markers in Irish English, ed. by Carolina P. Amador-Moreno, Kevin McCafferty, and Elaine Vaughan, 229–247. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Klerk, Vivian
1992 “How Taboo Are Taboo Words for Girls?Language in Society 21: 277–289. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Drummond, Rob
2018Researching Urban Youth Language and Identity. New York: Palgrave Macmillan. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2019 “Teenage Swearing in the UK”. English Worldwide 41 (1): 59–88. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eckert, Penelope
1988 “Adolescent Social Structure and the Spread of Linguistic Change”. Language in Society 17: 183–207. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fägersten, Kristy Beers
2012Who’s Swearing Now? The Social Aspects of Conversational Swearing. Newcastle: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.Google Scholar
Fox, Susan
2012 “Performed Narrative: The Pragmatic Function of this is + Speaker and Other Quotatives in London Adolescent Speech”. In Quotatives: Cross-linguistic and Cross-disciplinary Perspectives. Converging Evidence in Language and Communication Research, ed. by Isabelle Buchstaller, and Ingrid van Alphen, 231–258. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Heyd, Theresa
2010 “ How you guys Doin? Staged Orality and Emerging Plural Address in the Television Series Friends ”. American Speech 85 (1): 33–66. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huddleston, Rodney, Pullum, Geoffrey K. et al.
2002The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jaworski, Adam, and Dariusz Galasiński
2000 “Vocative Address Forms and Ideological Legitimisation in Political Debates”. Discourse Studies 2 (1): 35–53. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kiesling, Scott
2004 “Dude”. American Speech 79 (3): 281–305. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leech, Geoffrey
1999 “The Distribution and Function of Vocatives in American and British English”. In Out of Corpora: Studies in Honour of Stig Johansson, ed. by Hasselgård, Hilde, and Signe Oksefjell, 107–120. Amsterdam: Rodopi.Google Scholar
Luckmann de Lopez, Kathrin
2013 “Clause-final man in Tyneside English”. In English Corpus Linguistics: Variation in Time, Space and Genre, ed. by Gisle Andersen, and Kristin Beach, 139–162. Amsterdam: Rodopi. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mateo, José, and Francisco Yus
2013 “Towards a Cross-cultural Pragmatic Taxonomy of Insults”. Journal of Language Aggression and Conflict 1 (1): 87–114. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McCarthy, Michael, and Anne O’Keefe
2003 “What’s in a Name? Vocatives in Casual Conversation and Radio Phone-in Calls”. In Corpus Analysis: Language Structure and Language Use, ed. by Pepi Leistna, and Charles Meyer, 153–185. Amsterdam: Rodopi. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Palacios Martínez, Ignacio M.
2011a “The Language of British Teenagers. A Preliminary Study of its Main Grammatical Features”. Atlantis 33 (1): 105–126.Google Scholar
2011b “The Expression of Negation in British Teenagers’ Language: A Preliminary Study”. Journal of English Linguistics 39 (1): 4–35. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2015 “Variation, Development and Pragmatic Uses of innit in the Language of British Adults and Teenagers”. English Language and Linguistics 19 (3): 383–405. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2016 “ He don’t like football, does he? A Corpus-based Study of Third Person Singular don’t in the Language of British Teenagers”. In World Englishes: New Theoretical and Methodological Considerations, ed. by Elena Seoane, and Cristina Suárez-Gómez, 61–84. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2017 “Negative Concord in the Language of British Adults and Teenagers”. English World-Wide 38 (2): 153–181. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2018 “ Help me move to that, blood. A Corpus-based Study of the Syntax and Pragmatics of Vocatives in the Language of British Teenagers”. Journal of Pragmatics 130: 33–50. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Palacios Martínez, Ignacio M., and Paloma Núñez-Pertejo
2012 “ He’s absolutely massive. It’s a super day. Madonna, she is a wicked singer. Youth Language and Intensification. A Corpus-based Study”. Text and Talk 32 (6): 773–796. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2015 “ Go up to Miss thingy. He’s probably like a whatsit or something. Placeholders in Focus. The Differences in Use between Teenagers and Adults in Spoken English”. Pragmatics 25 (3): 425–451. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Quirk, Randolph, Sidney Greenbaum, Geoffrey Leech, and Jan Svartvik
1985A Comprehensive Grammar of the English Language. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Rendle-Short, Johanna
2010 “ Mate as a Term of Address in Ordinary Interaction”. Journal of Pragmatics 42: 1201–1218. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Risch, Barbara
1987 “Women’s Derogatory Terms for Men: That’s Right, Dirty Words”. Language in Society 16: 353–358. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rodríguez-González, Félix
2002Comunicación y cultura juvenil. Barcelona: Ariel.Google Scholar
Rodríguez-González, Félix, and Anna-Brita Stenström
2011 “Expressive Devices in the Language of English and Spanish-speaking Youth”. Revista Alicantina de Estudios Ingleses 24: 235–256. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schweinberger, Martin
2018 “Swearing in Irish English. A Corpus-based Quantitative Analysis of the Sociolinguistics of Swearing”. Lingua 209: 1–20. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sosa, David
2018Bad Words: Philosophical Perspectives on Slurs. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Spiegel, Carmen, and Daniel Gysin
(eds.) 2016Jugendsprache in Schule, Medien und Alltag (Sprache – Kommunikation – Kultur 19). Frankfurt am Main: Lang Verlag. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stenström, Anna-Brita
2006 “Taboo Words in Teenage Talk”. Spanish in Context 3 (1): 115–138. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stenström, Anna-Brita, Gisle Andersen, and Ingrid Hasund
2002Trends in Teenage Talk: Corpus Compilation, Analysis and Findings. Amsterdam, Philadelphia: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stenström, Anna-Brita, and Annette Myre Jørgensen
2008 “A Matter of Politeness? A Contrastive Study of Phatic Talk in Teenage Conversation”. Pragmatics 18 (4): 635–657. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
eds. 2009Youngspeak in a Multilingual Perspective. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tagliamonte, Sali
2016Teen Talk: The Language of Adolescents. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tagliamonte, Sali, and Alexandra D’Arcy
2004 “ He’s like She’s like. The Quotative System in Canadian Youth”. Journal of Sociolinguistics 8 (4): 493–514. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tagliamonte, Sali, and Denis Derek
2010 “The stuff of Change: General Extenders in Toronto, Canada”. Journal of English Linguistics 38: 335–368. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tagliamonte, Sali, and Chris Roberts
2005 “ So weird; so cool; so innovative: The Use of Intensifiers in the Television Series Friends ”. American Speech 80 (3): 280–300. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Torgersen, Eivind N., Costas Gabrielatos, Sebastian Hoffmann, and Susan Fox
2011A Corpus-based Study of Pragmatic Markers in London English. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory 7 (1): 93–118. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilson, Andrew J., and David Zeitlyn
1995 “The Distribution of Person-referring Expressions in Natural Conversation”. Research on Language and Social Interaction 28 (1): 61–92. DOI logoGoogle Scholar

Dictionaries and corpora

BNC: British National Corpus. version 3 (BNC XML Edition)
2007Distributed by Bodleian Libraries, University of Oxford, on behalf of the BNC Consortium. URL: http://​www​.natcorp​.ox​.ac​.uk/ (last accessed, 24 March, 2020)
COLT. Bergen Corpus of London Teenage Language
1993Stesntröm, Anna-Brita et al. University of Bergen.Google Scholar
LEC. London English Corpus
2004–2010Cheshire, Jenny et al. Queen Mary University of London.Google Scholar
OED: Oxford English Dictionary online edition
Urban Dictionary
URL: https://​www​.urbandictionary​.com/ (last accessed, 24 March, 2020)

Address for correspondence

Ignacio M. Palacios Martínez

Facultad de Filología – Departamento de Filología Inglesa y Alemana

University of Santiago de Compostela

Avda. de Castelao s/n

15782 Santiago de Compostela

Spain

[email protected]

Biographical notes

Ignacio M. Palacios Martínez obtained his Ph.D. in English in 1992 from the University of Santiago de Compostela (Spain). Since 1995 he has been working first as associate professor and now as full professor in the English Department of the same university. At present he is the principal investigator of a research project concerned with the description of English in the new digital genres in native and non-native contexts. He was also Head of the University’s Modern Language Centre between 2007 and 2010 and Secretary of the Spanish Association of Anglo-American Studies (AEDEAN) from 2004 to 2009.