Syntactic processing in sight translation by professional and trainee interpreters: Professionals are more time-efficient while trainees view the source text less

Agnieszka Chmiel and Agnieszka Lijewska
Adam Mickiewicz University in Poznań

Abstract

The study examines how professional and trainee interpreters process syntax in sight translation. We asked 24 professionals and 15 trainees to sight translate sentences with subject-relative clauses and more difficult object-relative clauses while measuring translation accuracy, eye movements and translation durations. We found that trainees took longer to achieve similar translation accuracy as professionals and viewed the source text less than professionals to avoid interference, especially when reading more difficult object-relative sentences. Syntactic manipulation modulated translation and viewing times: participants took longer to translate object-relative sentences but viewed them less in order to avoid interference in target language reformulations. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to show that reading measures in sight translation should be analysed together with translation times to explain complex reading patterns. It also proposes a new measure, percentage of dwell time, as an index of interference avoidance.

Keywords:
Publication history
Table of contents

When performing sight translation interpreters have to obtain input by reading a text in the visual modality in order to express its meaning orally. Unlike simultaneous interpreting, which is speaker-paced, sight translation is interpreter-paced (Agrifoglio 2004). However, the goal is the same – the target text should reach the target audience with no delay. Just as in simultaneous interpreting, when faced with a written text interpreters have to complete the necessary syntactic reformulation in order to provide a natural-sounding target text. However, unlike in simultaneous interpreting, in sight translation the source text is always available in front of the interpreter’s eyes. The presence of the source text is a double-edged sword: on the one hand, it reduces the memory load because all the information is available; however, on the other hand, it might lead to interference because of the constant visual access to the verbal form of the source text. It may also entail another disadvantage, namely divided attention. The transient nature of the source text in simultaneous interpreting naturally forces attention away from the source text. In sight translation, attention is divided between the ever-present source text and the emerging target text. It seems that experience should make interpreters more immune to interference and should lead to more restructuring, resulting in a more natural target text. Thanks to the widespread use of modern process research methods such as eye-tracking, we can now tap into the reading and processing of text in sight translation by tracking the eye movements of the interpreters. In the present study, we asked professional and trainee interpreters to perform sight translation. The participants’ eye movements and sight translation performance were recorded to examine how the participants cope with syntactic processing while completing the task. We operationalised syntactic processing through sentences including subject-relative and object-relative clauses. We expected to find an effect of experience, that is, professional interpreters being more efficient and successful in syntactic processing than trainee interpreters, and an effect of syntactic complexity, with more complex syntactic structures entailing longer viewing times and longer translation durations than less complex structures. The novel contribution of this study is the application of a combination of common eye-tracking measures and translation durations to explain the participants’ reading patterns in sight translation. We also propose a new eye-tracking measure, percentage of dwell time, which could help to interpret data patterns recorded when eye-tracking the process of sight translation.

Full-text access is restricted to subscribers. Log in to obtain additional credentials. For subscription information see Subscription & Price. Direct PDF access to this article can be purchased through our e-platform.

References

Agrifoglio, Marjorie
2004 “Sight Translation and Interpreting: A Comparative Analysis of Constraints and Failures.” Interpreting 6 (1): 43–67. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Andrews, Glenda, Damian Birney, and Graeme S. Halford
2006 “Relational Processing and Working Memory Capacity in Comprehension of Relative Clause Sentences.” Memory & Cognition 34 (6): 1325–1340. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barr, Dale J., Roger Levy, Christoph Scheepers, and Harry J. Tily
2013 “Random Effects Structure for Confirmatory Hypothesis Testing: Keep It Maximal.” Journal of Memory and Language 68 (3): 255–278. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, Douglas, Reinhold Kliegl, Shravan Vasishth, and Harald Baayen
2015 “Parsimonious Mixed Models.” Arxiv Preprint. https://​arxiv​.org​/abs​/1506​.04967
Caplan, David, Sujith Vijayan, Gina Kuperberg, Caroline West, Gloria Waters, Doug Greve, and Anders M. Dale
2002 “Vascular Responses to Syntactic Processing: Event-Related fMRI Study of Relative Clauses.” Human Brain Mapping 15 (1): 26–38. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Caplan, David, and Gloria Waters
2013 “Memory Mechanisms Supporting Syntactic Comprehension.” Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 20 (2): 243–268. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chmiel, Agnieszka
2018 “In Search of the Working Memory Advantage in Conference Interpreting – Training, Experience and Task Effects.” International Journal of Bilingualism 22 (3): 371–384. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chmiel, Agnieszka, Przemysław Janikowski, and Anna Cieślewicz
Forthcoming. “The Eye or the Ear? Source Language Interference in Sight Translation and Simultaneous Interpreting.” Interpreting. International Journal of Research and Practice in Interpreting.
Chmiel, Agnieszka, and Iwona Mazur
2013 “Eye Tracking Sight Translation Performed by Trainee Interpreters.” In Tracks and Treks in Translation Studies, edited by Catherine Way, Sonia Vandepitte, Reine Meylaerts, and Magdalena Bartłomiejczyk, 189–205. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Díaz-Galaz, Stephanie, Presentacion Padilla, and María Teresa Bajo
2015 “The Role of Advance Preparation in Simultaneous Interpreting: A Comparison of Professional Interpreters and Interpreting Students.” Interpreting 17 (1): 1–25. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dragsted, Barbara, and Inge G. Hansen
2009 “Exploring Translation and Interpreting Hybrids. The Case of Sight Translation.” Meta 54 (3): 588–604. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Garcia, Adolfo M., Augustin Ibanez, David Huepe, Alexander L. Houck, Maëva Michon, Carlos G. Lezama, Sumeer Chadha, and Alvaro Rivera-Rei
2014 “Word Reading and Translation in Bilinguals: The Impact of Formal and Informal Translation Expertise.” Frontiers in Psychology 5: 1302.Google Scholar
Gernsbacher, Morton A., and Miriam Shlesinger
1997 “The Proposed Role of Suppression in Simultaneous Interpretation.” Interpreting 2 (1–2): 119–140. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibson, Edward
2000 “The Dependency Locality Theory: A Distance-Based Theory of Linguistic Complexity.” In Image, Language, Brain, edited by Yashusi Miyashita, Alec P. Marantz, and Wayne O’Neil, 95–126. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gile, Daniel
2009Basic Concepts and Models for Interpreter and Translator Training. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hejwowski, Krzysztof
2004Kognitywno-Komunikacyjna Teoria Przekładu [Cognitive-communicative theory of translation]. Warszawa: Wydawnictwo Naukowe PWN.Google Scholar
Jakobsen, Arnt L., and Kristian Jensen
2008 “Eye Movement Behaviour across Four Different Types of Reading Task.” In Looking at Eyes – Eye Tracking Studies of Reading and Translation Processing, edited by Susane Göpferich, Arnt L. Jakobsen, and Inger Mees, 103–124. Copenhagen: Samfundslitteratur.Google Scholar
Jones, Roderick
2002Conference Interpreting Explained. Manchester: St. Jerome.Google Scholar
Jörg, Udo
1997 “Bridging the Gap: Verb Anticipation in German-English Simultaneous Interpreting.” In Translation as Intercultural Communication, edited by Mary Snell-Hornby, Zuzana Jettmarová, and Klaus Kaindl, 217–228. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
King, Jonathan, and Marcel Adam Just
1991 “Individual Differences in Syntactic Processing: The Role of Working Memory.” Journal of Memory and Language 30 (5): 580–602. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lemhöfer, Kristin, and Mirjam Broersma
2012 “Introducing Lextale: A Quick and Valid Lexical Test for Advanced Learners of English.” Behavior Research Methods 44 (2): 325–343. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liu, Minhua, Diane L. Schallert, and Patrick J. Carroll
2004 “Working Memory and Expertise in Simultaneous Interpreting.” Interpreting 6 (1): 19–42. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liversedge, Simon P., Kevin B. Paterson, and Martin J. Pickering
1998 “Eye Movements and Measures of Reading Time.” In Eye Guidance in Reading and Scene Perception, edited by Geoffrey Underwood, 55–75. Oxford: Elsevier. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Macizo, Pedro, and Maria T. Bajo
2004 “When Translation Makes the Difference: Sentence Processing in Reading and Translation.” Psicologica: International Journal of Methodology and Experimental Psychology 25 (1): 181–205.Google Scholar
Maier, Robert M., Martin J. Pickering, and Robert J. Hartsuiker
2017 “Does Translation Involve Structural Priming?Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 70 (8): 1575–1589. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pöchhacker, Franz
2004Introducing Interpreting Studies. London: Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
R: A Language and Environment For Statistical Computing
R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna.
Reichle, Erik D., Simon P. Liversedge, Alexander Pollatsek, and Keith Rayner
2009 “Encoding Multiple Words Simultaneously in Reading Is Implausible.” Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13 (3): 115–119. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Riccardi, Alessandra
1996 “Language-Specific Strategies in Simultaneous Interpreting.” In Teaching Translation and Interpreting 3, edited by Cay Dollerup and Vibeke Appel. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1998 “Interpreting Strategies and Creativity.” In Translators’ Strategies and Creativity, edited by Ann Beylard-Ozeroff, Jana Králová, and Barbara Moser-Mercer, 171–180. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ruiz, Carmen, Natalia Paredes, Pedro Macizo, and Maria Teresa Bajo
2008 “Activation of Lexical and Syntactic Target Language Properties in Translation.” Acta Psychologica 128 (3): 490–500. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Seeber, Kilian G.
2011 “Cognitive Load in Simultaneous Interpreting: Existing Theories ‒ New Models.” Interpreting 13 (2): 176–204. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Seeber, Kilian G., and Dirk Kerzel
2011 “Cognitive Load in Simultaneous Interpreting: Model Meets Data.” International Journal of Bilingualism 16 (2): 228–242. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Setton, Robin, and Manuela Motta
2007 “Syntacrobatics: Quality and Reformulation in Simultaneous-with-Text.” Interpreting 9 (2): 199–230. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shreve, Gregory M., Isabel Lacruz, and Erik Angelone
2010 “Cognitive Effort, Syntactic Disruption, and Visual Interference in a Sight Translation Task.” In Translation and Cognition, edited by Gregory M. Shreve and Erik Angelone, 63–84. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
SR Research Experiment Builder 1.10.165 [Computer Software]
(2011) Mississauga, Ontario, Canada: SR Research Ltd.Google Scholar
Sunnari, Marianna
1996 “Comparison of Expert and Novice Performance in Simultaneous Interpreting.” In Proceedings of the XIV World Congress of FIT, 993–1000. Melbourne.Google Scholar
Timarová, Šárka, Ivana Čeňková, and Reine Meylaerts
2015 “Simultaneous Interpreting and Working Memory Capacity.” In Psycholinguistic and Cognitive Inquiries into Translation and Interpreting, edited by Aline Ferreira and John W. Schwieter, 101–126. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Viezzi, Maurizio
1989 “Information Retention as a Parameter for the Comparison of Sight Translation and Simultaneous Interpretation: An Experimental Study.” The Interpreters’ Newsletter 2: 65–69.Google Scholar
Warren, Tessa, and Edward Gibson
2002 “The Influence of Referential Processing on Sentence Complexity.” Cognition 85: 79–112. DOI logoGoogle Scholar