The interactional functions of the Japanese demonstratives in conversation

Keiko Naruoka

Abstract

Although a large number of studies are conducted on Japanese demonstratives, most of them explain referential functions of the three demonstrative types (the so-called proximal ko-, medial so-, and distal a-) based on sentence-level analysis, and little previous work has been directed toward the analysis of the demonstrative use in spontaneous interaction. This study employs Japanese conversational data and examines the demonstrative usages whose main function is NOT to refer to some entity in the speech situation or the discourse. From the analysis, the paper shows that the use of Japanese demonstratives can exhibit and emphasize an interactional meaning, such as the speaker’s antipathy, insult, suspicion, surprise, and affection toward the referent, and that it can be selected from among other choices, such as a noun phrase or ellipsis, when the speaker is willing to express these emotions or attitudes. In order to understand the process of expressing these emotions or attitudes, the paper applies Hanks’ (1990, 1992) ‘indexical framework’ and the interactionally defined notion of the speaker’s and addressee’s sphere proposed by Laury (1997) and Enfield (2003). Using these frameworks, this study illustrates that the relationship among the speaker’s and addressee’s spheres and the referent, as well as the context in which the three are projected, are not static or predefined but instead are flexible and do change during ongoing interaction.

Keywords:
Quick links
A browser-friendly version of this article is not yet available. View PDF
Anderson, S., & E. Keenan
(1985) Deixis. In T. Shopen (ed.), Language typology and syntactic description. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 259-308.Google Scholar
Ando, S
(1986) Nichi-Eigo no daikushisu: Joo [Deixis in Japanese and English vol. 1]. Eigo kyooiku [The English teachers' magazine] 34-12: 70-75.Google Scholar
Cook, H.M
(1992) Meanings of non-referential indexes: A case study of the Japanese sentence-final particle ne . Text 12-4: 507-539. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1993) Functions of the filler ano in Japanese. In S. Choi (ed.), Japanese/Korean linguistics 3. Stanford: CSLI Publications, pp. 19-38.Google Scholar
Diessel, H
(1999) Demonstratives: Form, function, and grammaticalization. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Du Bois, J., S. Schuetze-Coburn, D. Paolino, & S. Cumming
(1993) Outline of discourse transcription. In J. Edwards & M. Lampert (eds.), Transcription and coding methods for language research. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 45-89.Google Scholar
Duranti, A
(1984) The social meaning of subject pronouns in Italian conversation. Text 4-4: 277-311. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Enfield, N.J
(2003) Demonstratives in space and interaction: Data from Lao speakers and implications for semantic analysis. Language 79-1: 82-117. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fillmore, C.J
(1982) Towards a descriptive framework for spatial deixis. In R. Jarvella & W. Klein (eds.), Speech, place, and action: Studies in deixis and related topics. New York: John Wiley and Sons Ltd, pp. 31-59.Google Scholar
Fujii, Y
(1995) Nihongo no gojun no gyakuten ni tsuite [On reversed word order in Japanese]. In K. Takami (ed.), Nihongo no uhoo idoo koobun: Sono koozoo to kinoo [Right dislocation in Japanese: Structure and function]. Tokyo: Hituzi Shobo, pp. 167-198.Google Scholar
Halliday, M.A.K
(1973) Explorations in the functions of language. London: Arnold.  BoPGoogle Scholar
Halliday, M.A.K., & R. Hasan
(1976) Cohesion in English. London: Longman.  BoPGoogle Scholar
Hamaguchi, T
(2001) Co-construction of meaning in intergenerational family conversations: A case of the Japanese demonstrative pronoun are . Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Georgetown University, Washington DC.
Hanks, W.F
(1990) Referential practice: Language and lived space among the Maya. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
(1992) The indexical ground of deictic reference. In A. Duranti & C. Goodwin (eds.), Rethinking context. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 43-76.Google Scholar
Hattori, S
(1968) Eigo kiso goi no kenkyuu [Studies on English basic lexicon]. Tokyo: Sanseido.Google Scholar
Hayashi, M
(2004) Projection and grammar: Notes on the 'action-projecting' use of the distal demonstrative are in Japanese. Journal of Pragmatics 36-8: 1337-1374. DOI logo  BoPGoogle Scholar
Himmelmann, N
(1996) Demonstratives in narrative discourse: A taxonomy of universal uses. In B. Fox (ed.), Studies in anaphora. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 205-254. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Horiguchi, K
(1978) Shijishi no hyoogensei [Expressivity of demonstratives]. Nihongo Nihon-bunka [Japanese language and culture] 8: 23-44.Google Scholar
Hymes, D
(1974) Foundations in sociolinguistics: An ethnographic approach. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press.  BoPGoogle Scholar
Kamio, A
(1990) Joohoo no nawabari riron: Gengo no kinooteki bunseki [Theory of territory of information: Functional analysis of language]. Tokyo: Taishukan.  BoPGoogle Scholar
Kinsui, S., & Y. Takubo
(1990) Danwa kanri riron kara mita Nihongo no shijishi [A discourse management of the Japanese demonstrative expressions]. In the Japanese Cognitive Science Society (ed.), Ninchi kagaku no hatten [Advances in Japanese cognitive science vol. 3]. Tokyo: Kodansha, pp. 85-115.Google Scholar
Kirsner, R
(1979) Deixis in discourse: An exploratory quantitative study of the modern Dutch demonstrative adjectives. In T. Givón (ed.), Syntax and semantics vol. 12: Discourse and syntax. New York: Academic Press, pp. 355-375.Google Scholar
Kitagawa, C
(1979) A note on sono and ano . In G. Bedell, E. Kobayashi & M. Muraki (eds.), Explorations in linguistics: Papers in honor of Kazuko Inoue. Tokyo: Kenkyusha, pp. 232-243.Google Scholar
Kitano, H
(1999) On interaction and grammar: Evidence from one use of the Japanese demonstrative are ('that'). Pragmatics 9-3: 383-400.  BoP DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuno, S
(1973) The structure of the Japanese language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kuroda, S
(1979) (Ko-) so- a- ni tsuite [On (ko-), so- and a-]. Eigo to Nihongo to [English and Japanese]. Tokyo: Kuroshio Shuppan, pp. 41-59.Google Scholar
Lakoff, R
(1974) Remarks on this and that . Papers from the tenth regional meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society , pp. 345-356.
Laury, R
(1997) Demonstratives in interaction. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logo  BoPGoogle Scholar
Lyons, J
(1977) Semantics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.  BoPGoogle Scholar
Mayes, P., & T. Ono
(1991) Social factors influencing reference in Japanese: With a special emphasis on ano hito . Santa Barbara Papers in Linguistics 3: 84-93.Google Scholar
Maynard, S.K
(1989) Japanese conversation: Self-contextualization through structure and interactional management. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Mikami, A
(1970) Ko-so-a-do- shoo [On ko-so-a-do-]. Bunpoo shooron shuu [Papers on grammar]. Tokyo: Kuroshio Shuppan, pp. 145-154.Google Scholar
Mithun, M
(1987) The grammatical nature and discourse power of demonstratives. Proceedings of the thirteenth annual meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society , pp. 184-194.
Naruoka, K
(2004) Variations of demonstratives and their functions in Japanese and American English conversation. Papers from the twenty-first national conference of the English Linguistic Society of Japan , pp. 130-139.
Naruoka, K.
in press) Expressive function of Japanese adnominal demonstrative konna/sonna/anna . In M. E. Hudson & S.A. Jun & P. Sells (eds.) Japanese/Korean linguistics 13 Stanford CSLI Publications
Ochs, E., & B. Schieffelin
(1989) Language has a heart. Text 9-1: 7-25.  BoP DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ono, K
(1994) Territories of information and Japanese demonstratives. Journal of the Association of Teachers of Japanese 28-1: 131-155. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ono, T., & R. Suzuki
(1992) Word order variability in Japanese conversation: Motivations and grammaticization. Text 12-3: 429-445.  BoP DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sakuma, K
(1951) Geidai Nihongo no hyoogen to gohoo [Expressions and usages in modern Japanese]. Tokyo: Kooseikaku.Google Scholar
Schiffrin, D
(1987) Discourse marker. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logo  BoPGoogle Scholar
Shibatani, M
(1990) The languages of Japan. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Silverstein, M
(1976) Shifters, linguistic categories, and cultural description. In K. Basso & H. Selby (eds.), Meaning in anthropology. Albuquerque: University of New Mexico Press, pp. 11-55.Google Scholar
Strauss, S
(1993) Why 'this' and 'that' are not complete without 'it'. Papers from the twenty-ninth regional meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society, pp. 403-417.
Takahashi, T
(1956) Bamen to ba [Situation and context]. Kokugo Kokubun [Japanese Language and Japanese Literature] 25-9: 53-61.Google Scholar
Talmy, L
(1978) Figure and ground in complex sentences. In J. Greenberg (ed.), Universals of human language vol. 4: Syntax. Stanford: Stanford University Press, pp. 625-649.Google Scholar