Children’s formal division of labor in requests

Yupin Chen
Abstract

This study aims to shed a light on children’s pragmatic development by examining Mandarin-speaking children’s requests in interactions with their parents. It is found that children between 24 and 36 months old appear to use various request forms, primarily with simple imperatives and WANT statements. A closer examination reveals that children prefer to use simple imperatives in activity-based interactions while both primary forms are found in unstructured daily conversations. The findings suggest that children’s preference for simple imperatives may be driven by their awareness of parents’ cooperativeness in interactional situations. It is thus speculated that simple imperatives and WANT statements may be children’s two primal request forms, but they may pivot on one request form when the situation is right.

Keywords:
Quick links
A browser-friendly version of this article is not yet available. View PDF
Austin, J.L
(1962) How to do things with words. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Axia, Giovanna
(1996) How to persuade mum to buy a toy. First Language 16: 301-317. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Babelot, Géraldine, and Haydée Marcos
(1999) Comprehension of directives in young children: Influence of social situation and linguistic form. First Language 19: 165-186. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, Elizabeth
(1976) Acquisition of polite forms: Longitudinal evidence. Language and context: The acquisition of pragmatics. New York: Academic Press, pp. 225-354.  BoPGoogle Scholar
Bates, E., L. Camaioni, and V. Volterra
(1975) The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill Palmer Quarterly 21: 205-226.  BoPGoogle Scholar
Bosco, Francesca M., Monica Bucciarelli, and Bruno Bara
(2004) The fundamental context categories in understanding communicative intention. Journal of Pragmatics 36: 467-488. DOI logo  BoPGoogle Scholar
Bruner, Jerome
(1981) The social context of language acquisition. Language and Communication 1: 115-178. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1983) Child’s talk: Learning to use language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Carter, Anne
(1974) Communication in the sensorimoter period. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Berkeley, California: University of California-Berkeley.
Clark, Eve V
(2003) First language acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dore, John
(1973) The development of speech acts. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. New York: City University of New York.
Deutscher, Guy
(2005) The unfolding of language: The evolution of mankind’s greatest invention. London: Arrow Books.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, Susan
(1976) Is Sybil there? The structure of some American English directives. Language in Society 5: 25-66. DOI logo  BoPGoogle Scholar
(1977) Wait for me, roller skate! In Susan Ervin-Tripp, and Claudia Mitchell-Kernan (eds.), Child discourse. New York: Academic Press, pp. 165-208.  BoP DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1980) Speech acts, social meaning and social learning. In Howard Giles, W. Peter Robinson, and Philip M. Smith (eds.), Language: Social psychological perspectives. Oxford: Pergamon Press, pp. 389-395. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, Susan, Jiansheng Guo, and Martin Lampert
(1990) Politeness and persuasion in children’s control acts. Journal of Pragmatics 14: 307-331. DOI logo  BoPGoogle Scholar
Garton, Alison F., and Chris Pratt
(1990) Children’s pragmatic judgments of direct and indirect requests. First Language 10: 51-59. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Garvey, Catherine
(1974) Requests and responses in children’s speech. Journal of Child Language 2: 41-63. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gordon, David, and Susan Ervin-Tripp
(1984) The structure of children’s requests. In Richard L. Schiefelbusch, and J. Pickard (eds.), The acquisition of communicative competence. Baltimore: University Park Press, pp. 295-321.Google Scholar
Halliday, Michael Alexander Kirkwood
(1975) Learning how to mean. In Eric Lenneberg, and Elizabeth Lenneberg (eds.), A multidisciplinary perspective. London: Academic Press, pp. 239-265.Google Scholar
Hsiao, Hui-chen
(1999) A development study of polite registers in school-age children’s request. Unpublished M.A. thesis of the Catholic Fu-Jen University: Taipei, Taiwan.
Hsu, Joseph H
(1996) A study of the stages of development and acquisition of Mandarin Chinese by children in Taiwan. Taipei, Taiwan: The Crane Publishing, pp. 137-148.Google Scholar
Kelly, Barbara F
(2007) “Mummy! Ball! Fish!”: Why English-learning children produce nouns earlier than verbs. In Illana Mushin, and Mary Laughren (eds.), Selected Papers from the 2006 Annual Meeting of the Australian Linguistic Society. Australia Linguistic Society.Google Scholar
Landis, J.R., and G.G. Koch
(1977) The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics 33: 159-174. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leonard, Rosemary J
(1993) Requests, refusals, and reasons in children’s negotiations. Social Development 2.2: 131-144. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, Brian
(2000) The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk, Third Edition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.  BoPGoogle Scholar
Mitchell-Kernan, Claudia, and Keith T. Kernan
(1977) Pragmatics of directive choice among children. In Susan Ervin-Tripp, and Claudia Mitchell-Kernan (eds.), Child discourse. New York: Academic Press, pp. 189-208.  BoP DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ninio, Anat, and Catherine E. Snow
(1996) Pragmatic development. Colorado: Westview Press.  BoPGoogle Scholar
Pan, Yuling
(2000) Politeness in Chinese face-to-face interaction. Stamford, CT: Alex.  BoPGoogle Scholar
Searle, John R
(1975) Indirect speech acts. In P. Cole, and J.L. Morgan (eds.), Syntax and semantics, vol. 3: Speech acts. New York: Academic Press, pp. 59-82.Google Scholar
Tomasello, Michael
(1992) The social bases of language acquisition. Social Development 1.1: 67-87. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., M. Carpenter, J. Call, T. Behne, and H. Moll
(2005) Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Science 28: 675-735.Google Scholar
Wood, Barbara S., and Royce Gardner
(1980) How children “get their way”: Directives in communication. Communication Education 29: 264-272. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yont, Kristine M., Catherine E. Snow, and Lynne Vernon-Feagans
(2003) The role of context in mother-child interactions: An analysis of communicative intents expressed during toy play and book reading with 12-month-olds. Journal of Pragmatics 35: 435-454. DOI logo  BoPGoogle Scholar
Zhou, Jing
(2002) Pragmatic development of Mandarin-speaking children: From 14 months to 32 months. Nanjing: Nanjing Normal University Press.Google Scholar