'Acceptability' and Language-Specific Preference in the Distribution of Information

Monika Doherty
Abstract

The paper generalizes upon some basic aspects of acceptability concerning language-specific preferences in the distribution of information in original texts and translations. It is assumed that the pragmatic principle of Optimal Relevance and major grammatical parameters jointly determine language-specific processing conditions for an optimal distribution of information. The claims are illustrated by the German translation of a passage from an English novel, where the preferred translational variants meet different processing conditions in 'right-peripheral' German, as opposed to 'left-peripheral' English. The differences concern word order, including initial and final position in simple and complex sentences, and may have an impact on the semantic readings of formally similar sentence structures, which can require redistribution of information beyond sentence boundaries.

Table of contents

Gideon Toury has recently claimed that "it is no concern of a scientific discipline ... to effect changes in the world of our experience". "Drawing conclusions is up to the practitioners, not the scholars. It is they", he says, "who must bare the consequences anyway". Toury does not exclude the possibility of "drawing conclusions from theoretical reasoning, or scientific findings, to actual behaviour, be its orientation retrospective (such as translation criticism) or prospective (such as translator training or translation planning)" (Toury 1995: 17). Applying this to his own view of translation theory he suggests that trainees be given the opportunity "to abstract their own guiding principles and routines from actual instances of behaviour" (p. 256), and even be encouraged "to violate translational norms", albeit "not as an end in itself but rather as a means of opening the students' eyes to the multiplicity of modes of translation, all of which may be legitimate, according to one set of norms or another, and helping them wend their own way through the dark forest" (p. 258).

Full-text access is restricted to subscribers. Log in to obtain additional credentials. For subscription information see Subscription & Price. Direct PDF access to this article can be purchased through our e-platform.

References

Abraham, Werner
1992 "Clausal Focus Versus Discourse Rhema in German: A Program-matic View". Language und Cognition II. 1–19.Google Scholar
Altmann, Gerry T.M.
1989 "Parsing and Interpretation: An Introduction". Language and Cognitive Processes 4:3/4. 1–19.Google Scholar
Büring, Daniel
1995The 59th Street Bridge Accent: On the Meaning of Topic and Focus. University of Tubingen. [PhD Dissertation.]Google Scholar
Doherty, Monika
1992 "Informationelle Holzwege". LiLi 84. 30–49.Google Scholar
Féry, Caroline
1993German Intonational Patterns. Tubingen: Niemeyer.   DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Frazier, Lyn
1988 "Grammar and Language Processing". Frederick J. Newmeyer, ed. Linguistics: The Cambridge Survey II. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press 1988 15–34. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grice, Paul
1975 "Logic and Conversation". Peter Cole and Jerry L. Morgan, eds. Syntax and Semantics 3: Speech Acts. New York: Academic Press 1975 41–58.Google Scholar
Haider, Hubert
1995 "Wenn die Semantik arbeitet,—und die Syntax sie gewähren läßt". Universität Stuttgart. [Unveröffentlichtes Manuskript.]Google Scholar
Isham, William P.
1994 "Memory for Sentence Form after Simultaneous Interpretation: Evidence both for and against Deverbalization". Sylvia Lambert and Barbara Moser-Mercer, eds. Bridging the Gap: Empirical Research in Simultaneous Interpretation. Amsterdam-Philadelphia: John Benjamins 1994 191–211.   DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Koller, Werner
1992Einfuhrung in die Übersetzungswissenschaft. Wiesbaden: Quelle & Meyer.Google Scholar
Levelt, Willem
1989Speaking: From Intention to Articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
[ p. 24 ]
Lohnstein, Horst
1994 "Grammatik und Parsing". ZS 13:1. 56–109.Google Scholar
Macheiner, Judith
1995Übersetzen: Ein Vademecum. Frankfurt: Eichborn.Google Scholar
Maienborn, Claudia
1996 "Towards a Compositional Semantics for Locative Modifiers". M. Simons and T. Galloway, eds. Proceedings from Semantics and Linguistic Theory V. Ithaca, N.Y.: Cornell University Linguistic Publications 1996 237–254.Google Scholar
Quirk, Randolph
et al. 1985A Comprehensive Grammar of the English Language. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Russell, Bertrand
1945A History of Western Philosophy. New York: Touchstone.Google Scholar
Schmitt, Peter A.
1992 "Culturally Specific Elements in Technical Translations". Joachim Schwend, Susanne Hagemann and Hermann Völkel, eds. Literatur im Kontext/Litera-ture in Context: Festschrift für Horst W. Drescher. Frankfurt: Peter Lang 1992 495–515.Google Scholar
Sperber, Dan and Deirdre Wilson
1986Relevance: Communication and Cognition. Ox-ford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Tanenhaus, Michael K.
1988 "Psycholinguistics: An Overview". Frederick J. Newmeyer, ed. Linguistics: The Cambridge Survey III. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press 1988 1–37. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Toury, Gideon
1995Descriptive Translation Studies and Beyond. Amsterdam-Philadelphia: John Benjamins. [Benjamins Translation Library, 4.]   DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vallduví, Enric
1993Information Packaging: A Survey. Edinburgh. [Research Paper HCRC/RP-44.]Google Scholar
Vallduví, Enric and Elisabeth Engdahl
1996 "The Linguistic Realisation of Information Packaging". Monika Doherty, ed. Information Structure: A Key Concept for Transla-tion Theory [= Linguistics 34:3]. 459–520.Google Scholar
Wolfe, Thomas
1984Look Homeward, Angel. New York: Collier Books.Google Scholar