References (42)
Biggs, S. (2011). Abduction and modality. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 83(2), 283–326. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Block, N. (2007). Consciousness, accessibility, and the mesh between psychology and neuroscience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30(5–6), 481–499.Google Scholar
Bloom, P. (2004). Descartes’ baby: How the science of child development explains what makes us human. New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Carnap, R. (1932/1959). The elimination of metaphysics through logical analysis of language. (A. Pap, Trans.). In A.J. Ayer (Ed.), Logical positivism (pp.60–81). New York, NY: The Free Press.Google Scholar
Chalmers, D.J. (1996). The conscious mind: In search of a fundamental theory. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
. (2000). What is a neural correlate of consciousness? In T. Metzinger (Ed.), Neural correlates of consciousness: Empirical and conceptual questions (pp.17–39). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Crick, F. (1996). Visual perception: rivalry and consciousness. Nature, 379(6565), 485–486. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crick, F., & Koch, C. (2003). A framework for consciousness. Nature Neuroscience, 6(2), 119–125. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J.A. (1974). Special sciences (or: The disunity of science as a working hypothesis). Synthese, 28(2), 97–115. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Earman, J. (1992). Bayes or bust? A critical examination of Bayesian confirmation theory. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Fine, K. (2001). The question of realism. Philosophers’ Imprint, 1(2), 1–30. Retrieved from [URL]Google Scholar
Frith, C., Perry, R., & Lumer, E. (1999). The neural correlates of conscious experience: An experimental framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(3), 105–114. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibbard, A. (1975). Contingent identity. Journal of Philosophical Logic, 4(2), 187–221. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Horgan, T. (1993). From supervenience to superdupervenience: Meeting the demands of a material world. Mind, 102(408), 555–586. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Johnston, M. (1997). Manifest kinds. Journal of Philosophy, 94(11), 564–583. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kanwisher, N. (2001). Neural events and perceptual awareness. Cognition, 79(1–2), 89–113. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kitcher, P. (1981). Explanatory unification. Philosophy of Science, 48(4), 507–531. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (1993). The advancement of science: Science without legend, objectivity without illusions. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kozuch, B.P. (2013). Merging the philosophical and scientific studies of consciousness. (Doctoral Dissertation, 2013, University of Arizona). Retrieved from [URL]
Kriegel, U. (2007). Functionalism, intentionalism, and the search for NCC in Jeffrey Gray’s work. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 14(4), 96–116.Google Scholar
. (2011). Two defenses of common-sense ontology. Dialectica, 65(2), 117–204. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Land, E.H. (1974). The retinex theory of colour vision. Proceedings of the Royal Institution Great Britain, 47, 23–57.Google Scholar
Mill, J.S. (1843/2002). A system of logic. Honolulu, HI: University Press of the Pacific.Google Scholar
Miller, S.M. (2001). Binocular rivalry and the cerebral hemispheres: With a note on the correlates and constitution of visual consciousness. Brain and Mind, 2(1), 119–149. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2007). On the correlation/constitution distinction problem (and other hard problems) in the scientific study of consciousness. Acta Neuropsychiatrica, 19(3), 159–176. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (Ed.). (2013). The constitution of visual consciousness: Lessons from binocular rivalry. Advances in Consciousness Research (Vol. 90). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins Publishing Company. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Noë, A., & Thompson, E. (2004). Are there neural correlates of consciousness? Journal of Consciousness Studies, 11(1), 3–28.Google Scholar
Papineau, D. (1993). Philosophical naturalism. Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishers.Google Scholar
Platt, J.R. (1964). Strong inference. Science, 146(3642), 347–353. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Popper, K.R. (1959). The logic of scientific discovery. New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Putnam, H. (1967). Psychological predicates. In W.H. Capitan & D.D. Merrill (Eds.), Art, mind, and religion (pp.37–48). Pittsburgh, PA: University of Pittsburgh Press.Google Scholar
Quine, W.V., & Ullian, J.S. (1970). The web of belief. New York, NY: Random House.Google Scholar
Rees, G., Kreiman, G., & Koch, C. (2002). Neural correlates of consciousness in humans. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(4), 261–270. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Revonsuo, A. (2000). Prospects for a scientific research program on consciousness. In T. Metzinger (Ed.), Neural correlates of consciousness: Empirical and conceptual questions (pp.57–75). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
. (2001). Can functional brain imaging discover consciousness in the brain? Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8(3), 3–23.Google Scholar
Smart, J.J.C. (1959). Sensations and brain processes. Philosophical Review, 68(2), 141–156. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stoljar, D. (2006). Ignorance and imagination: The epistemic origin of the problem of consciousness. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Teller, D.Y., & Pugh, E.N. (1983). Linking propositions in colour vision. In J.D. Mollon & L.T. Sharpe (Eds.), Colour vision: Physiology and psychophysics (pp.576–589). London, UK: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Tong, F., Nakayama, K., Vaughan, J.T., & Kanwisher, N. (1998). Binocular rivalry and visual awareness in human extrastriate cortex. Neuron, 21(4), 753–759. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van Fraassen, B.C. (1980). The scientific image. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zeki, S. (1982). Do wavelength selective cells in monkey striate cortex respond to colours? Journal of Physiology, 330(Suppl.), 57P–58P. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (1983). Colour coding in the cerebral cortex: The reaction of cells in monkey visual cortex to wavelengths and colours. Neuroscience, 9(4), 741–765. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (3)

Cited by three other publications

Kozuch, Benjamin
2023. A new low: Reassessing (and revising) the local recurrency theory of consciousness. The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science DOI logo
Kozuch, Benjamin
2024. An embarrassment of richnesses: the PFC isn’t the content NCC. Neuroscience of Consciousness 2024:1 DOI logo
Niikawa, Takuya
2020. A Map of Consciousness Studies: Questions and Approaches. Frontiers in Psychology 11 DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 25 september 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.