Part of
Perspectives on Pantomime
Edited by Przemysław Żywiczyński, Johan Blomberg and Monika Boruta-Żywiczyńska
[Advances in Interaction Studies 12] 2024
► pp. 1657
References (116)
References
Aboitiz, F. (2013). How did vocal behavior “take over” the gestural communication system? Language and Cognition, 5, 167–176. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Abramova, E. (2018). The role of pantomime in gestural language evolution, its cognitive bases and an alternative. Journal of Language Evolution, 3(1), 26–40. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Almor, A., Smith, D. V., Bonilha, L., Fridriksson, J., & Rorden, C. (2007). What is in a name? Spatial brain circuits are used to track discourse references. NeuroReport, 18, 1215–1219. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arbib, M. A. (1989). The Metaphorical Brain 2: Neural Networks and Beyond. New York: Wiley-Interscience.Google Scholar
(2005a). From monkey-like action recognition to human language: an evolutionary framework for neurolinguistics. The Behavioral and brain sciences, 28(2), 105–124; discussion 125-167-105-124; discussion 125–167. Retrieved from [URL]. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2005b). Interweaving protosign and protospeech: Further developments beyond the mirror. Interaction Studies: Social Behavior and Communication in Biological and Artificial Systems, 6, 145–171. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012). How the brain got language: The mirror system hypothesis. Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2013). Complex Imitation and the Language-Ready Brain. Language and Cognition, 5(2–3), 273–312. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arbib, M. A., Ganesh, V., & Gasser, B. (2014). Dyadic Brain Modeling, Ontogenetic Ritualization of Gesture in Apes, and the Contributions of Primate Mirror Neuron Systems. Phil Trans Roy Soc B, 369 (1644), 20130414.Google Scholar
Arbib, M. A. (2016a). How language evolution reshaped human consciousness. In R. R. Poznanski, J. A. Tuszynski, & T. E. Feinberg (Eds.), Biophysics of Consciousness: A Foundational Approach (pp. 87–128). Singapore: World Scientific. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016b). Towards a computational comparative neuroprimatology: Framing the language-ready brain. Physics of Life Reviews, 16, 1–54. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2018). In support of the role of pantomime in language evolution. Journal of Language Evolution, 3(1), 41–44. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2020a). From spatial navigation via visual construction to episodic memory and imagination. Biological Cybernetics, 114, 139–167. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2021a). The aboutness of language and the evolution of the construction-ready brain. In A. Lock, C. Sinha, & N. Gontier (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of symbolic evolution. Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2021b). When brains meet buildings: A conversation between neuroscience and architecture. Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2024). From the protolanguage spectrum to the underlying bases of language. In D. Adone & A. Gramatke (Eds.), On the evolution, acquisition and development of syntax (pp. to appear). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
(Ed.) (2020b). How the brain got language: Towards a new road map. John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arbib, M. A., & Bickerton, D. (Eds.). (2010). The emergence of protolanguage: Holophrasis vs compositionality. John Benjamins Publishing Company. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arbib, M. A., & Fellous, J. M. (2004). Emotions: from brain to robot. Trends Cogn Sci, 8(12), 554–561. Retrieved from [URL]. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arbib, M. A., Fragaszy, D. M., Healy, S. D., & Stout, D. (2023). Tooling and Construction: From Nut-Cracking and Stone-Tool Making to Bird Nests and Language Current Research in Behavioral Sciences, 5, 100121.Google Scholar
Arbib, M. A., Liebal, K., & Pika, S. (2008). Primate Vocalization, Gesture, and the Evolution of Human Language. Current Anthropology, 49(6), 1053–1076. [URL]. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arbib, M. A., & Rizzolatti, G. (1997). Neural expectations: a possible evolutionary path from manual skills to language. Communication and Cognition, 29, 393–424.Google Scholar
Arbib, M. A., Schweighofer, N., & Thach, W. T. (1995). Modeling the Cerebellum: From Adaptation to Coordination. In D. J. Glencross & J. P. Piek (Eds.), Motor Control and Sensory-Motor Integration: Issues and Directions (pp. 11–36). Amsterdam: North-Holland Elsevier Science. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Armstrong, D. F., Stokoe, W. C., & Wilcox, S. E. (1995). Gesture and the Nature of Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition 21, 37–46. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barnard, A. (2013). Cognitive and social aspects of language origins. In C. Lefebvre, B. Comrie, & H. Cohen (Eds.), New perspectives on the origins of language (pp. 53-71). John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Barton, R. A. (2012). Embodied cognitive evolution and the cerebellum. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1599), 2097–2107. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bergman, T. J., Beehner, J. C., Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M. (2003). Hierarchical classification by rank and kinship in baboons. Science, 302(5648), 1234–1236. Retrieved from [URL]. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bergman, T. J., Beehner, J. C., Painter, M. C., & Gustison, M. L. (2019). The speech-like properties of nonhuman primate vocalizations. Animal Behaviour, 151, 229–237. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bickerton, D. (2009). Adam’s Tongue. How Humans Made Language, How Language Made Humans. Hill & Wang.Google Scholar
Brown, S. (this volume). The Pantomimic Origins of the Narrative Arts. In P. Żywiczyński, S. Wacewicz, M. Boruta-Żywiczyńska, & J. Blomberg (Eds.), Perspectives on pantomime: evolution, development, interaction. John Benjamins. DOI logo
Butterworth, G. (2003). Pointing is the royal road to language for babies. In S. Kita (Ed.), Pointing: Where language, culture, and cognition meet (pp. 9–33). Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. W. (1999). Imitation without intentionality. Using string parsing to copy the organization of behaviour. Animal cognition, 2, 63–72. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2003). Imitation as behaviour parsing. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci, 358(1431), 529–536. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Byrne, R. W., & Russon, A. E. (1998). Learning by imitation: a hierarchical approach. Behav Brain Sci, 21(5), 667–684; discussion 684–721. Retrieved from [URL]. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Corballis, M. C. (2002). From hand to mouth, the origins of language. Princeton University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2013). Wandering tales: evolutionary origins of mental time travel and language. Frontiers in Psychology, 4. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2018). Mental travels and the cognitive basis of language. Interaction Studies, 19(1–2), 352–369. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2019). Language, memory, and mental time travel: An evolutionary perspective. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 13. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Croft, W. (2000). Explaining language change: An evolutionary approach. Longman.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1872). The expression of the emotions in man and animals (republished in 1965). University of Chicago Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dediu, D., & Levinson, S. C. (2018). Neanderthal language revisited: not only us. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 21, 49-55. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dehaene, S., Pegado, F., Braga, L. W., Ventura, P., Filho, G. N., Jobert, A., … Cohen, L. (2010). How Learning to Read Changes the Cortical Networks for Vision and Language. Science, 330(6009), 1359–1364. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Diessel, H. (2013). Where does language come from? Some reflections on the role of deictic gesture and demonstratives in the evolution of language. Language and Cognition, 5(2-3), 239-249.Google Scholar
Donald, M. (1991). Origins of the modern mind: Three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Fay, N., Arbib, M., & Garrod, S. (2013). How to bootstrap a human communication system. Cognitive Science, 37(7), 1356–1367. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fay, N., Lister, C. J., Ellison, T. M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2014). Creating a communication system from scratch: gesture beats vocalization hands down. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 354. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ferrari, P. F., Gerbella, M., Coudé, G., & Rozzi, S. (2017). Two different mirror neuron networks: The sensorimotor (hand) and limbic (face) pathways. Neuroscience. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ferretti, F. (2021). The narrative origins of language. In N. Gontier, A. Lock, & C. Sinha (Eds.), Oxford Handbook of Human Symbolic Evolution . Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ferretti, F., Adornetti, I., & Chiera, A. (2022). Narrative pantomime: A protolanguage for persuasive communication. Lingua, 271 103247. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ferretti, F., Chiera, A., & Adornetti, I. (this volume). Narrative and Pantomime at the Origin of Language. In P. Żywiczyński, S. Wacewicz, M. Boruta-Żywiczyńska, & J. Blomberg (Eds.), Perspectives on pantomime: evolution, development, interaction. John Benjamins. DOI logo
Fitch, W. T. (2010). The evolution of language. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fogassi, L., Coudé, G., & Ferrari, P. F. (2013). The extended features of mirror neurons and the voluntary control of vocalization in the pathway to language. Language and Cognition, 5, 145–155. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gärdenfors, P. (2021). Demonstration and pantomime in the evolution of teaching and communication. Language & Communication, 80, 71–79. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2023). The relations of demonstration and pantomime to causal reasoning and event cognition. In P. Żywiczyński, S. Wacewicz, M. Boruta-Żywiczyńska, & J. Blomberg (Eds.), Perspectives on pantomime: evolution, development, interaction. John Benjamins.Google Scholar
(this volume). The relations of demonstration and pantomime to causal reasoning and event cognition. In P. Żywiczyński, S. Wacewicz, M. Boruta-Żywiczyńska, & J. Blomberg (Eds.), Perspectives on pantomime: evolution, development, interaction. John Benjamins. DOI logo
Gärdenfors, P., & Högberg, A. (2017). The Archaeology of Teaching and the Evolution of Homo docens. Current Anthropology, 58(2), 188–208. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Ghazanfar, A. A., & Eliades, S. J. (2014). The neurobiology of primate vocal communication. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 28(0), 128–135. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ghazanfar, A. A., Liao, D. A., & Takahashi, D. Y. (2019). Volition and learning in primate vocal behaviour. Animal Behaviour, 151, 239–247. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Girard-Buttoz, C., Zaccarella, E., Bortolato, T., Friederici, A. D., Wittig, R. M., & Crockford, C. (2022). Chimpanzees produce diverse vocal sequences with ordered and recombinatorial properties. Communications Biology, 5(1), 410. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Halina, M., Rossano, F., & Tomasello, M. (2013). The ontogenetic ritualization of bonobo gestures. Animal cognition, 16(4), 653–666. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hartmann, S., & Pleyer, M. (2021). Constructing a protolanguage: reconstructing prehistoric languages in a usage-based construction grammar framework. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 376(1824), 20200200. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Heine, B., & Kuteva, T. (2007). The genesis of grammar: A reconstruction. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Heintz, C., Karabegovic, M., & Molnar, A. (2016). The co-evolution of honesty and strategic vigilance. Frontiers in Psychology, 7. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Herman, D. (2013). Storytelling and the sciences of mind. The MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hewes, G. W. (1973). Primate communication and the gestural origin of language. Current Anthropology, 12(1–2), 5–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Humphrey, N. K. (1970). What the frog’s eye tells the monkey’s brain. Brain Behavior and Evolution, 3, 324–337. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Iriki, A., & Taoka, M. (2012). Triadic (ecological, neural, cognitive) niche construction: a scenario of human brain evolution extrapolating tool use and language from the control of reaching actions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367, 10–23. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Itti, L., & Arbib, M. A. (2006). Attention and the minimal subscene. In M. A. Arbib (Ed.), Action to language via the mirror neuron system (pp. 289–346). Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jackson, J. H. (1878–79). On affections of speech from disease of the brain. Brain, 1, 304–330. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kemmerer, D. (2005). Against innate grammatical categories. Behavioral Brain Sciences, [URL]Google Scholar
Kendon, A. (1980). Gesticulation and speech: Two aspects of the process of utterance. In M. R. Key (Ed.), Relationships of the verbal and nonverbal communication (pp. 207–228). Mouton. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kline, M. A. (2015). How to learn about teaching: An evolutionary framework for the study of teaching behavior in humans and other animals. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 38, 10.1017/S0140525X14000090, e14000031 (14000071 pages with commentaries). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretense and representation: The origins of “theory of mind. Psychological Review, 94, 412–426. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
MacNeilage, P. F., & Davis, B. L. (2005). The frame/content theory of evolution of speech: A comparison with a gestural-origins alternative. Interaction Studies, 6(2), 173–199. Retrieved from [URL]. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marentette, P., Inaba, C., & Petrie, R. (this volume). Symbolic Distancing in Three-Year-Old Children’s Pantomime. In P. Żywiczyński, S. Wacewicz, M. Boruta-Żywiczyńska, & J. Blomberg (Eds.), Perspectives on pantomime: evolution, development, interaction. John Benjamins. DOI logo
McNeill, D. (1998). Speech and gesture integration. In J. M. Iverson & S. Goldin-Meadow (Eds.), The nature and functions of gesture in children’s communication (pp. 11–27). Jossey Bass. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mineiro, A., & Moita, M. (this volume). The pantomime roots of Sao Tome and Principe Sign Language In P. Żywiczyński, S. Wacewicz, M. Boruta-Żywiczyńska, & J. Blomberg (Eds.), Perspectives on pantomime: evolution, development, interaction. John Benjamins. DOI logo
Müller, C. (2014). Gestural modes of representation as techniques of depiction. In C. Müller, A. Cienki, E. Fricke, S. Ladewig, M. D., & S. Tessendorf (Eds.), Body-language-communication: An international handbook on multimodality in human interaction (pp. 1687–1702). De Gruyter Mouton. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016). From mimesis to meaning: A systematics of gestural mimesis for concrete and abstract referential gestures. In J. Zlatev, G. Sonesson, & P. Konderak (Eds.), Meaning, mind and communication: Explorations in cognitive semiotics (pp. 211–226). Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Olton, D. S., Becker, J. T., & Handelmann, G. E. (1980). Hippocampal function: Working memory or cognitive mapping? Physiol. Psychol., 8, 239–246. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pleyer, M. (2023). The role of interactional and cognitive mechanisms in the evolution of (proto)language(s). Lingua, 282, 103458. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Poizner, H., Klima, E., & Bellugi, U. (1987). What the hands reveal about the brain. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Polich, L. (2005). The emergence of the deaf community in Nicaragua: “With sign language you can learn so much”. Gallaudet University Press.Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G., & Arbib, M. A. (1998). Language within our grasp. Trends in Neurosciences, 21(5), 188–194. Retrieved from [URL]. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Russon, A. (2016). Reconsidering great ape imitation and pantomime: Comment on “Towards a computational comparative neuroprimatology: Framing the language-ready brain” by M.A. Arbib. Physics of Life Reviews. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2018). Pantomime and imitation in great apes: Implications for reconstructing the evolution of language. Interaction Studies, 19(1–2), 200–215. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Russon, A., & Andrews, K. (2010). Orangutan pantomime: Elaborating the message. Biology Letters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011). Pantomime in great apes: Evidence and implications. Communicative & Integrative Biology, 4(3), 315–317. Retrieved from [URL]. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sandler, W. (2013). Vive la différence: Sign language and spoken language in language evolution. Language and Cognition, 5(2–3), 189–203. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schmid, H.-J. (2015). A blueprint of the Entrenchment-and- Conventionalization Model. Yearbook of the German Cognitive Linguistics Association, 3(1), 3–26. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Senghas, A., Kita, S., & Özyürek, A. (2004). Children creating core properties of language: Evidence from an emerging sign language in Nicaragua. Science, 305, 1779–1782. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sibierska, M., Boruta-Żywiczyńska, M., Żywiczyński, P., & Wacewicz, S. (2023). What’s in a mime? An exploratory analysis of predictors of communicative success of pantomime Interaction Studies, 23:2, 289–321, DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Slocombe, K. E., Waller, B. M., & Liebal, K. (2011). The language void: The need for multimodality in primate communication research. Animal Behaviour, 81(5), 919–924. Retrieved from [URL]. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smith, C. C., & Reichman, O. J. (1984). The evolution of food caching by birds and mammals. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 15, 329–351. Retrieved from [URL]. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sperber, D., Clément, F., Heintz, C., Mascaro, O., Mercier, H., Origgi, G., & Wilson, D. (2010). Epistemic vigilance. Mind & Language, 25(4), 359–393. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stokoe, W. C. (2001). Language in hand: Why sign came before speech. Gallaudet University Press.Google Scholar
Stout, D. (2010). Possible relations between language and technology in human evolution. In A. Nowell & I. Davidson (Eds.), Stone tools and the evolution of human cognition (pp. 159–184). University Press of Colorado.Google Scholar
(2011). Stone toolmaking and the evolution of human culture and cognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366, 1050–1059. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stout, D., Toth, N., Schick, K., & Chaminade, T. (2008). Neural correlates of Early Stone Age toolmaking: technology, language and cognition in human evolution. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci, 363(1499), 1939–1949. Retrieved from [URL]. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Suddendorf, T., & Corballis, M. C. (1997). Mental time travel and the evolution of the human mind. Genet Soc Gen Psychol Monogr, 123(2), 133–167. Retrieved from [URL]Google Scholar
(2007). The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 299–351. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (1997). Primate Cognition. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2008). Origins of Human Communication. The MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wacewicz, S., & Żywiczyński, P. (2018). Language origins: The platform of trust, cooperation, and turn-taking. Interaction Studies, 19(1–2).Google Scholar
Wray, A. (1998). Protolanguage as a holistic system for social interaction. Language & Communication, 18, 47–67. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2000). Holistic utterances in protolanguage: The link from primates to humans. In C. Knight, M. Studdert-Kennedy, & J. Hurford (Eds.), The evolutionary emergence of language: Social function and the origins of linguistic form (pp. 285–202). Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zlatev, J., Persson, T., & Gärdenfors, P. (2005). Bodily mimesis as “the missing link” in human cognitive evolution. Lund University Cognitive Studies, 121, 1–45.Google Scholar
Zlatev, J., Sibierska, M., & Żywiczyński, P. (this volume). Can pantomime narrate? A cognitive semiotic approach. In P. Żywiczyński, S. Wacewicz, M. Boruta-Żywiczyńska, & J. Blomberg (Eds.), Perspectives on pantomime: evolution, development, interaction. John Benjamins. DOI logo
Zlatev, J., Żywiczyński, P., & Wacewicz, S. (2020). Pantomime as the original human-specific communicative system. Journal of Language Evolution, 5(2), 156–174. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zukow-Goldring, P. (2012). Assisted imitation: first steps in the seed model of language development. Language Sciences, 34(5), 569–582. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zukow-Goldring, P., & Arbib, M. A. (2007). Affordances, effectivities, and assisted imitation: Caregivers and the directing of attention. Neurocomputing, 70, 2181–2193. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Żywiczyński, P., Wacewicz, S., & Sibierska, M. (2018). Defining Pantomime for Language Evolution Research. Topoi, 37(2), 307–318. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (2)

Cited by two other publications

Mineiro, Ana & Mara Moita
Żywiczyński, Przemysław, Marek Placiński, Marta Sibierska, Monika Boruta-Żywiczyńska, Sławomir Wacewicz, Michał Meina & Peter Gärdenfors
2024. Praxis, demonstration and pantomime: a motion capture investigation of differences in action performances. Language and Cognition  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 4 july 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.