Aben, B., Stapert, S., & Blokland, A. (2012). About the distinction between working memory and short-term memory. Frontiers in Psychology, 3(301).
Adams, J. M. (2006). The performative nature and function of Isaiah 40–55. Norfolk: T & T Clark.
Aikhenvald, A. Y. (2004). Evidentiality. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Alston, W. P. (2000). Illocutionary acts and sentence meaning. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.
Amaral, P., & Cummins, C. (2015). A cross-linguistic study on information backgrounding and presupposition projection. In F. Schwarz (Ed.), Experimental perpsectives on presuppositions (pp. 157–172). Dordrecht: Springer International Publishing.
Anderson, L. B. (1986). Evidentials, paths of change, and mental maps: Typologically regular asymmetries. In W. Chafe & J. Nichols (Eds.), Evidentiality: The linguistic coding of epistemology (pp. 273–312). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Andorno, C. (2005). Che cos’è la pragmatica linguistica. Roma: Carocci.
Annett, M. (1967). The binomial distribution of right, left and mixed handedness. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 19, 327–333.
Arbib, M. A. (2005). From monkey-like action recognition to human language: An evolutionary framework for neurolinguistics. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 105–167.
Austen, J. (1811). Sense and sensibility. Reprinted by ESD Cloud Media, 2014.
Austin, J. L. (1962). How to do things with words. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Baker, N. D., & Greenfield, P. (1988). The development of new and old information in young children’s early language. Language Sciences, 10(1), 3–34.
Bambini, V. (2010). Neuropragmatics: A foreword. Italian Journal of Linguistics, 22(1), 1–20.
Bambini, V. (2012). Neurolinguistics. In J-O. Östman & J. Verschueren (Eds.), Handbook of pragmatics (pp. 1–34). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Bambini, V., Resta, D., & Grimaldi, M. (2014). A dataset of metaphors from the Italian literature: Exploring psycholinguistic variables and the role of context. PLoS ONE, 9, e105634.
Bastiaansen, M. C. M., Van Berkum, J. J., & Hagoort, P. (2002a). Event-related theta power increase in the human EEG during online sentence processing. Neuroscience Letters, 323, 13–16.
Bastiaansen, M. C. M., Van Berkum, J. J., & Hagoort, P. (2002b). Syntactic processing modulates the theta rhythm of the human EEG. Neuroimage, 17, 1479–1492.
Bastiaansen, M. C. M., Van der Linden, M., ter Keurs, M., Dijkstra, T., & Hagoort, P. (2005). Theta responses are involved in lexical-semantic retrieval during language processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(3), 530–541.
Bastiaansen, M., & Hagoort, P. (2006). Oscillatory neural dynamics during language comprehension. Progress in Brain Research, 159, 179–196.
Bates, E. (1976). Language and context: The acquisition of pragmatics. New York: Academic Press.
Baumann, S., & Schumacher, B. P. (2011). (De-)Accentuation and the processing of information status: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Language and Speech, 55(3), 361–381.
Benatar, A., & Clifton, C. Jr. (2014). Newness, givenness and discourse updating: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 71, 1–16.
Benveniste, E. (Ed.). (1971[1958]). Subjectivity in language. In Problems in General Linguistics [Trans. by Mary Elizabeth Meek]. Coral Gables, FL: University of Miami Press.
Berger, H. (1929). Über das elektrenkephalogramm des menschen. Arch Psychiatr Nervenkr, 87, 527–570.
Best, C. T. (1988). The emergence of cerebral asymmetries in early human development: A literature review and a neuroembryological model. In D. L. Molfese & S. J. Segalowitz (Eds.), Brain lateralization in children (pp. 5–34). New York: Guilford Press.
Bianchi, C. (2009). Pragmatica cognitiva. I meccanismi della comunicazione. Roma-Bari: Laterza.
Bickerton, D. (1995). Language and Human Behavior. Washington: University of Washington Press.
Birch, S., & Rayner, K. (1997). Linguistic focus affects eye movements during reading. Memory & Cognition, 25(5), 653–660.
Blakemore, D. (1987). Semantic constraints on relevance. Oxford: Blackwell.
Boas, F. (1900). Sketch of the Kwakiutl language. American Anthropologist, 2, 708–721.
Boas, F. (1910). Kwakiutl. An illustrative sketch. Washington: Government Printing Office.
Bock, K. J. & Mazzella, J. R. (1983). Intonational marking of given and new information: Some consequences for comprehension. Memory & Cognition, 11(1), 64–76.
Bolinger, D. L. (1965). Forms of English: Accent, morpheme, order. Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press.
Bolinger, D. (1986). Intonation and its parts. Melody in Spoken English. Stanford: Stanford University Press.
Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I., & Schumacher, P. B. (2016). Towards a neurobiology of information structure. In F. Caroline & I. Sinichiro (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of information structure (pp. 581–598). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Bréal, M. (1964 [1900]). Semantics: Studies in the science of meaning. New York: Dover.
Bredart, S. & Modolo, K. (1988). Moses strikes again: Focalization effect on a semantic illusion. Acta Psychologica, 67, 135–144.
Brentano, F. (1973[1874]). Psychology from an empirical point of view. Translated by Antos C. Rancurello, D. B. Terrell and Linda L. McAlister, from Psychologie vom empirischen Standtpunkt
, Sections V–IX.
Brodmann, K. (1909). Vergleichende Lokalisationslehre der Grosshirnrinde in ihren Prinzipien dargestellt auf Grund des Zellenbaues. Leipzig: Johann Ambrosius Barth Verlag.
Brody, M. (1990). Some remarks on the focus field in Hungarian. University College London Working Papers in Linguistics, 2, 201–225.
Burkhardt, P. (2006). Inferential bridging relations reveal distinct neural mechanisms: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Brain and Language, 98, 159–168.
Burkhardt, P. (2007). The P600 reflects cost of new information in discourse memory. Neuroreport, 18(17), 1851–1854.
Burkhardt, P., & Roehm, D. (2007). Differential effects of saliency: An event-related brain potential study. Neuroscience Letters, 413, 115–120.
Burkhardt, P. (2008). Two types of definites: Evidence for presupposition cost. In A. Grønn (Ed.), Proceedings of SuB12 (pp. 66–80). Oslo: ILOS.
Burmester, J., Spalek, K., & Wartenburger, I. (2014). Context updating during sentence comprehension: The effect of aboutness topic. Brain and Language, 137, 62–76.
Büring, D. (2003). On D-trees, beans, and B-accents. Linguistics and Philosophy, 26, 511–545.
Camblin, C. C., Gordon, P. C., & Swaab, T. Y. (2007). The interplay of discourse congruence and lexical association during sentence processing: Evidence from ERPs and eye tracking. Journal of Memory and Language, 56(1), 103–128.
Campbell, A. L., Brooks, P., & Tomasello, M. (2000). Factors affecting young children’s use of pronouns as referring expressions. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 43, 1337–1349.
Cardona, T. R., & Volterra, V. (2007). Le lingue dei segni. Storia e semiotica. Roma: Carocci.
Chafe, W. (1974). Language and consciousness. Language, 50, 111–133.
Chafe, W. (1976). Givenness, contrastiveness, definiteness, subjects, topics, and point of view. In C. Li (Ed.), Subject and topic (pp. 25–55). New York: Academic Press.
Chafe, W. L., & Nichols, J. (Eds.). (1986). Evidentiality: The linguistic coding of epistemology. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Chafe, W. (1994). Discourse, consciousness and time. The flow and displacement of conscious experience in speaking and writing. Chicago: Chicago University Press.
Chaiklin, S., & Lave, J. (1993). Understanding practice: Perspectives on activity and context. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Chemla, E. (2009). Similarity: Towards a unified account of scalar implicatures, free choice permission and presupposition projection. Under revision for Semantics and Pragmatics
.
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Massachusetts: MIT Press.
Chomsky, N. (1971). Deep structure, surface structure, and semantic interpretation. In D. Steinberg & L. Jakobovitz (Eds.), Semantics. An interdisciplinary reader in philosophy, linguistics and psychology (pp. 183–216). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Chomsky, N. (1980). Rules and representations. New York: Columbia University Press.
Chomsky, N. (2006). Language and the mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Christiansen, M. H., & Chater, N. (2008). Language as shaped by the brain. Brain and Behavioral Sciences, 31, 489–558.
Clark, H. H., & Haviland, S. E. (1977). Comprehension and the given-new contract. In R. O. Freedle (Ed.), Discourse production and comprehension (pp. 1–40). Norwood: N. J.-Ablex.
Cole, M. (1992). Context, modularity, and the cultural constitution of development. In L. T. Winegar & J. Valsiner (Eds.), Children’s development within social contexts (pp. 5–31). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Coolidge, F. L., & Wynn, T. (2005). Working memory, its executive functions, and the emergence of modern thinking. Cambridge Archeological Journal, 15(1), 5–26.
Coppock, E. (2014). Introduction to dynamic semantics. Slides available at [URL].
Corballis, M. C. (2003). From hand to mouth: The origins of language. New Jersey: Princeton University Press.
Cory, B., Romoli, J., Schwarz, F., & Crain, S. (2014). Scalar Implicatures vs. Presuppositions: The View from Acquisition. Topoi, 35(1), 57–71.
Cowan, N. (2008). What are the differences between long-term, short-term, and working memory? Progress in Brain Research, 169, 323–338.
Cowles, H. W., Kluender, R., Kutas, M., & Polinsky, M. (2007). Violations of information structure. An electrophysiological study of answers to wh-questions. Brain and Language, 102, 228–242.
Cresti, E. (1987). L’articolazione dell’informazione nel parlato. In AA. VV., Gli italiani parlati (pp. 27–90). Firenze: Accademia della Crusca.
Cresti, E. (1992). Le unità d’informazione e la teoria degli atti linguistici. In G. Gobber (Ed.), Atti del XXIV Congresso Internazionale di Studi della Società di Linguistica Italiana. Linguistica e pragmatica (pp. 501–529). Roma: Bulzoni.
Cresti, E. (2000). Corpus di italiano parlato. Firenze: Accademia della Crusca.
Cresti, E., & Moneglia, M. (2010). Informational patterning theory and the corpus-based description of spoken language: The compositionality issue in the topic-comment pattern. In M. Moneglia & A. Panunzi (Eds.), Bootstrapping information from corpora in a cross-linguistic perspective. Firenze: Firenze University Press.
Croft, W. (2001). Functional approaches to grammar. In N. J. Smelser & P. B. Baltes (Eds.), International encyclopedia of the social and behavioral sciences (pp. 6323–6330). Oxford: Elsevier Sciences.
Daneš, F. (1966). A three-level approach to syntax. In F. Daneš et al. (Eds.), Travaux linguistiques de Prague (pp. 225–240). Paris: Klincksieck.
Daneš, F. (1974). Functional sentence perspective and the organization of the text. In F. Daneš (Ed.), Papers on functional sentence persepctive (pp. 106–128). Prague: Academia/The Hague-Mouton.
Darwin, C. (1859). The origin of species. London: Clowes & Sons.
Delorme, A., & Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: An open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including indepllendent component analysis. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 134, 9–21.
Desimone, R., & Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18, 193–222.
Dik, S. C. (1978). Functional grammar [North-Holland Linguistic Series 37]. Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Domaneschi, F., Carrea, E., Penco, C., & Greco, A. (2014). The cognitive load of presupposition triggers: Mandatory and optional repairs in presupposition failure. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29(1), 136–146.
Domaneschi, F., & Carrea, E. (2015). Attivatori presupposizionali e failure: uno studio psicolinguistico. Sistemi Intelligenti, 2, 303–322.
Domaneschi, F. (Ed.). (2015). Special issue of Topoi on presuppositions: Philosophy, linguistics and psychology. Dordrecht: Springer.
Domaneschi, F., Carrea, E., Penco, C., & Greco, A. (2016). Selecting presuppositions in conditional clauses. Results from a psycholinguistic experiment. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, Article 2026.
Donnellan, K. (1966). Reference and Definite Descriptions. The Philosophical Review, 75(3), 281–304.
Dudley, R., Orita, N., Hacquard, V., & Lidz, J. (2015).
Three-year-olds’ understanding of know and think. In F. Schwarz (Ed.), Experimental Perspectives on Presuppositions (pp. 241–262). Dordrecht: Springer International Publishing.
Dummett, M. (1981). Frege: Philosophy of language, 2nd ed. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Dummett, M. (2006). Thought and reality. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Dunn, J. (1988). The beginning of social understanding. Oxford: Blackwell.
Erickson, T. D., & Mattson, M. E. (1981). From words to meanings: A semantic illusion. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20(5), 540–551.
Erteschik-Shir, N. (1997). The dynamics of focus structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Faller, M. T. (2002). Semantics and pragmatics. Department of Linguistics, University of Stanford, PhD Dissertation Thesis.
Fillmore, C. J. (1971). Verbs of judging: An exercise in semantic description. In C. J. Fillmore & T. D. Langendoen (Eds.), Studies in linguistic semantics (pp. 272–289). New York: Holt-Rinehart and Winston.
Firbas, J. (1992). Functional sentence perspective in written and spoken communication. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Flavell, J., Green, F. L., & Flavell, E. R. (1990). Developmental changes in young children’s knowledge about the mind. Cognitive Development, 5, 1–27.
Fox, B. A. (2001). Evidentiality, authority, responsibility, and entitlement in English conversation. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology, 11(2), 167–192.
Fox, D., & Katzir, R. (2010). On the characterization of alternatives. Available at: [URL].
Frascarelli, M. (2008). The fine structure of the topic field. In C. De Cat & K. Demuth (Eds.), The Bantu-Romance Connection. A comparative investigation of verbal agreement, DPs, and information structure (pp. 261–292). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Friederici, A. D. (2002). Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing. Trends in Cognitive Science, 6(2), 78–84.
Frege, G. (1892). Über Sinn und Bedeutung. Zeitschrift für Philosophie und philosophische Kritik, 100, 25–50.
Frege, G. (1918). Negation: A logical investigation. In P. T. Geach & M. Black (Eds.), Translations from the philosophical writings of Gottlob Frege (2nd ed., pp. 117–135). Oxford: Blackwell.
Friedman, V. A. (1986). Evidentiality in the Balkans. Macedonian and Albanian. In W. Chafe & J. Nichols (Eds.), Evidentiality: The linguistic coding of epistemology (pp. 168–187). Norwood: Ablex.
Gabelentz, von der G. (1869). Ideen zu einer vergleichenden Syntax. Zeitschrift für Völkerpsychologie und Sprachwissenschaft, 6.
Gagliardi, G., Lombardi Vallauri, E., & Tamburini, F. (2012). A topologic view of topic and focus marking in Italian. In N. Calzolari, K. Choukri, T. Declerck, M. U. M. Doğan, M. Bente, J. Mariani, J. Odijk, & S. Piperidis (Eds.), Proceedings 8th International Conference on Language Resources and – LREC 2012 (pp. 948–955). Istanbul: LREC.
Geurts, B. (1999). Presuppositions and pronouns. [Current research in the Semantics/Pragmatics Interface 3]. United Kingdom: Emerald Group Publishing.
Givón, T. (1975). Focus and the scope of assertion: Some Bantu evidence. Studies in African Linguistics, 6, 185–205.
Givón, T. (1979). On understanding grammar. New York: Academic Press.
Givón, T. (1989). Mind, code and context: Essays in pragmatics. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Glanzberg, M. (2003). Felicity and presupposition triggers. Workshop in Philosophy and Linguistics
. Michigan: University of Michigan.
Glanzberg, M. (2005). Presuppositions, truth values and expressing propositions. In G. Preyer & G. Peter (Eds.), Contextualism in philosophy: Knowledge, meaning and truth (pp. 349–396). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Goldstein, E. B. (2010). Sensation and perception. USA: Wadsworth.
Goodnow, J. J., & Warton, P. (1993). Contexts and cognitions: Taking a pluralistic view. In P. Light & G. Butterworth (Eds.), Context and cognition: Ways of learning and knowing (pp. 157–177). New York: Harvester Wheatsheaf.
Gordon, P. C., Grosz, L. A., & Gilliom, L. A. (1993). Pronouns, names and the centering of attention in discourse. Cognitive Sciences, 17, 311–347.
Gordon, P. C., & Chan, D. (1995). Pronouns, passives, and discourse coherence. Journal of Memory and Language, 34(2), 2016–2031.
Gould, S. J., & Vrba, E. S. (1982). Exaptation, a missing term in the science of form. Paleobiology, 8(1), 4–15.
Gouvea, A. C., Phillips, C., Kazanina, N., & Poeppel, D. (2010). The linguistic processes underlying the P600. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25(2), 149–188.
Grice, P. H. (1975). Logic and conversation. In P. Cole & J. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and semantics 3: Speech acts (pp. 41–58). New York: Academic Press.
Grice, P. H. (1989). Studies in the way of words. Cambridge-London: Harvard University Press.
Guiard, Y. (1987). Asymmetric division of labor in human skilled bimanual action: The kinematic chain as a model. Journal of Motor Behavior, 19, 486–517.
Hagoort, P., Brown, C., & Groothusen, J. (1993). The syntactic positive shift as an ERP measure of syntactic processing. Language & Cognitive Processes, 8, 337–364.
Hagoort, P., Hald, L., Bastiaansen, M., & Petersson, K. M. (2004). Integration of word meaning and world knowledge in language comprehension. Science, 304, 438–442.
Hagoort, P., & Levinson, S. C. (2014). Neuropragmatics. In M. S. Gazzaniga & G. R. Mangun (Eds.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 667–674). Cambridge: MIT Press.
Halliday, M. A. K. (1985). An introduction to functional grammar. London: Arnold.
Hahne, A., & Friederici, A. D. (1999). Electrophysiological evidence for two steps in syntactic analysis: Early automatic and late controlled processes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(2), 194–205.
Harnad, S. (1976). Induction, evolution and accountability. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 280, 58–60.
Hawkins, J. A. (2004). Efficiency and complexity in grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Heim, I. (1982). The semantics of definite and indefinite noun phrases. University of Massachussetts, Unpublished doctoral dissertation.
Heim, I. (1983). On the projection problem for presuppositions. Proceedings of the
Second West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics
, 114–125.
Heritage, J. (2012). Epistemics in action: Action formation and territories of knowledge. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 45(1), 1–29.
Hertrich, I., Kirsten, M., Tiemann, S., Beck, S., Whüle, A., Ackermann, H., & Rolke, B. (2015). Context-dependent impact of presuppositions on early magnetic brain responses during speech perception. Brain and Language, 148, 1–12.
Hill, J., & Irvine, J. (Eds.). (1993) Responsibility and evidence in oral discourse. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Horn, L. R. (1981). Exhaustiveness and the semantics of clefts. North East Linguistic Society (NELS), 11, 125–142.
Hornby, P. A. (1971). A psychological investigation of presupposition. Talk presented at the 1971 Summer
Meeting of the Linguistic Society of America
. Buffalo, New York.
Hornby, P. A. (1973). Intonation and syntactic structure in the development of presupposition. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development
, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, 18 p.
Hornby, P. A. (1974). Surface structure and presupposition. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13(5), 530–538.
Hruska, C., & Alter, K. (2004). Prosody in dialogues and single sentences: How prosody can influence speech perception. In A. Steube (Ed.), Language, context and cognition. Information structure: Theoretical and empirical aspects (pp. 211–223). Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.
Irwin, D. E., Bock, K. J., & Stanovich, K. E. (1982). Effects of information structure cues on visual word processing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21, 307–325.
Jakobson, R. (1957). Shifters, verbal categories, and the Russian verb. Cambridge Mass.: Russian Language Project, Department of Slavic Languages and Literature, Harvard University, p. 14. Repub. as SW II, 10 (130-147).
Jayez, J., Mongelli, V., Reboul, A., & Henst van der, J. B. (2015). Weak and strong triggers. In F. Schwarz (Ed.), Experimental perspectives on presuppositions (pp. 173–193). Dordrecht: Springers International Publishing.
Jäncke, L., Schlaug, G., & Steinmetz, H. (1997). Hand skill asymmetry in professional musicians. Brain and Cognition, 34, 424–432.
Jäncke, L., Shah, N. J., & Peters, M. (2000). Cortical activations in primary and secondary motor areas for complex bimanual movements in professional pianists. Cognitive Brain Research, 10, 177–183.
Jenkins, L. (2000). Biolinguistics. Exploring the biology of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Johnson, C. N., & Maratsos, M. P. (1977). Early comprehension of mental verbs: Think and know. Child Development, 48(4), 1743–1747.
Jouravlev, O., Stearns, L., Bergen, L., Eddy, M., Gibson, E., & Fedorenko, E. (2016). Processing temporal presuppositions: An event-related potential study. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience,
Kaan, E., Harris A., Gibson E., & Holcomb, P. J. (2000). The P600 as an index of syntactic integration difficulty. Language and Cognitive Processes, 14, 159–201.
Kamio, A. (1994). The theory of territory of information. The case of Japanese. Journal of Pragmatics, 21, 67–100.
Kamp, H. (1981). A theory of truth and semantic representation. In J. A. G. Groenendijk, T. M. V. Janssen, & M. B. J. Stokhof (Eds.), Formal methods in the study of language (pp. 277–322). Amsterdam: Mathematical Centre Tracts 135.
Kamp, H., & Reyle, U. (1993). From discourse to logic: Introduction to model-theoretic semantics of natural language, formal logic and discourse representation theory, Vol. 2. Dordrecht: Kluwer.
Kamp, H., Van Genabith, J., & Reyle, U. (2011). Discourse representation theory. Handbook of philosophical logic, Vol. 15 (pp. 125–394). Netherlands: Springer.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Karttunen, L. (1973). Presupposition of compound sentences. Linguistic Inquiry, 4(2), 169–193.
Karttunen, L. (1974). Presupposition and linguistic context. Theoretical Linguistics, 1(1–3), 181-194.
Keenan-Ochs, E., & Schieffelin, B. B. (1976). Topic as a discourse notion: A study of topic in the conversations of children and adults. In C. Li (Ed.), Subject and topic (pp. 335–384). New York: Academic Press.
Kempson, R. M. (1975). Presupposition and the delimitation of semantics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Kerbrat-Orecchioni, C. (1986). L’implicite. Paris: Armand Colin.
Kiparsky, C., & Kiparsky, P. (1971). Fact. In D. D. Steinberg & L. A. Jakobovitz (Eds.), Semantics: An interdisciplinary reader (pp. 345–369). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Kirby, S. (1999). Function, selection, and innateness. The emergence of language universals. New York: Oxford University Press.
Köhler, W. (1947). Gestalt psychology. New York: Liveright.
Krifka, M. (2007). Functional similarities between bimanual coordination and topic/comment structure. In S. Ishihara, S. Jannedy, & A. Schwarz (Eds.). Interdisciplinary Studies on Information Structure, 8, 61–96.
Krifka, M. (2008). Basic notions of information structure. Acta Linguistica Hungarica, 55(3-4), 243–276.
Križ, M. (2014). Homogeneity, Non-Maximality, and all. Available at: [URL].
Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: Brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207, 203–205.
Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2000). Electrophysiology reveals semantic memory use in language comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(12), 463–470.
Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2011). Thirty years and counting: Finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 621–647.
Kutas, M., De Long, K. A., & Smith, J. N. (2011). A look around at what lies ahead: Prediction and predictability in language processing. In M. Bar (Ed.), Predictions in the brain. Using our past to generate a future (pp. 190–207). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Lakens, D. (2013). Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: A practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAs. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 863.
Lambrecht, K. (1994). Information structure and sentence form. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Langacker, R. W. (1990). Subjectification. Cognitive Linguistics, 1, 5–38.
Langacker, R. W. (2003). Extreme subjectification: English tense and modals. In H. Cuyckens, T. Berg, R. Dirven, & K-U. Panther (Eds.), Motivation in language: Studies in honor of Günther Radden [Current Issues in Linguistic Theory 243] (pp. 3–26). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Langford, J., & Holmes, V. M. (1979). Syntactic presupposition in sentence comprehension. Cognition, 7, 363–383.
LaPolla, R. J. (2003). Overview of Sino-Tibetan morphosyntax. In G. Thurgood & R. J. LaPolla (Eds.), The Sino-Tibetan languages (pp. 22–42). London: Routledge.
La Rocca, D., Masia, V., Maiorana, E., Lombardi Vallauri, E., & Campisi, P. (2016). Brain response to information structure misalignments in linguistic contexts. Neurocomputing, 199, 1–15.
Lempert, H., & Kinsbourne, M. (1985). Possible origin of speech in Selective orienting. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 62–73.
Levinson, S. C. (1983). Pragmatics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Levinson, S. C. (2000). Presumptive meanings: The theory of generalized conversational implicature. USA: The MIT Press.
Levy, R. (2007). Expectation-based syntactic comprehension.
Lewis, D. (1979). Scorekeeping in a language game. Journal of Philosophical Logic, 8(3), 339–359.
Lieberman, P. (1984). The biology and evolution of language. USA: Harvard University Press.
Lightfoot, D. W. (1999). The development of language: Acquisition, change and evolution. Oxford: Blackwell.
Loftus, E. F. (1975). Leading questions and the eyewitness report. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 560–572.
Lombardi Vallauri, E. (1993). Clausole a contenuto presupposto e loro funzione discorsiva in italiano antico. Quaderni del Dipartimento di Linguistica dell’Università degli Studi di Firenze, 4, 71–95.
Lombardi Vallauri, E. (1995). Tratti linguistici della persuasione in pubblicità. Lingua Nostra, 2-3, 41–51.
Lombardi Vallauri, E. (1996). A simple test for Theme and Rheme in the clause complex. Language Sciences, 17(4), 357–378.
Lombardi Vallauri, E. (2000). Grammatica funzionale delle avverbiali italiane. Roma: Carocci.
Lombardi Vallauri, E. (2001). La teoria come separatrice di fatti di livello diverso. L’esempio della struttura informativa dell’enunciato. In Dati empirici e teorie linguistiche, Atti del XXXIII Congresso SLI, Napoli 1999 (pp. 151–173). Roma: Bulzoni.
Lombardi Vallauri, E. (2002). La struttura informativa dell’enunciato. Firenze: La Nuova Italia.
Lombardi Vallauri, E. (2004). The relation between mind and language: The innateness hypothesis and the poverty of the stimulus. The Linguistic Review, 21(3-4), 345–387.
Lombardi Vallauri, E. (2009). La struttura informativa. Forma e funzione negli enunciati linguistici. Roma: Carocci.
Lombardi Vallauri, E., & Masia, V. (2014). Implicitness impact: Measuring texts. Journal of Pragmatics, 61, 161–184.
Lombardi Vallauri, E., & Masia, V. (2015a). Cognitive constraints on the emergence of topic-focus structure in human communication. In V. Ganfi & A. Chiera (Eds.), Immagine e pensiero. Bilanci nelle scienze cognitive attuali (pp. 180–204). Roma-Messina: Corisco.
Lombardi Vallauri, E., & Masia, V. (2015b). Context-dependent information processing: Towards an expectation-based parsing model of Information Structure. In H. Christiansen, I. Stojanovic, & G. Papadopoulos (Eds.), Modeling and Using Context [LNAI 9405 Series] (pp. 440–453). Switzerland: Springer.
Löbner, S. (1985). Definites. Journal of Semantics, 4, 279–326.
Luck, S. J., & Kappenman, E. S. (2012). The Oxford handbook of event-related potential components. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Lyons, J. (1982). Deixis and subjectivity: Loquor ergo sum? In R. J. Jarvella & K. Wolfgang (Eds.), Speech, place, and action: Studies in deixis and related topics (pp. 101–124). New York: Wiley.
MacFarlane, J. (2011). What is assertion? In J. Brown & H. Cappelen (Eds.), Assertion: New philosophical essays (pp. 79–96). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
MacNeilage, P. F., Studdert-Kennedy, M. G., & Linblom, B. (1984). Functional precursors to language and its lateralization. The American Journal of Physiology, 246, 912–914.
McCready, E. (2014). What is evidence in natural language? In E. Mc Cready, K. Yabushita, & K. Yoshimoto (Eds.), Formal approaches to semantics and pragmatics, studies in linguistics and philosophy (pp. 155–180). Netherlands: Springer.
Marois, R., & Ivanoff, J. (2005). Capacity limits of information processing in the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(6), 296–305.
Martinet, A. (1955). Economie des changements phonétiques, Traité de phonologie diachronique. Bern: Francke. [trad. it, Economia dei mutamenti fonetici, Torino: Einaudi, 1968].
Marty, A. (1918). Gesammelte Schriften, Vol. II, part 1. Abteilung, Halle: Max Niemeyer, Verlag.
Masia, V., Canal, P., Ricci, I., Lombardi Vallauri, E., & Bambini, V. (2017). Presupposition of new information as a pragmatic garden path: Evidence from Event-Related Brain Potentials. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 42, 31–48.
Mathesius, V. (1939). O tak zvaném aktuálním členĕní vĕty (On the So-Called Topic/Comment Articulation of the Sentence). SaS, 5, 171–174.
Mennes, M., Wouters, H., Vanrumste, B., Lagae, L., & Stiers, P. (2010). Validation of ICA as a tool to remove eye movement artifacts from EEG/ERP. Psychophysiology, 47, 1142–1150.
Mesoudi, A., Whiten, A., & Dunbar, R. (2006). A bias for social information in human cultural transmission. British Journal of Psychology, 97, 405–423.
Molinaro, N., Conrad, M., Barber, H. A., & Carreiras, M. (2010). On the functional nature of the N400: Contrasting effects related to visual word recognition and contextual semantic integration. Cognitive Neuroscience, 1(1), 1–7.
Morey, R. D. (2008). Confidence intervals from normalized data: A correction to Cousineau (2005). Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 4, 61–64.
Morris, C. (1938). Foundations of the theory of signs. In C. Morris (Ed.), Writings on the general theory of signs. The Hague: Mouton.
Narasimhan, B., & Dimroth, C. (2008). Word order and information status in child language. Cognition, 107, 317–319.
Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 38 (Serial No. 149).
Nieuwland, M. S., & Van Berkum, J. J. A. (2006). When peanuts fall in love: N400 evidence for the power of discourse. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(7), 1098–1111.
Nuyts, J. (2005). Modality: Overview and linguistic issues. In W. Frawley (Ed.), The Expression of Modality [The Expression of Cognitive Categories 1] (pp. 1–26). Berlin/New York: De Gruyter.
Noveck, I., & Sperber, D. (2004). Experimental pragmatics. UK: MacMillan, Palgrave.
Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh handedness inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9(1), 97–113.
Ong, W. J. (1982). Orality and literacy. The technologizing of the word. New York: Routledge.
Oostenveld, R., Fries, P., Maris, E., & Schoffelen, J. M. (2010). FieldTrip: Open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Article ID 156869 Computational Intelligence and Neuroscience, 9(5), 105–113. »
Orletti, F. (2000). La conversazione diseguale. Roma: Carocci.
Osterhout, L., & Holcomb P. J. (1992). Event-related brain potentials elicited by syntactic anomaly. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 785–806.
Osterhout, L., Holcomb P. J., & Swinney, D. A. (1994). Brain potentials elicited by garden-path sentences: Evidence of the application of verb information during parsing. Journal of Experimental and Psychological Learning, 20, 786–803.
Özcan, A., Tulum, Z., Lamia, P., & Başkurt, F. (2004). Comparison of pressure pain threshold, grip strength, dexterity and touch pressure of dominant and non-dominant hands within and between right- and left-handed subjects. Journal of Korean Medical Science, 19, 874–878.
Pagin, P. (2004). Is assertion social? Journal of Pragmatics, 36, 833–859.
Park, H., & Reder, L. M. (2004). Moses illusion: Implication for human cognition. Available at: [URL].
Partee, B. H., Hajičová, E., & Sgall, P. (1998). Topic-focus articulation, tripartite structures, and semantic content. Amsterdam: Kluwer.
Pfurtscheller, G., & Lopes da Silva, F. H. (1999). Event-related EEG/MEG synchronization and desynchronization: Basic principles. Clinical Neurophysiology, 110, 1842–1857.
Piemontese, E. (1996). Capire e farsi capire. Teorie e tecniche della scrittura controllata. Napoli: Tecnodid.
Pierce, C. S. (1934). Belief and judgment. In C. Hartshorne & P. Weiss (Eds.), Collected Papers, Vol. V. Cambridge/Mass.: Harvard University Press.
Pinker, S., & Bloom, P. (1990). Natural language and natural selection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 13(4), 707–784.
Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a “theory of mind”? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4, 515–526.
Price, H. (1987). Truth and the nature of assertion. Mind, 96, 202–220.
Puglielli, A., & Frascarelli, M. (2008). L’analisi linguistica. Dai dati alla teoria. Italia: Caissa.
Rayner, K., Sereno, S. C., Morris, R. K., Schmauder, A. R. et al. (1989). Eye movements and on-line language comprehension processes. Language and Cognitive Processes, 4(3-4), 21–49.
Récanati, F. (1987). Meaning and force. The pragmatics of performative utterances. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Regel, S., Gunter, T. C., & Friederici, A. D. (2011). Isn’t it ironic? An electrophysiological exploration of figurative language processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 277–293.
Reinhart, T. (1982). Pragmatics and linguistics: An analysis of sentence topics. Philosophica, 27(1), 53–94.
Rigal, R. A. (1992). Which handedness: Preference or performance? Perceptual and Motor Skills, 75, 851–866.
Rizzi, L. (1997). The fine structure of the left periphery. In L. Haegeman (Ed.), Elements of grammar: Handbook in generative syntax (pp. 281–337). Netherlands: Springer.
Romoli, J., & Schwarz, F. (2015). An experimental comparison between presupposition and indirect scalar implicatures. In F. Schwarz (Ed.), Experimental perspectives on presuppositions [Studies in Theoretical Psycholinguistics Series] (pp. 215–240). Dordrecht: Springer International Publishing.
Rooth, M. (1985). Association with focus. PhD Thesis, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, GLSA, Dept. of Linguistics, South College, UMASS, Amherst MA 01003.
Rooth, M. (1992). A theory of focus interpretation. Natural Language Semantics, 1, 75–116.
Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of categorization. In E. Rosch & B. B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization (pp. 27–48). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Roth, J. (1979). Die indirekten Erlebnisformen im Bulgarischen: Eine Untersuchung zu ihrem Gebrauch in der Umgangssprache [Slavistische Beiträge 130]. Wiesbaden: Sagner.
Rothschild, D. (2011). Explaining presupposition projection with dynamic semantics. Semantics & Pragmatics, 4(3), 1–43.
Russell, B. (1905). On denoting. Mind, 14(56), 479–493.
Sapir, E. (1922). The fundamental elements of Northern Yana. University of California Publications in American Archaeology and Linguistics, 13, 215–234.
Sasse, H-J. (1987). The thetic/categorical distinction revisited. Linguistics, 25, 511–580.
Saussure, L. de. & Oswald, S. (2009). Argumentation et engagement du locuteur pour un point de vue subjectiviste. Nouveaux cahiers de linguistique française, 29, 215–243.
Saussure, L. de. (2013). Background relevance. Journal of Pragmatics, 59, 178–189.
Saussure, L. de. (2014). Présuppositions discursives, assertion d’arrière-plan et persuasion. In T. Herman & S. Oswald (Éds), Rhétorique et cognition: perspectives théoriques et stratégies persuasives/Rhetoric & Cognition theoretical perspectives and persuasive strategies (pp. 279–311). Berne: Peter Lang.
Sbisà, M. (2007). Detto non detto. Le forme della comunicazione implicita. Roma-Bari: Laterza.
Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1984). Theoretical note: Automatic and controlled processing revisited. Psychological Review, 91(2), 269–276.
Schumacher, P. B., & Hung, Y-C. (2012). Positional influences on information packaging: Insights from topological fields in German. Journal of Memory and Language, 67(2), 295–310.
Schwarz, F. (2014). Presuppositions are fast, whether hard or soft – Evidence from the visual world. Proceedings of
SALT
, 24, 1–22.
Schwarz, F., & Tiemann, S. (2015). Presupposition projection in online processing. Available at: [URL].
Schwarz, F. (2015). Presuppositions vs. Asserted content in online processing. In F. Schwarz (Ed.), Experimental perspectives on presupposition. Studies in theoretical psycholinguistics (pp. 89–108). Dordrecht: Springer International Publishing.
Searle, J. (1969). Speech acts: An essay in the philosophy of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Searle, J. (1975). A taxonomy of illocutionary acts. In K. Gunderson (Ed.), Language, mind and knowledge (pp. 344–369). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
Sgall, P., Hajičová, E., & Benešová, E. (1973). Topic, focus, and generative semantics. Michigan: Scriptor Verlag.
Simone, R., & Lombardi Vallauri, E. (2010). Natural constraints on language. Nature and consequences. Cahiers Ferdinand de Saussure, 63, 205–224.
Simone, R., & Lombardi Vallauri, E. (2011). Natural constraints on language. The ergonomics of the software. Cahiers Ferdinand de Saussure, 64, 119–141.
Simons, M. (2001). On the conversational basis of some presuppositions. In R. Hastings, B. Jackson, & Z. Zvolenszky (Eds.), SALT XI (pp. 431–448). Ithaca, NY: Cornell University.
Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (1986). Relevance: Communication and cognition. Oxford: Basil, Backwell.
Sperber, D., Clément, F., Heintz, C., Mascaro, O., Mercier, H., Origgi, G., & Wilson, D. (2010). Epistemic vigilance. Mind & Language, 25(4), 359–393.
Stalnaker, R. C. (1973). Presuppositions. Journal of Philosophical Logic, 2, 447–457.
Stalnaker, R. C. (1974). Pragmatic presuppositions. In M. K. Milton & U. K. Peter (Eds.), Semantics and philosophy (pp. 471–482). New York: University Press.
Stalnaker, R. C. (1978). Assertion. In P. Cole (Ed.), Syntax and semantics. Pragmatics 9 (pp. 315–332). New York: Academic Press.
Stalnaker, R. C. (1999). Context and content: Essays on intentionality in speech and thought. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Stoica, P., & Moses, R. (2005). Spectral analysis of signals. New Jersey: Prentice Hall.
Stokoe, W. C. (1960). Sign language structure: An outline of the visual communication system of the American Deaf. Studies in Linguistics, [Occasional papers, No. 8]. University of Buffalo: Department of Anthropology and Linguistics.
Strawson, P. F. (1949). Truth. Analysis, 8, 83–97.
Strawson, P. F. (1950). On referring. Mind, 59, 320–344.
Sturt, P., Sandford, A. J., Stewart, A., & Dawydiak, E. (2004). Linguistic focus and good-enough representations: An application of the change-detection paradigm. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 882–888.
Sweller, J. (2003). Evolution of human cognitive architecture. In B. H. Ross (Ed.), The Psychology of learning and motivations: Advances in research and theory 43 (pp. 215–266). USA: Elsevier Science.
Tallerman, M. (2007). Did our Ancestors speak a holistic protolanguage? Lingua, 117(3), 579–604.
Tanner, D., Morgan-Short, K., & Luck, S. J. (2015). How inappropriate high-pass filters can produce artifactual effects and incorrect conclusions in ERP studies of language and cognition. Psychophysiology, 52(8), 997–1009.
Thompson, S. A. (1985). Grammar and written discourse: Initial vs. final purpose clauses in English. Text, 5(1/2), 55–84.
Tiemann, S., Schmid, M., Bade, N., Rolke, B., Hertrich, I., Ackermann, H., et al. (2011). Psycholinguistic evidence for presuppositions: On-line and off-line data. In I. Reich, et al. (Eds.), Proceedings of Sinn & Bedeutung, Vol. 15 (pp. 581–595). Saarbrücken: Saarland University Press.
Toga, A. W., & Thompson, P. M. (2003). Mapping brain asymmetry. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 37–48.
Traugott, E. C. (2006). Grammaticalization, emergent constructions, and the notion of “newness”. Paper presented at HDLS, Albuquerque, Nov. German trans. [to appear] as “Grammatikalisierung und emergente Konstruktionen". In Anatol Stefanowitsch & Kerstin Fischer (Eds.), Konstruktionsgrammatik und grammatische Konstruktionen. Tübingen: Stauffenburg.
Traugott, E. C. (2010). Revisiting subjectification and intersubjectification. In K. Davidse, L. Vandelanotte, & H. Cuyckens (Eds.), Subjectification, intersubjectification and grammaticalization (pp. 29–70). Berlin: De Gruyter.
Trent, N. (1997). Linguistic coding of evidentiality in Japanese spoken discourse and Japanese politeness. Doctoral Dissertation, Department of Foreign Language Education. Austin, University of Texas.
Vallduví, E. (1993). Information packaging: A survey. Paper presented at the WOP IS (Word Order, Prosody, and Information Structure), Center for Cognitive Science & Human Communication Research Center, University of Edinburgh.
Vallduví, E., & Engdahl, E. (1996). The linguistic realization of information packaging. Linguistics, 34(3), 459–520.
Van Berkum, J. J. A., Van den Brink, D., Tesink, C. M. J. Y., Kos, M., & Hagoort, P. (2008). The neural integration of speaker and message. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(4), 580–591.
Van Berkum, J. J. A., Holleman, B., Nieuwland, M. S., Otten, M., & Murre, J. (2009). Right or wrong? The brain's fast response to morally objectionable statements. Psychological Science, 20(9), 1092–1099.
Van Berkum, J. J. A. (2010). The brain is a prediction machine that cares about good and bad – any implications for neuropragmatics? Italian Journal of Linguistics, 22, 181–208.
Van der Sandt, R. A. (1992). Presupposition projection as anaphora. Journal of Semantics, 9, 333–377.
Van Oers, B. (1998). From context to contextualizing. Learning and Instruction, 8(6), 473–488.
Van Osstendorp, H., & Kok, I. (1990). Failing to noticing errors in sentences. Language and Cognitive Processes, 5, 105–113.
Van Petten, C., & Kutas, M. (1990). Interactions between sentence context and word frequency in event-related brain potentials. Memory & Cognition, 18(4), 380–393.
Vespignani, F., Canal, P., Molinaro, N., Fonda, S., & Cacciari, C. (2010). Predictive mechanisms in idiom comprehension. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(8), 1682–1700.
Walton, D. (1993). Commitment, types of dialogues, and fallacies. Informal Logic, XIV(2–3), 93–103.
Wang, L., Bastiaansen, M., Yang, Y., & Hagoort, P. (2011). The influence of information structure on the depth of semantic processing: How focus and pitch accent determine the size of the N400 effect. Neuropsychologia, 49(5), 813–820.
Wang, L., Jensen, O., Van den Brink, D., Weder, N., Schoffelen, J-M., Magyari, L., Hagoort, P., & Bastiaansen, M. (2012). Beta oscillations relate to the N400 during language comprehension. Human Brain Mapping, 33(12), 2898–2912.
Wang, L., & Schumacher, P. B. (2013). New is not always costly: Evidence from online processing of topic and contrast in Japanese. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 363.
Ward, P., & Sturt, P. (2007). Linguistic focus and memory: An eye movement study. Memory & Cognition, 35(1), 73–86.
Weil, H. (1844). De l’ordre des mots dans les langages anciennes comparées aux langues modernes. Paris: Joubert. [En. tr. By Charles William Super as The order of words in the ancient languages compared with that of the modern languages, 1978, Amsterdam, Benjamins].
Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72(3), 655–684.
Wetzel, F., & Molfese, D. L. (1992). The processing of presuppositional information contained in sentences: Electrohpysiological correlates. Brain and Language, 42, 286–307.
Wolfe, J. M., Butcher, S. J., Lee, C., & Hyle, M. (2003). Changing your mind: On the contributions of top-down and bottom-up guidance in visual search for features singletons. Journal pf Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29(2), 483–502.
Wray, A. (1998). Protolanguage as a holistic system for social interaction. Language and Communication, 18, 47–67.
Wray, A. (2000). Holistic utterances in protolanguage: The link from primates to humans. In C. Knight, M. Studdert-Kennedy, & J. Hurford (Eds.), The evolutionary emergence of language (pp. 285–302). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.