Article published In:
Linguistics in the Netherlands 2021
Edited by Mark Dingemanse, Eva van Lier and Jorrig Vogels
[Linguistics in the Netherlands 38] 2021
► pp. 144162
References (39)
References
Apperly, Ian. 2010. Mindreaders: The Cognitive Basis of “Theory of Mind.” London: Psychology Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Banfield, Ann. 1982. Unspeakable sentences: Narration and representation in the language of fiction. Boston: Routledge & Kegan Paul Ltd.Google Scholar
Barr, Dale J., Roger Levy, Christoph Scheepers & Harry J. Tily. 2013. “Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal.” Journal of memory and language 68 (3): 255–278. DOI logo)Google Scholar
Bates, Douglas, Reinhold Kliegl, Shravan Vasishth & Harald Baayen. 2015. “Parsimonious mixed models.” arXiv preprint arXiv:1506.04967.Google Scholar
Bates, Douglas, Martin Mächler, Ben Bolker & Steve Walker. 2015. “Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4.” Journal of Statistical Software 67 (1): 1–48. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bray, Joe. 2007. “The ‘dual voice’ of free indirect discourse: a reading experiment.” Language and Literature. Sage Publications Sage CA: Thousand Oaks, CA 16 (1): 37–52. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brennan, Susan E. & Joy E. Hanna. 2009. “Partner-specific adaptation in dialog.” Topics in Cognitive Science 1 (2): 274–291. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brown-Schmidt, Sarah & Joy Hanna. 2011. “Talking in another person’s shoes: Incremental perspective-taking in language processing.” Dialogue & Discourse 2 (1): 11–33. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dancygier, Barbara. 2011. The Language of Stories: A Cognitive Approach. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dancygier, Barbara, Wei-lun Lu & Arie Verhagen, eds. 2016. Viewpoint and the Fabric of Meaning (Form and Use of Viewpoint Tools across Languages and Modalities, 10). Berlin: Mouton. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eckardt, Regine. 2014. The semantics of free indirect speech. How texts let you read minds and eavesdrop. Vol. 311. Leiden: Brill.Google Scholar
Epley, Nicholas, Boaz Keysar, Leaf Van Boven & Thomas Gilovich. 2004. “Perspective Taking as Egocentric Anchoring and Adjustment.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 87 (3): 327–339. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ferguson, Heather J., Ian Apperly & Jumana Ahmad, Markus Bindemann & James Cane. 2015. “Task constraints distinguish perspective inferences from perspective use during discourse interpretation in a false belief task.” Cognition 1391: 50–70. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fludernik, Monika. 2003. The fictions of language and the languages of fiction. London, New York: Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Freeman, Jonathan B. & Nalini Ambady. 2010. “MouseTracker: Software for studying real-time mental processing using a computer mouse-tracking method.” Behavior Research Methods 42 (1): 226–241. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hanna, Joy E., Michael K. Tanenhaus & John C. Trueswell. 2003. “The effects of common ground and perspective on domains of referential interpretation.” Journal of Memory and Language 49 (1): 43–61. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hehman, Eric, Ryan M. Stolier & Jonathan B. Freeman. 2015. “Advanced mouse-tracking analytic techniques for enhancing psychological science.” Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 18 (3): 384–401. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Heller, Daphna, Daniel Grodner & Michael K. Tanenhaus. 2008. “The role of perspective in identifying domains of reference.” Cognition 108 (3): 831–836. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Horton, William S. & Boaz Keysar. 1996. “When do speakers take into account common ground?Cognition 59 (1): 91–117. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Horton, William S. & David N. Rapp. 2003. “Out of sight, out of mind: Occlusion and the accessibility of information in narrative comprehension.” Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 10 (1): 104–110. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaiser, Elsi. 2015. “Perspective-shifting and free indirect discourse: Experimental investigations.” Proceedings of Semantics and Linguistic Theory 251: 346–372. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Keysar, Boaz, Dale J. Barr, Jennifer A. Balin & Jason S. Brauner. 2000. “Taking perspective in conversation: The role of mutual knowledge in comprehension.” Psychological Science 11 (1): 32–38. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Keysar, Boaz, Shuhong Lin & Dale J. Barr. 2003. “Limits on theory of mind use in adults.” Cognition 89 (1): 25–41. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kieslich, Pascal J. & Felix Henninger. 2017. “Mousetrap: An integrated, open-source mouse-tracking package.” Behavior Research Methods 49 (5): 1652–1667. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kieslich, Pascal J., Felix Henninger, Dirk U. Wulff, Jonas Haslbeck & Michael Schulte-Mecklenbeck. 2018. “Mouse-tracking: A practical guide to implementation and analysis.” PsyArXiv. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Köder, Franziska & Emar Maier. 2018. “The advantage of story-telling: children’s interpretation of reported speech in narratives.” Journal of Child Language. Cambridge University Press 45 (2): 541–557. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Köder, Franziska, Emar Maier & Petra Hendriks. 2015. “Perspective shift increases processing effort of pronouns: a comparison between direct and indirect speech.” Language, Cognition and Neuroscience. Routledge 30 (8): 940–946. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kronmüller, Edmundo & Ernesto Guerra. 2020. “Processing speaker-specific information in two stages during the interpretation of referential precedents.” Frontiers in Psychology 111. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuznetsova, Alexandra, Per Bruun Brockhoff & Rune Haubo Bojesen Christensen. 2017. “lmerTest Package: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects ModelsJournal of Statistical Software 821: 1–26. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meuser, Sara, Stefan Hinterwimmer & Maximilian Hörl. 2020. “Online-processing of protagonists’ perspective-taking” (poster presented at CUNY 2020).
Nadig, Aparna S. & Julie C. Sedivy. 2002. “Evidence of Perspective-Taking Constraints in Children’s On-Line Reference Resolution.” Psychological Science 13 (4): 329–336. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
R Core Team. 2020. “R: A language and environment for statistical computing.” R foundation for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria, [URL]
Salem, Susanna, Thomas Weskott & Anke Holler. 2017. “Does narrative perspective influence readers’ perspective-taking? An empirical study on free indirect discourse, psycho-narration and first-person narration.” Glossa: a journal of general linguistics 2 (1): 61. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sato, Manami, Hiromu Sakai, Jennifer Wu & Benjamin K. Bergen. 2012. “Towards a Cognitive Science of Literary Style: Perspective-Taking in Processing Omniscient versus Objective Voice.” Proceedings of the 34th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society ed. by Naomi Miyake, David Peebles and Richard P. Cooper, 959–964. Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Schlenker, Philippe. 2004. “Context of thought and context of utterance: A note on free indirect discourse and the historical present.” Mind & Language. Wiley Online Library 19 (3): 279–304. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Spivey, Michael J., Marc Grosjean & Günther Knoblich. 2005. “Continuous attraction toward phonological competitors.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. National Academy of Sciences 102 (29): 10393–10398. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomlinson, John M., Todd M. Bailey & Lewis Bott. 2013. “Possibly all of that and then some: Scalar implicatures are understood in two steps.” Journal of Memory and Language 69 (1): 18–35. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vandelanotte, Lieven. 2009. Speech and Thought Representation in English: A Cognitive-Functional Approach. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zehr, Jeremy & Florian Schwartz. 2018. “PennController for Internet Based Experiments (IBEX).” DOI logoGoogle Scholar