Chapter published in:
How the Brain Got Language – Towards a New Road Map
Edited by Michael A. Arbib
[Benjamins Current Topics 112] 2020
► pp. 216238
References

References

Antinoro Pizzuto, E., Rossini, P., Sallandre, M. A. & Wilkinson, E.
(2006) Deixis, anaphora and highly iconic structures: Cross-linguistic evidence on American (ASL), French (LSF) and Italian (LIS) signed languages. TISLR9 (pp. 475–495). Editora Arara Azul, Brazil.Google Scholar
Arbib, M. A., Ganesh, V. & Gasser, B.
(2014) Dyadic Brain Modeling, Ontogenetic Ritualization of Gesture in Apes, and the Contributions of Primate Mirror Neuron Systems. Phil Trans Roy Soc B, 369 (1644), 20130414. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Arbib, M. A.
(2016) Towards a Computational Comparative Neuroprimatology: Framing the language-ready brain. Physics of Life Reviews, 16, 1–54. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Arbib, M. A. & Rizzolatti, G.
(1997) Neural expectations: A possible evolutionary path from manual skills to language. Communication and Cognition, 29, 393–424.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Camaioni, L. & Volterra, V.
(1975) The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merril Palmer Quarterly, 21, 205–226.[ p. 231 ]Google Scholar
Bello, A., Giannantoni, P., Pettenati, P., Stefanini, S. & Caselli, M. C.
(2012) Assessing lexicon: validation and developmental data of the Picture Naming Game (PiNG), a new picture naming task for toddlers. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 47, 589–602. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Boyatzis, C. J. & Watson, M. W.
(1993) Preschool children’s symbolic representation of objects through gestures. Child Development, 64, 729–735. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Boyes Braem, P.
(1981) Features of the handshape in American Sign Language. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California, San Diego.Google Scholar
Brentari, D., Di Renzo, A., Keane, J. & Volterra, V.
(2014) Cognitive, Cultural, and Linguistic Sources of a Handshape Distinction Expressing Agentivity. Topics in Cognitive Science 1–29. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Capirci, O., Contaldo, A., Caselli, M. C. & Volterra, V.
(2005) From Action to language through gesture: a longitudinal perspective, Gesture, 5, 155–177. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Capirci, O., Cristilli, C., De Angelis, V. & Graziano, M.
(2011) Learning to use gesture in narratives: developmental trends in formal and semantic gesture competence. In G. Stam & M. Ishino (Eds.) Integrating Gestures (pp. 189–200), Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Capirci, O., Iverson, J. M., Pizzuto, E. & Volterra, V.
(1996) Gestures and words during the transition to two-word speech. Journal of child language, 23, 645–673. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Capobianco, E., Antinoro Pizzuto, E. & Devescovi, A.
(2017) Gesture–speech combinations and early verbal abilities. New longitudinal data during the second year of age. Interaction Studies 18(1), 55–76. Crossref.Google Scholar
Caselli, M. C. & Volterra, V.
(1994) From communication to language in hearing and deaf children. In V. Volterra & C. J. Erting (Eds.) From Gesture to Language in Hearing and Deaf Children (pp. 263–277). 2nd ed., Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.Google Scholar
Caselli, M. C.
(1990) Communicative gestures and first words. In V. Volterra, & C. J. Erting (Eds.), From gesture to Language in Hearing and Deaf Children (pp. 56–67). Berlin: Springer-Verlag. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Caselli, M. C., Rinaldi, P., Stefanini, S. & Volterra, V.
(2012) Early Action and Gesture ‘‘Vocabulary’’ and Its Relation With Word Comprehension and Production. Child Development, 83, 526–542. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H.
(2016) Depicting as a method of communication. Psychological review, 123(3), 324–347. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Corballis, M. C.
(2002) From hand to mouth. The origins of language. Princeton, Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Cuxac, C.
(2000) La langue des signes française (LSF): les voies de l’iconocité (No. 15–16). Ophrys.Google Scholar
Enfield, N. J.
(2001) ‘Lip-pointing’: A discussion of form and function with reference to data from Laos. Gesture, 1(2), 185–211. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. & Butcher, C.
(2003) Pointing toward two-word speech in children. In S. Kita (Ed.), Pointing: where language, culture and cognition meet (pp. 85–107). Mahwah, N.J.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gretscher, H., Tempelmann, S., Haun, D. B. M., Liebal, K. & Kaminski, J.
(2017) Prelinguistic human infants and great apes show different communicative strategies in a triadic request situation. PLoS ONE 12, 4, e0175227. Crossref[ p. 232 ]Google Scholar
Halina, M., Rossano, F. & Tomasello, M.
(2013) The ontogenetic ritualization of bonobo gestures. Animal cognition, 16(4), 653–666. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Iverson, J. M., Capirci, O. & Caselli, M. C.
(1994) From communication to language in two modalities. Cognitive Development, 9, 23–43. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Iverson, J. M. & Thelen, E.
(1999) Hand, mouth, and brain: The dynamic emergence of speech and gestures. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6, 19–40.Google Scholar
Kendon, A.
(2014) Semiotic diversity in utterance production and the concept of ‘language’. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 369, 20130293. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kita, S.
(Ed.) (2003) Pointing: Where language, culture, and cognition meet. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Leavens, D. A.
(2004) Manual deixis in apes and humans. Interaction Studies, 5(3), 387–408. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Liddell, S. K. & Metzger, M.
(1998) Gesture in sign language discourse. Journal of Pragmatics, 30, 657–697. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Liebal, K.
(2016) The ontogeny of great ape gesture – not a simple story: comment on “Towards a computational comparative neuroprimatology: framing the language-ready brain” by M.A. Arbib. Physics of Life Reviews, 16, 85–87. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Marentette, P. & Nicoladis, E.
(2012) Does ontogenetic ritualization explain early communicative gestures in human infants? In S. Pika & K. Liebal (Eds.). Developments in Primate Gesture Research (pp. 33–53). John Benjamins Publishing Company CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Marentette, P., Pettenati, P., Bello, A. & Volterra, V.
(2016) Gesture and symbolic representation in Italian and English-speaking Canadian two-year-olds. Child Development, 87(3), 944–961. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D.
(1992) Hand and mind. What the hands reveal about thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Newman, A. J., Supalla, T., Fernandez, N., Newport, E. L. & Bavelier, D.
(2015) Neural systems supporting linguistic structure, linguistic experience, and symbolic communication in sign language and gesture. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 112(37), 11684–89. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Occhino, C. & Wilcox, S.
(2017) Gesture or sign? A categorization problem. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 40, e66. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Padden, C. A., Meir, I., Hwang, S. O., Lepic, R., Seegers, S. & Sampson, T.
(2013) Patterned iconicity in sign language lexicons. Gesture, 13(3), 287–308. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pettenati, P., Stefanini, S. & Volterra, V.
(2010) Motoric characteristics of representational gestures produced by young children in a naming task. Journal of Child Language, 37(4), 887–911. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J.
(1945) La formation du symbole chez l’enfant: Imitation, jeu et rêve, image et representation. Neuchatel; Paris: Delachaux et Niestlé.Google Scholar
Reddy, V.
(2003) On being the object of attention: implications for self–other consciousness. TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences, 7(9), 397–402. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Russon, A. E. & Andrews, K. A.
(2011) Pantomime in great apes: evidence and implications. Communicative and Integrative Biology, 4(3), 315–317. Crossref[ p. 233 ]Google Scholar
Pettenati, P., Sekine, K., Congestrì, E. & Volterra, V.
(2012) A comparative study on representational gestures in Italian and Japanese children. Journal of Non Verbal Behaviour, 36, 149–164. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Slobin, D.
(2008) Breaking the Molds: Signed language and the nature of human language. Sign Language Studies, 8(2), 114–130. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sparaci, L. & Volterra, V.
(2017) Hands shaping communication: From gestures to signs. In: M. Bertolaso & N. Di Stefano (Eds.), The Hand. Studies in Applied Philosophy, Epistemology and Rational Ethics, vol 38, (pp. 29–54). Springer, Cham.Google Scholar
Stefanini, S., Bello, A., Caselli, M. C., Iverson, J. M. & Volterra, V.
(2009) Co-Speech gestures in a naming task: Developmental data. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24(2), 168–189. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M.
(2006) Why don’t apes point? In N. J. Nicholadis & S. C. Levinson (Eds.), Roots of human sociality: Culture, Cognition, and Interaction (pp. 506–524). Oxford: Berg.Google Scholar
(2008) The origin of human communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Volterra, V., Capirci, O., Caselli, M. C., Rinaldi, P., & Sparaci, L.
(2017) Developmental evidence for continuity from action to gesture to sign/word. Language, Interaction, Acquisition, 8 (1), 13–41. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Zukow-Goldring, P.
(2012) Assisted imitation: First steps in the seed model of language development. Language Sciences, 34, 569–582. CrossrefGoogle Scholar