Part of
How the Brain Got Language – Towards a New Road Map
Edited by Michael A. Arbib
[Benjamins Current Topics 112] 2020
► pp. 216238
References
Antinoro Pizzuto, E., Rossini, P., Sallandre, M. A. & Wilkinson, E.
(2006) Deixis, anaphora and highly iconic structures: Cross-linguistic evidence on American (ASL), French (LSF) and Italian (LIS) signed languages. TISLR9 (pp. 475–495). Editora Arara Azul, Brazil.Google Scholar
Arbib, M. A., Ganesh, V. & Gasser, B.
(2014) Dyadic Brain Modeling, Ontogenetic Ritualization of Gesture in Apes, and the Contributions of Primate Mirror Neuron Systems. Phil Trans Roy Soc B, 369 (1644), 20130414. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arbib, M. A.
(2016) Towards a Computational Comparative Neuroprimatology: Framing the language-ready brain. Physics of Life Reviews, 16, 1–54. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arbib, M. A. & Rizzolatti, G.
(1997) Neural expectations: A possible evolutionary path from manual skills to language. Communication and Cognition, 29, 393–424.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Camaioni, L. & Volterra, V.
(1975) The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merril Palmer Quarterly, 21, 205–226.Google Scholar
Bello, A., Giannantoni, P., Pettenati, P., Stefanini, S. & Caselli, M. C.
(2012) Assessing lexicon: validation and developmental data of the Picture Naming Game (PiNG), a new picture naming task for toddlers. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 47, 589–602. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boyatzis, C. J. & Watson, M. W.
(1993) Preschool children’s symbolic representation of objects through gestures. Child Development, 64, 729–735. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boyes Braem, P.
(1981) Features of the handshape in American Sign Language. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California, San Diego.Google Scholar
Brentari, D., Di Renzo, A., Keane, J. & Volterra, V.
(2014) Cognitive, Cultural, and Linguistic Sources of a Handshape Distinction Expressing Agentivity. Topics in Cognitive Science 1–29. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Capirci, O., Contaldo, A., Caselli, M. C. & Volterra, V.
Capirci, O., Cristilli, C., De Angelis, V. & Graziano, M.
(2011) Learning to use gesture in narratives: developmental trends in formal and semantic gesture competence. In G. Stam & M. Ishino (Eds.) Integrating Gestures (pp. 189–200), Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Capirci, O., Iverson, J. M., Pizzuto, E. & Volterra, V.
(1996) Gestures and words during the transition to two-word speech. Journal of child language, 23, 645–673. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Capobianco, E., Antinoro Pizzuto, E. & Devescovi, A.
Caselli, M. C. & Volterra, V.
(1994) From communication to language in hearing and deaf children. In V. Volterra & C. J. Erting (Eds.) From Gesture to Language in Hearing and Deaf Children (pp. 263–277). 2nd ed., Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.Google Scholar
Caselli, M. C.
(1990) Communicative gestures and first words. In V. Volterra, & C. J. Erting (Eds.), From gesture to Language in Hearing and Deaf Children (pp. 56–67). Berlin: Springer-Verlag. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Caselli, M. C., Rinaldi, P., Stefanini, S. & Volterra, V.
(2012) Early Action and Gesture ‘‘Vocabulary’’ and Its Relation With Word Comprehension and Production. Child Development, 83, 526–542. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H.
(2016) Depicting as a method of communication. Psychological review, 123(3), 324–347. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Corballis, M. C.
(2002) From hand to mouth. The origins of language. Princeton, Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Cuxac, C.
(2000) La langue des signes française (LSF): les voies de l’iconocité (No. 15–16). Ophrys.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. & Butcher, C.
(2003) Pointing toward two-word speech in children. In S. Kita (Ed.), Pointing: where language, culture and cognition meet (pp. 85–107). Mahwah, N.J.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gretscher, H., Tempelmann, S., Haun, D. B. M., Liebal, K. & Kaminski, J.
(2017) Prelinguistic human infants and great apes show different communicative strategies in a triadic request situation. PLoS ONE 12, 4, e0175227. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Halina, M., Rossano, F. & Tomasello, M.
(2013) The ontogenetic ritualization of bonobo gestures. Animal cognition, 16(4), 653–666. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Iverson, J. M., Capirci, O. & Caselli, M. C.
(1994) From communication to language in two modalities. Cognitive Development, 9, 23–43. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Iverson, J. M. & Thelen, E.
(1999) Hand, mouth, and brain: The dynamic emergence of speech and gestures. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6, 19–40.Google Scholar
Kendon, A.
(2014) Semiotic diversity in utterance production and the concept of ‘language’. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 369, 20130293. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kita, S.
(Ed.) (2003) Pointing: Where language, culture, and cognition meet. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leavens, D. A.
(2004) Manual deixis in apes and humans. Interaction Studies, 5(3), 387–408. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liddell, S. K. & Metzger, M.
(1998) Gesture in sign language discourse. Journal of Pragmatics, 30, 657–697. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liebal, K.
(2016) The ontogeny of great ape gesture – not a simple story: comment on “Towards a computational comparative neuroprimatology: framing the language-ready brain” by M.A. Arbib. Physics of Life Reviews, 16, 85–87. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marentette, P. & Nicoladis, E.
(2012) Does ontogenetic ritualization explain early communicative gestures in human infants? In S. Pika & K. Liebal (Eds.). Developments in Primate Gesture Research (pp. 33–53). John Benjamins Publishing Company DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marentette, P., Pettenati, P., Bello, A. & Volterra, V.
(2016) Gesture and symbolic representation in Italian and English-speaking Canadian two-year-olds. Child Development, 87(3), 944–961. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D.
(1992) Hand and mind. What the hands reveal about thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Newman, A. J., Supalla, T., Fernandez, N., Newport, E. L. & Bavelier, D.
(2015) Neural systems supporting linguistic structure, linguistic experience, and symbolic communication in sign language and gesture. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 112(37), 11684–89. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Occhino, C. & Wilcox, S.
(2017) Gesture or sign? A categorization problem. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 40, e66. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Padden, C. A., Meir, I., Hwang, S. O., Lepic, R., Seegers, S. & Sampson, T.
(2013) Patterned iconicity in sign language lexicons. Gesture, 13(3), 287–308. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pettenati, P., Stefanini, S. & Volterra, V.
(2010) Motoric characteristics of representational gestures produced by young children in a naming task. Journal of Child Language, 37(4), 887–911. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J.
(1945) La formation du symbole chez l’enfant: Imitation, jeu et rêve, image et representation. Neuchatel; Paris: Delachaux et Niestlé.Google Scholar
Reddy, V.
(2003) On being the object of attention: implications for self–other consciousness. TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences, 7(9), 397–402. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Russon, A. E. & Andrews, K. A.
(2011) Pantomime in great apes: evidence and implications. Communicative and Integrative Biology, 4(3), 315–317. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pettenati, P., Sekine, K., Congestrì, E. & Volterra, V.
(2012) A comparative study on representational gestures in Italian and Japanese children. Journal of Non Verbal Behaviour, 36, 149–164. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Slobin, D.
(2008) Breaking the Molds: Signed language and the nature of human language. Sign Language Studies, 8(2), 114–130. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sparaci, L. & Volterra, V.
(2017) Hands shaping communication: From gestures to signs. In: M. Bertolaso & N. Di Stefano (Eds.), The Hand. Studies in Applied Philosophy, Epistemology and Rational Ethics, vol 38, (pp. 29–54). Springer, Cham.Google Scholar
Stefanini, S., Bello, A., Caselli, M. C., Iverson, J. M. & Volterra, V.
(2009) Co-Speech gestures in a naming task: Developmental data. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24(2), 168–189. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M.
(2006) Why don’t apes point? In N. J. Nicholadis & S. C. Levinson (Eds.), Roots of human sociality: Culture, Cognition, and Interaction (pp. 506–524). Oxford: Berg.Google Scholar
(2008) The origin of human communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Volterra, V., Capirci, O., Caselli, M. C., Rinaldi, P., & Sparaci, L.
(2017) Developmental evidence for continuity from action to gesture to sign/word. Language, Interaction, Acquisition, 8 (1), 13–41. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zukow-Goldring, P.
(2012) Assisted imitation: First steps in the seed model of language development. Language Sciences, 34, 569–582. DOI logoGoogle Scholar