Chapter published in:
How the Brain Got Language – Towards a New Road Map
Edited by Michael A. Arbib
[Benjamins Current Topics 112] 2020
► pp. 256271
References

References

Aboitiz, Francisco
(2018) Voice, gesture and working memory in the emergence of speech. Interaction Studies. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Abramova, Ekaterina
(2018) The role of pantomime in gestural language evolution, its cognitive bases and an alternative. Journal of Language Evolution. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Antón, Susan C., Potts, Richard, & Aiello, Leslie C.
(2014) Evolution of early Homo: An integrated biological perspective. Science, 345(6192), 1236828. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Arbib, Michael A.
(2018) In support of the role of pantomime in language evolution. Journal of Language Evolution. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2012) How the brain got language: The mirror system hypothesis (Vol. 16). Oxford University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2016) Towards a Computational Comparative Neuroprimatology: Framing the language-ready brain. Physics of Life Reviews, 16, 1–54. Crossref[ p. 268 ]Google Scholar
Bogin, Barry, Bragg, Jared, & Kuzawa, Christopher
(2014) Humans are not cooperative breeders but practice biocultural reproduction. Annals of human biology, 41(4), 368–380. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Brauer, Jens, Anwander, Alfred, Perani, Daniela, & Friederici, Angela D.
(2013) Dorsal and ventral pathways in language development. Brain and language, 127(2), 289–295. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Christiansen, Morten H., & Chater, Nick
(2016) Creating language: integrating evolution, acquisition, and processing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
d’Errico, F., & Stringer, C. B.
(2011) Evolution, revolution or saltation scenario for the emergence of modern cultures? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366(1567), 1060. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dippel, Gabriel, & Beste, Christian
(2015) A causal role of the right inferior frontal cortex in implementing strategies for multi-component behaviour. Nature communications, 6. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Emmorey, Karen, McCullough, Stephen, Mehta, Sonya, Ponto, Laura L. B., & Grabowski, Thomas J.
(2011) Sign language and pantomime production differentially engage frontal and parietal cortices. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26(7), 878–901. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fagg, Andrew H., & Arbib, Michael A.
(1998) Modeling parietal-premotor interactions in primate control of grasping. Neural Networks, 11(7–8), 1277–1303. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fitch, W., & Martins, Mauricio D.
(2014) Hierarchical processing in music, language, and action: Lashley revisited. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1316(1), 87–104. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fragaszy, Dorothy M., Biro, D., Eshchar, Y., Humle, T., Izar, P., Resende, B., & Visalberghi, E.
(2013) The fourth dimension of tool use: temporally enduring artefacts aid primates learning to use tools. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 368(1630). CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Frost, Ram, Armstrong, Blair C., Siegelman, Noam, & Christiansen, Morten H.
(2015) Domain generality versus modality specificity: the paradox of statistical learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(3), 117–125. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gärdenfors, Peter, & Högberg, Anders
(2017) The archaeology of teaching and the evolution of Homo docens. Current Anthropology, 58(2), 000–000. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gobet, Fernand, Lane, Peter C. R., Croker, Steve, Cheng, Peter C. H., Jones, Gary, Oliver, Iain, & Pine, Julian M.
(2001) Chunking mechanisms in human learning. Trends in cognitive sciences, 5(6), 236–243. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, Adele E.
(2003) Constructions: a new theoretical approach to language. Trends in cognitive sciences, 7(5), 219–224. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gómez-Robles, Aida, Hopkins, William D., Schapiro, Steven J., & Sherwood, Chet C.
(2015) Relaxed genetic control of cortical organization in human brains compared with chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(48), 14799–14804. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gowlett, John A. J.
(1984) Mental abilities of early man: A look at some hard evidence. In R. Foley (Ed.), Hominid Evolution and Community Ecology (pp. 167–192). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Greenfield, Patricia M.
(1991) Language, tools, and brain: The development and evolution of hierarchically organized sequential behavior. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 14, 531–595. Crossref[ p. 269 ]Google Scholar
Hecht, Erin E., Gutman, D. A., Khreisheh, N., Taylor, S. V., Kilner, J., Faisal, A. A., … Stout, D.
(2014) Acquisition of Paleolithic toolmaking abilities involves structural remodeling to inferior frontoparietal regions. Brain Structure and Function, 1–17. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hecht, Erin E., Gutman, David A., Preuss, Todd M., Sanchez, Mar M., Parr, Lisa A., & Rilling, James K.
(2013) Process Versus Product in Social Learning: Comparative Diffusion Tensor Imaging of Neural Systems for Action Execution–Observation Matching in Macaques, Chimpanzees, and Humans. Cerebral Cortex, 23(5), 1014–1024. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hecht, Erin E., Gutman, David A., Bradley, Bruce A., Preuss, Todd M., & Stout, Dietrich
(2015) Virtual dissection and comparative connectivity of the superior longitudinal fasciculus in chimpanzees and humans. NeuroImage, 108, 124–137. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Heyes, Cecilia M., & Frith, Chris D.
(2014) The cultural evolution of mind reading. Science, 344(6190), 1243091. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hill, Kim, Barton, M., & Hurtado, A. M.
(2009) The emergence of human uniqueness: Characters underlying behavioral modernity. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 18(5), 187–200. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ingold, Tim
(1997) Eight themes in the anthropology of technology. Social Analysis, 41, 106–138.Google Scholar
Isler, Karin, & Van Schaik, Carel P.
(2014) How humans evolved large brains: Comparative evidence. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 23(2), 65–75. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kaplan, Hillard, Hill, Kim, Lancaster, Jane, & Hurtado, A. Magdalena
(2000) A theory of human life history evolution: Diet, intelligence, and longevity. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 9(4), 156–185. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Koechlin, Etienne, & Jubault, Thomas
(2006) Broca’s Area and the hierarchical organization of human behavior. Neuron, 50(6), 963–974. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kolodny, Oren, Edelman, Shimon, & Lotem, Arnon
(2015) Evolution of protolinguistic abilities as a by-product of learning to forage in structured environments. Proc. R. Soc. B, 282(1811), 20150353. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Laland, Kevin N., Uller, Tobias, Feldman, Marcus W., Sterelny, Kim, Müller, Gerd B., Moczek, Armin, … Odling-Smee, John
(2015) The extended evolutionary synthesis: its structure, assumptions and predictions. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 282(1813). CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lambon Ralph, Matthew A., Jefferies, Elizabeth, Patterson, Karalyn, & Rogers, Timothy T.
(2016) The neural and computational bases of semantic cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience.Google Scholar
Levinson, Stephen C., & Holler, Judith
(2014) The origin of human multi-modal communication. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 369(1651). CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, Jason E., & Harmand, Sonia
(2016) An earlier origin for stone tool making: implications for cognitive evolution and the transition to Homo. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B, 371(1698), 20150233. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Milner, A. David, & Goodale, Melvyn A.
(1995) The visual brain in action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.[ p. 270 ]Google Scholar
Montgomery, Kimberly J., Isenberg, Nancy, & Haxby, James V.
(2007) Communicative hand gestures and object-directed hand movements activated the mirror neuron system. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2(2), 114–122. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Moore, Mark W.
(2010) “Grammars of action” and stone flaking design space. In A. Nowell & I. Davidson (Eds.), Stone tools and the evolution of human cognition (pp. 13–43). Boulder, Colorado: University Press of Colorado.Google Scholar
Morgan, T. J. H., Uomini, N. T., Rendell, L. E., Chouinard-Thuly, L., Street, S. E., Lewis, H. M., … Laland, K. N.
(2015) Experimental evidence for the co-evolution of hominin tool-making teaching and language. 6, 6029. Crossref https://​www​.nature​.com​/articles​/ncomms7029#supplementary​-information
Nonaka, Tetsushi, Bril, Blandine, & Rein, Robert
(2010) How do stone knappers predict and control the outcome of flaking? Implications for understanding early stone tool technology. Journal of Human Evolution, 59(2), 155–167. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Orban, Guy A., & Caruana, Fausto
(2014) The neural basis of human tool use. Frontiers in psychology, 5(310), 12.Google Scholar
Putt, Shelby S., Wijeakumar, Sobanawartiny, Franciscus, Robert G., & Spencer, John P.
(2017) The functional brain networks that underlie Early Stone Age tool manufacture. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(6), 0102. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rilling, James K., Glasser, Matthew F., Jbabdi, Saad, Andersson, Jesper, & Preuss, Todd M.
(2011) Continuity, Divergence, and the Evolution of Brain Language Pathways. Frontiers in Evolutionary Neuroscience, 3, 11. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Roepstorff, Andreas, Niewöhner, Jörg, & Beck, Stefan
(2010) Enculturing brains through patterned practices. Neural Networks, 23(8), 1051–1059. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ruff, Christopher B., Trinkaus, Erik, & Holliday, Trenton W.
(1997) Body mass and encephalization in Pleistocene Homo. Nature, 387(6629), 173–176. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Russon, Anne
(2018) Pantomime and imitation in great apes: Implications for reconstructing the evolution of language. Interaction Studies. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schell, Marianne, Zaccarella, Emiliano, & Friederici, Angela D.
(2017) Differential cortical contribution of syntax and semantics: An fMRI study on two-word phrasal processing. Cortex, 96, 105–120. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stewart, J. R., & Stringer, C. B.
(2012) Human Evolution Out of Africa: The Role of Refugia and Climate Change. Science, 335(6074), 1317–1321. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stout, Dietrich
(2002) Skill and cognition in stone tool production: An ethnographic case study from Irian Jaya. Current Anthropology, 45(3), 693–722. Crossref[ p. 271 ]Google Scholar
(2011) Stone toolmaking and the evolution of human culture and cognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366(1567), 1050–1059. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2013) Neuroscience of technology. In P. J. Richerson & M. Christiansen (Eds.), Cultural Evolution: Society, Technology, Language, and Religion (pp. 157–173). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stout, Dietrich, Apel, Jan, Commander, Julia, & Roberts, Mark
(2014) Late Acheulean technology and cognition at Boxgrove, UK. Journal of Archaeological Science, 41, 576–590. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stout, Dietrich, & Chaminade, Thierry
(2007) The evolutionary neuroscience of tool making. Neuropsychologia, 45, 1091–1100. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2012) Stone tools, language and the brain in human evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1585), 75–87. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stout, Dietrich, & Hecht, Erin E.
(2017) Evolutionary neuroscience of cumulative culture. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(30), 7861–7868. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stout, Dietrich, Hecht, Erin, Khreisheh, Nada, Bradley, Bruce, & Chaminade, Thierry
(2015) Cognitive Demands of Lower Paleolithic Toolmaking. PLoS ONE, 10(4), e0121804. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stout, Dietrich, & Khreisheh, Nada
(2015) Skill Learning and Human Brain Evolution: An Experimental Approach. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 25(04), 867–875. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stout, Dietrich, Passingham, Richard, Frith, Christopher, Apel, Jan, & Chaminade, Thierry
(2011) Technology, expertise and social cognition in human evolution. European Journal of Neuroscience, 33(7), 1328–1338. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stout, Dietrich, Toth, N., Schick, K., & Chaminade, T.
(2008) Neural correlates of Early Stone Age toolmaking: technology, language and cognition in human evolution. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci, 363(1499), 1939–1949. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Strike, Lachlan T., Hansell, Narelle K., Couvy-Duchesne, Baptiste, Thompson, Paul M., de Zubicaray, Greig I., McMahon, Katie L., & Wright, Margaret J.
(2018) Genetic Complexity of Cortical Structure: Differences in Genetic and Environmental Factors Influencing Cortical Surface Area and Thickness. Cerebral Cortex. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Toth, Nicholas, Schick, Kathy D., & Semaw, Sileshi
(2006) A comparative study of the stone tool-making skills of Pan, Australopithecus, and Homo sapiens. In N. Toth & K. D. Schick (Eds.), The Oldowan: case studies into the earliest stone age (pp. 155–222). Gosport, IN: Stone Age Institute Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, Stephen M., Saygin, Ayşe Pinar, Sereno, Martin I., & Iacoboni, Marco
(2004) Listening to speech activates motor areas involved in speech production. Nature neuroscience, 7(7), 701–702. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wolpert, D., Doya, K., & Kawato, M.
(2003) A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 358, 593–602. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Xiao, Yaqiong, Friederici, Angela D., Margulies, Daniel S., & Brauer, Jens
(2016) Development of a selective left-hemispheric fronto-temporal network for processing syntactic complexity in language comprehension. Neuropsychologia, 83, 274–282. CrossrefGoogle Scholar