Part of
Child Bilingualism and Second Language Learning: Multidisciplinary perspectives
Edited by Fangfang Li, Karen E. Pollock and Robbin Gibb
[Bilingual Processing and Acquisition 10] 2020
► pp. 4564
References (38)
References
Bedore, L. M., Peña, E. D., García, M., & Cortez, C. (2005). Clinical forum: Conceptual versus monolingual scoring: When does it make a difference? Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 36, 188–200. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2009). Bilingualism: The good, the bad, and the indifferent. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12, 3–11. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Borodkin, K., Kenett, Y. N., Faust, M., & Mashal, N. (2016). When pumpkin is closer to onion than to squash: The structure of the second language lexicon. Cognition, 156, 60–70. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Core, C., Hoff, E., Rumiche, R., & Señor, M. (2013). Total and conceptual vocabulary in Spanish-English bilinguals from 22 to 30 months: Implications for assessment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56, 1637–1649. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., & Caramazza, A. (1999). Is lexical selection in bilingual speech production language-specific? Further evidence from Spanish-English and English-Spanish bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 2, 231–244. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cronin, V. S. (2002). The syntagmatic-paradigmatic shift and reading development. Journal of Child Language, 29, 189–204. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Curtin, S., Byers-Heinlein, K., & Werker, J. F. (2011). Bilingual beginnings as a lens for theory development: PRIMIR in focus. Journal of Phonetics, 39, 492–504. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
DeAnda, S., Poulin-Dubois, D., Zesiger, P., & Friend, M. (2016). Lexical processing and organization in bilingual first language acquisition: Guiding future research. Psychological Bulletin, 142, 655–667. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Examiner’s manual for the PPVT-III: Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Third Edition. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Dunn, L. M., Thériault, C. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1993). Échelle de vocabulaire en images Peabody. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Faust, M., Dimitrovsky, L. & Davidi, S. (1997). Naming difficulties in language-disabled children: Preliminary findings with the application of the tip-of-the-tongue paradigm. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 40, 1026–1037. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fumagalli, J., Soriano, F., Shalóm, D., Barreyro, J. P., & Martínez-Cuitiño, M. M. (2017). Phonological and semantic verbal fluency tasks in a sample of Argentinean children. Trends in Psychology, 25, 995–1005.Google Scholar
Gathercole, V. C. M., & Hoff, E. (2007). Input and the acquisition of language: Three questions. In E. Hoff & M. Shatz (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of language development (pp. 107–127). Oxford: Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., Fennema-Notestine, C., & Morris, S. K. (2005). Bilingualism affects picture naming but not picture classification. Memory and Cognition, 33, 1220–1234. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T., Montoya, R., & Werner, G. (2002). Semantic and letter fluency in Spanish-English bilinguals. Neuropsychology, 16, 562–576. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goñi, J., Arrondo, G., Sepulcre, J., Martincorena, I., de Mendizábal, N. V., Corominas-Murtra, B., Bejarano, B., Adranza-Trevijano, S., Peraita, H., Wall, D. P. & Villoslada, P. (2011). The semantic organization of the animal category: Evidence from semantic verbal fluency and network theory. Cognitive Processing, 12(2), 183–196.. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jarrold, C., Hartley, S. J., Phillips, C., & Baddeley, A. D. (2000). Word fluency in Williams syn-drome: Evidence for unusual semantic organisation? Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 5, 293–319. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jiang, Z., & Nicoladis, E. (2015). Bilingual children’s concepts are distributed across languages: Evidence from a semantic fluency task (Unpublished manuscript). Department of Psychology, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Canada.Google Scholar
Keith, M. & Nicoladis, E. (2013). The role of within-language vocabulary size in children’s semantic development: Evidence from bilingual children. Journal of Child Language, 40, 873–884. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F., & Curley, J. (1988). Lexical memory in novice bilinguals: The role of concepts in retrieving second language words. In M. Gruneberg, P. Morris, & R. Sykes (Eds.), Practical aspects of memory (Vol. 2, pp. 389–395). London: Wiley.Google Scholar
Kroll, J. F., & de Groot, A. M. B. (2005). Handbook of bilingualism. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Legacy, J., Reider, J., Crivello, C., Kuzyk, O., Friend, M., Zesiger, P., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2017). Dog or chien? Translation equivalents in the receptive and expressive vocabularies of young French-English bilinguals. Journal of Child Language, 44, 881–904. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mancilla-Martinez, J., & Vagh, S. B. (2013). Growth in toddlers’ Spanish, English, and conceptual vocabulary knowledge. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28, 555–567. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. (1977). The syntagmatic-paradigmatic shift revisited: A review of research and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 84, 93–116. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Norrby, C., & Håkansson, G. (2007). Girl – lass or curl? Word associations in second language learners. Australian Review of Applied Linguistics, 30, 22.1–22.17. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pearson, B. Z., & Fernández, S. C. (1994). Patterns of interaction in the lexical growth in two languages of bilingual infants and toddlers. Language Learning, 44, 617–653. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Peña, E. D., Bedore, L. M., & Zlatic-Giunta, R. (2002). Category-generation performance of bilingual children: The influence of condition, category, and language. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 938–947. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Perraudin, S., & Mounoud, P. (2009). Contribution of the priming paradigm to the understanding of the conceptual developmental shift from 5 to 9 years of age. Developmental Science, 12, 956–977. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Poulin-Dubois, D., Bialystok, E., Blaye, A., Polonia, A., & Yott, J. (2013). Lexical access and vocabulary development in very young bilinguals. The International Journal of Bilingualism, 17, 57–70. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roberts, P. M., & Le Dorze, G. (1997). Semantic organization, strategy use, and productivity in bilingual semantic verbal fluency. Brain and Language, 59, 412–449. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rosselli, M., Ardila, A., Araujo, K., Weekes, V. A., Caracciolo, V., Padilla, M., & Ostrosky-Solis, F. (2000). Verbal fluency and repetition skills in healthy older Spanish-English bilinguals. Applied Neuropsychology, 7, 17–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sandoval, T. C., Gollan, T. H., Ferreira, V. S., & Salmon, D. P. (2010). What causes the bilingual disadvantage in verbal fluency? The dual-task analogy. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13, 231–252. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sheng, L., McGregor, K. K., & Marian, V. (2006). Lexical-semantic organization in bilingual children: Evidence from a repeated word association task. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 572–587. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Silva-Corvalán, C., & Treffers-Daller, J. (Eds.) (2016). Operationalising and measuring language dominance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smithson, L., & Nicoladis, E. (2013). Verbal memory resources predict iconic gesture use among bilinguals and monolinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16, 934–944. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smithson, L., Paradis, J., & Nicoladis, E. (2014). Bilingualism and receptive vocabulary achievement: Could sociocultural context make a difference? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17, 810–821. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yan, S., & Nicoladis, E. (2009). Finding le mot juste: Differences between bilingual and monolingual children’s lexical access in comprehension and production. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12, 323–335. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zareva, A. (2007). Structure of the second language mental lexicon: How does it compare to native speakers’ lexical organization? Second Language Research, 23, 123–153. DOI logoGoogle Scholar