Part of
Bilingualism through the Prism of Psycholinguistics: In honour of Albert Costa
Edited by Mikel Santesteban, Jon Andoni Duñabeitia and Cristina Baus
[Bilingual Processing and Acquisition 17] 2023
► pp. 54101
References
Abdel Rahman, R., & Melinger, A.
(2007) When bees hamper the production of honey: Lexical interference from associates in speech production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33 (3), 604–614. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009a) Semantic context effects in language production: A swinging lexical network proposal and a review. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24 (5), 713–734. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009b) Dismissing lexical competition does not make speaking any easier: A rejoinder to Mahon and Caramazza (2009). Language and Cognitive Processes, 24 (5), 749–760. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011) The dynamic microstructure of speech production: Semantic interference built on the fly. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37 (1), 149–161. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2019) Semantic processing during language production: an update of the swinging lexical network. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 34 (9), 1176–1192. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Alario, F. X., & del Prado Martín, F. M.
(2010) On the origin of the “cumulative semantic inhibition” effect. Memory & Cognition, 38 (1), 57–66. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Xavier Alario, F., Segui, J., & Ferrand, L.
(2000) Semantic and associative priming in picture naming. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A, 53 (3), 741–764. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Belke, E.
(2013) Long-lasting inhibitory semantic context effects on object naming are necessarily conceptually mediated: Implications for models of lexical-semantic encoding. Journal of Memory and Language, 69 (3), 228–256. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Belke, E., Brysbaert, M., Meyer, A. S., & Ghyselinck, M.
(2005) Age of acquisition effects in picture naming: Evidence for a lexical-semantic competition hypothesis. Cognition, 96 (2), B45–B54. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Belke, E., Meyer, A. S., & Damian, M. F.
(2005) Refractory effects in picture naming as assessed in a semantic blocking paradigm. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 58 (4), 667–692. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Belke, E., Shao, Z., & Meyer, A. S.
(2017) Strategic origins of early semantic facilitation in the blocked-cyclic naming paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 43 (10), 1659–1668. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Belke, E., & Stielow, A.
(2013) Cumulative and non-cumulative semantic interference in object naming: Evidence from blocked and continuous manipulations of semantic context. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66 (11), 2135–2160. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Biggs, T., & Marmurek, H.
(1990) Picture and word naming: Is facilitation due to processing overlap? American Journal of Psychology, 103 , 81–100. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Blanco-Elorrieta, E., & Caramazza, A.
(2021) A common selection mechanism at each linguistic level in bilingual and monolingual language production. Cognition, 213 , 104625. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bloem, I., & La Heij, W.
(2003) Semantic facilitation and semantic interference in word translation: Implications for models of lexical access in language production. Journal of Memory and Language, 48 (3), 468–488. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bloem, I., van den Boogaard, S., & La Heij, W.
(2004) Semantic facilitation and semantic interference in language production: Further evidence for the conceptual selection model of lexical access. Journal of Memory and Language, 51 (2), 307–323. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brown, A. S.
(1981) Inhibition in cued retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 7 , 204–215. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brysbaert, M., Stevens, M., Mandera, P., & Keuleers, E.
(2016) How many words do we know? Practical estimates of vocabulary size dependent on word definition, the degree of language input and the participant’s age. Frontiers in Psychology, 7 , 1116. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bürki, A., Elbuy, S., Madec, S., & Vasishth, S.
(2020) What did we learn from forty years of research on semantic interference? A Bayesian meta-analysis. Journal of Memory and Language, 114 , 104125. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Caramazza, A.
(1997) How many levels of processing are there in lexical access? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14 (1), 177–208. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Caramazza, A., & Hillis, A. E.
(1990) Where do semantic errors come from? Cortex, 26 , 95–122. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Caramazza, A., & Costa, A.
(2000) The semantic interference effect in the picture-word interference paradigm: does the response set matter? Cognition, 75 (2), B51–B64. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2001) Set size and repetition in the picture–word interference paradigm: Implications for models of naming. Cognition, 80 (3), 291–298. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Caramazza, A., Costa, A., Miozzo, M., & Bi, Y.
(2001) The specific-word frequency effect: Implications for the representation of homophones in speech production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27 (6), 1430–1450. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F.
(1975) A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82 (6), 407. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Costa, A.
(2017) El cerebro bilingüe: La neurociencia del lenguaje. Penguin.Google Scholar
(2021) The bilingual brain: And what it tells us about the science of language (J. W. Schwieter, Trans.). Penguin. (Original work published in 2017)Google Scholar
Costa, A., Alario, F.-X., & Caramazza, A.
(2005) On the categorical nature of the semantic interference effect in the picture-word interference paradigm. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 12 , 125–131. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Caramazza, A., & Sebastian-Galles, N.
(2000) The cognate facilitation effect: implications for models of lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26 (5), 1283–1296. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Mahon, B., Savova, V., & Caramazza, A.
(2003) Level of categorization effect: A novel effect in the picture-word interference paradigm. Language and Cognitive Processes, 18 , 205–233. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Miozzo, M., & Caramazza, A.
(1999) Lexical selection in bilinguals: Do words in the bilingual’s two lexicons compete for selection? Journal of Memory and Language, 41 (3), 365–397. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Pannunzi, M., Deco, G., & Pickering, M. J.
(2017) Do bilinguals automatically activate their native language when they are not using it. Cognitive science, 41 (6), 1629–1644. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Strijkers, K., Martin, C., & Thierry, G.
(2009) The time course of word retrieval revealed by event-related brain potentials during overt speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106 (50), 21442–21446. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Damian, M. F., & Als, L. C.
(2005) Long-lasting semantic context effects in the spoken production of object names. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31 (6), 1372–1384. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Damian, M. F., & Bowers, J. S.
(2003) Locus of semantic interference in picture-word interference tasks. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10 (1), 111–117. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Damian, M. F., Vigliocco, G., & Levelt, W. J.
(2001) Effects of semantic context in the naming of pictures and words. Cognition, 81 (3), B77–B86. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Damian, M. F., & Spalek, K.
(2014) Processing different kinds of semantic relations in picture-word interference with nonmasked and masked distractors. Frontiers in Psychology, 5 , 1183. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dalrymple-Alford, E. C.
(1972) Associative facilitation and interference in the Stroop color-word task. Perception and Psychophysics, 11 (4), 274–276. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dean, M. P., Bub, D. N., & Masson, M. E.
(2001) Interference from related items in object identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27 (3), 733–743. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Le Clec’H, G., Koechlin, E., Mueller, M., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., van de Moortele, P.-F., & Le Bihan, D.
(1998) Imaging unconscious semantic priming. Nature, 395 (6702), Article 6702. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dell, G. S.
(1986) A spreading activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review, 93 , 283–321. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1988) The retrieval of phonological forms in production: Test of predictions from a connectionist model. Journal of Memory and Language, 27 , 124–142. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dell, G. S., Oppenheim, G. M., & Kittredge, A. K.
(2008) Saying the right word at the right time: Syntagmatic and paradigmatic interference in sentence production. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23 (4), 583–608. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dell, G. S., & O’Seaghdha, P. G.
(1992) Stages of lexical access in language production. Cognition, 42 (1), 287–314. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dhooge, E., & Hartsuiker, R. J.
(2010) The distractor frequency effect in picture-word interference: Evidence for response exclusion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36 , 878–891. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011a) How do speakers resist distraction? Evidence from a taboo picture-word interference task. Psychological Science, 22 , 855–859. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011b) The distractor frequency effect in a delayed picture- word interference task: Further evidence for response exclusion. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18 , 116–122. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012) Lexical selection and verbal self-monitoring: Effects of lexicality, context, and time pressure in picture-word interference. Journal of Memory and Cognition, 1 , 163–176. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dylman, A. S., & Barry, C.
(2018) When having two names facilitates lexical selection: Similar results in the picture-word task from translation distractors in bilinguals and synonym distractors in monolinguals. Cognition, 171 , 151–171. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Finkbeiner, M., & Caramazza, A.
(2006) Now you see it, now you don’t: On turning semantic interference into facilitation in a Stroop-like task. Cortex, 42 , 790–796. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fox, L. A., Shor, R. E., & Steinman, R. J.
(1971) Semantic gradients and interference in naming color, spatial direction, and numerosity. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 91 (1), 59–65. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fromkin, V. A.
(1971) The non-anomalous of anomalous utterances. Language, 47 , 27–52. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Garrett, M. F.
(1975) The analysis of sentence production. In G. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 9). Academic Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1980) Levels of processing in sentence production. In B. Butterworth (Ed.), Language production. Vol. 1: Speech and talk. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Goldrick, M., & Rapp, B.
(2002) A restricted interaction account (RIA) of spoken word production: The best of both worlds. Aphasiology, 16 , 20–55. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grice, P.
(1989) Studies in the way of words. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Hantsch, A., Jescheniak, J. D., & Schriefers, H.
(2009) Distractor modality can turn semantic interference into semantic facilitation in the picture–word interference task: Implications for theories of lexical access in speech production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35 (6), 1443–1453. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hantsch, A., & Mädebach, A.
(2013) What does the articulatory output buffer know about alternative picture names? Evidence against the response-exclusion hypothesis. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28 (5), 684–700. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Harley, T. A.
(1984) A critique of top-down independent levels of speech production. Evidence from non-plan-internal speech errors. Cognitive Science, 8 , 191–219. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hartsuiker, R. J., & Kolk, H. H.
(2001) Error monitoring in speech production: A computational test of the perceptual loop theory. Cognitive Psychology, 42 (2), 113–157. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hartsuiker, R. J., Pickering, M. J., & de Jong, N. H.
(2005) Semantic and phonological context effects in speech error repair. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 5 , 921–932. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hirschfeld, G., Jansma, B., Bölte, J., & Zwitserlood, P.
(2008) Interference and facilitation in overt speech production investigated with event-related potentials. Neuroreport, 19 (12), 1227–1230. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Howard, D., Nickels, L., Coltheart, M., & Cole-Virtue, J.
(2006) Cumulative semantic inhibition in picture naming: Experimental and computational studies. Cognition, 100 , 464–482. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Humphreys, G. W., Riddoch, M. J., & Quinlan, P. T.
(1988) Cascade processes in picture identification. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 5 , 67–103. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Humphreys, G. W., Lloyd-Jones, T. J., & Fias, W.
(1995) Semantic interference effects on naming using a postcue procedure: Tapping the links between semantics and phonology with pictures and words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21 (4), 961–980. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., & Kubicek, L. F.
(1983) The source of relatedness effects on naming latency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 9 , 486–496. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ischebeck, A.
(2003) Differences between digit naming and number word reading in a flanker task. Memory and Cognition, 31 , 529–537. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jescheniak, J. D., Matushanskaya, A., Mädebach, A., & Müller, M. M.
(2014) Semantic interference from distractor pictures in single-picture naming: Evidence for competitive lexical selection. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21 (5), 1294–1300. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jescheniak, J. D., Meyer, A., & Levelt, W. J. M.
(2003) Specific-word frequency is not all that counts in speech production. Evidence from the production of homophones in Dutch and German. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29 , 432–438. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Klein, G. S.
(1964) Semantic power measured through the interference of words with color-naming. The American Journal of Psychology, 77 , 576–588. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F., & Stewart, E.
(1994) Category interference in translation and picture naming: Evidence for asymmetric connections between bilingual memory representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 33 (2), 149–174. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuipers, J. R., La Heij, W., & Costa, A.
(2006) A further look at semantic context effects in language production: The role of response congruency. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21 (7–8), 892–919. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuipers, J. R., & La Heij, W.
(2008) Semantic facilitation in category and action naming: Testing the message-congruency account. Journal of Memory and Language, 58 (1), 123–139. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012) Congruency effects in conceptualizing for speech. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65 (11), 2155–2168. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
La Heij, W.
(1988) Components of Stroop-like interference in picture naming. Memory & Cognition, 16 (5), 400–410. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2005) Selection processes in monolingual and bilingual lexical access. In J. F. Kroll & A. M. B. de Groot (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches (pp. 289–307). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
La Heij, W., Heikoop, K. W., Akerboom, S., & Bloem, I.
(2003) Picture naming in picture context: Semantic interference or semantic facilitation? Psychology Science, 45 (1), 49–62.Google Scholar
Levelt, W. J. M.
(1989) Speaking: From intention to articulation. The MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levelt, W. J. M., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S.
(1999) A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22 (1), 1–38. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levelt, W. J. M., Schriefers, H., Vorberg, D., Meyer, A. S., Pechmann, T., & Havinga, J.
(1991) The time course of lexical access in speech production: A study of picture naming. Psychological Review, 98 (1), 122–142. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lupker, S. J.
(1979) The semantic nature of response competition in the picture-word interference task. Memory & Cognition, 7 (6), 485–495. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1982) The role of phonetic and orthographic similarity in picture–word interference. Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie, 36 (3), 349–367. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1988) Picture naming: An investigation of the nature of categorical priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14 , 444–455. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lupker, S. J., & Katz, A. N.
(1981) Input, decision, and response factors in picture–word interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 7 (4), 269–282. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
MacLeod, C. M.
(1991) Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109 (2), 163–203. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mahon, B. Z., & Caramazza, A.
(2009) Why does lexical selection have to be so hard? Comment on Abdel Rahman and Melinger’s swinging lexical network proposal. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24 (5), 735–748. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mahon, B. Z., Costa, A., Peterson, R., Vargas, K. A., & Caramazza, A.
(2007) Lexical selection is not by competition: A reinterpretation of semantic interference and facilitation effects in the picture-word interference paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33 (3), 503–535. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mahon, B. Z., & Navarrete, E.
(2014) The critical difference in models of speech production: A response to Roelofs and Piai. Cortex, 52 , 123–127. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016) Modelling lexical access in speech production as a ballistic process. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31 (4), 521–523. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2019) Adjudicating conflict in speech production – Do we need a central selection mechanism? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 36 (5–6), 220–224. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mahon, B. Z., Garcea, F. E., & Navarrete, E.
(2012) Picture-word interference and the response- exclusion hypothesis: A response to Mulatti and Coltheart. Cortex, 48 (3), 373–377. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Matushanskaya, A., Mädebach, A., Müller, M. M., & Jescheniak, J. D.
(2017) When sufficiently processed, semantically related distractor pictures hamper picture naming. Experimental Psychology, 63 (3), 307–317. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meyer, A. S., & Damian, M. F.
(2007) Activation of distractor names in the picture-picture interference paradigm. Memory & Cognition, 35 (3), 494–503. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miozzo, M., & Caramazza, A.
(2003) When more is less: A counterintuitive effect of distractor frequency in the picture-word interference paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132 , 228–252. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morsella, E., & Miozzo, M.
(2002) Evidence for a cascade model of lexical access in speech production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28 (3), 555–563. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Muehlhaus, J., Heim, S., Sachs, O., Schneider, F., Habel, U., & Sass, K.
(2013) Is the motor or the garage more important to the car? The difference between semantic associations in single word and sentence production. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 42 (1), 37–49. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Navarrete, E., Benavides-Varela, S., Lorusso, R., & Arfè, B.
(2021) Cumulative semantic cost without successful naming. Memory & Cognition, 49 (7), 1348–1359. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Navarrete, E., Caccaro, A., Pavani, F., Mahon, B. Z., & Peressotti, F.
(2015) With or without semantic mediation: Retrieval of lexical representations in sign production. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 20 (2), 163–171. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Navarrete, E., & Costa, A.
(2005) Phonological activation of ignored pictures: Further evidence for a cascade model of lexical access. Journal of Memory and Language, 53 (3), 359–377. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009) The distractor picture paradox in speech production: Evidence from the word translation task. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 38 (6), 527–547. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Navarrete, E., Del Prato, P., & Mahon, B. Z.
(2012) Factors determining semantic facilitation and interference in the cyclic naming paradigm. Frontiers in Psychology, 3 , 38. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Navarrete, E., Del Prato, P., Peressotti, F., & Mahon, B. Z.
(2014) Lexical selection is not by competition: Evidence from the blocked naming paradigm. Journal of Memory and Language, 76 , 253–272. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Navarrete, E., & Mahon, B. Z.
(2013) A rose by any other name is still a rose: A reinterpretation of Hantsch and Mädebach. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28 (5), 701–716. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Navarrete, E., Mahon, B. Z., & Caramazza, A.
(2010) The cumulative semantic cost does not reflect lexical selection by competition. Acta Psychologica, 134 , 279–289. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nozari, N., Dell, G. S., & Schwartz, M. F.
(2011) Is comprehension necessary for error detection? A conflict-based account of monitoring in speech production. Cognitive Psychology, 63 ( 1 ), 1–33. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nozari, N., & Hepner, C. R.
(2019) To select or to wait? The importance of criterion setting in debates of competitive lexical selection. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 36 (5–6), 193–207. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Oppenheim, G.
(2017) Strong competitors facilitate target name retrieval in simple picture naming. Abstract from Architectures and Mechanisms in Language Processing 2017, Lancaster, United Kingdom.Google Scholar
Oppenheim, G. M., Dell, G. S., & Schwartz, M. F.
(2010) The dark side of incremental learning: A model of cumulative semantic interference during lexical access in speech production. Cognition, 114 (2), 227–252. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Peterson, R. R., & Savoy, P.
(1998) Lexical selection and phonological encoding during language production: Evidence for cascaded processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24 (3), 539–557. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Piai, V., Roelofs, A., & Schriefers, H.
(2012) Distractor strength and selective attention in picture-naming performance. Memory & Cognition, 40 (4), 614–627. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rapp, B., & Goldrick, M.
(2000) Discreteness and interactivity in spoken word production. Psychological Review, 107 , 460–499. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Redmann, A., FitzPatrick, I., & Indefrey, P.
(2019) The time course of colour congruency effects in picture naming. Acta Psychologica, 196 , 96–108. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roelofs, A.
(1992) A spreading-activation theory of lemma retrieval in speaking. Cognition, 42 , 107–142. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2003) Goal-referenced selection of verbal action: modeling attentional control in the Stroop task. Psychological Review, 110 (1), 88–125. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2018) A unified computational account of cumulative semantic, semantic blocking, and semantic distractor effects in picture naming. Cognition, 172 , 59–72. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roelofs, A., Meyer, A. S., & Levelt, W. J. M.
(1996) Interaction between semantic and orthographic factors in conceptually driven naming: Comment on Starreveld and La Heij (1995). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22 , 246–251. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roelofs, A., & Piai, V.
(2013) Associative facilitation in the Stroop task: Comment on Mahon et al. (2012). Cortex, 49 (6), 1767–1769. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rose, S. B., Aristei, S., Melinger, A., & Abdel Rahman, R.
(2019) The closer they are, the more they interfere: Semantic similarity of word distractors increases competition in language production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 45 (4), 753–763. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rosinski, R. R., Golinkoff, R. M., & Kukish, K. S.
(1975) Automatic semantic processing in the picture-word interference task. Child Development, 46 , 247–253. [URL]
Runnqvist, E., Strijkers, K., Alario, F. X., & Costa, A.
(2012) Cumulative semantic interference is blind to language: Implications for models of bilingual speech production. Journal of Memory and Language, 66 , 350–869. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sailor, K., Brooks, P. J., Bruening, P. R., Seiger-Gardner, L., & Guterman, M.
(2009) Exploring the time course of semantic interference and associative priming in the picture–word interference task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62 (4), 789–801. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sailor, K., & Brooks, P. J.
(2014) Do part–whole relations produce facilitation in the picture– word interference task? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67 (9), 1768–1785. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Santesteban, M., Costa, A., Pontin, S., & Navarrete, E.
(2006) The effect of word-frequency on lexical selection in speech production: Evidence from semantic homogeneous naming contexts. Cognitiva, 18 (1), 75–84. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Scaltritti, M., Peressotti, F., & Navarrete, E.
(2017) A joint investigation of semantic facilitation and semantic interference in continuous naming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 43 (5), 818–823. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schnur, T. T.
(2014) The persistence of cumulative semantic interference during naming. Journal of Memory and Language, 75 , 27–44. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schriefers, H., Meyer, A. S., & Levelt, W. J. M.
(1990) Exploring the time course of lexical access in language production: Picture-word interference studies. Journal of Memory and Language, 29 , 86–102. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shattuck-Hufnagel, S.
(1987) The role of word onset consonants in speech production planning: New evidence from speech error patterns. In E. Keller & M. Gopnik (Eds.), Sensory processes in language. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Simon, J. R., & Sudalaimuthu, P.
(1979) Effects of S-R mapping and response modality on performance in a Stroop Task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5 , 176–187. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sperber, R. D., McCauley, C., Ragain, R. D., & Weil, C. M.
(1979) Semantic priming effects on picture and word processing. Memory & Cognition, 7 , 339–345. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Starreveld, P. A., & La Heij, W.
(1995) Semantic interference, orthographic facilitation and their interaction in naming tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21 , 686–698. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1996) Time-course analysis of semantic and orthographic context effects in picture naming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22 , 896–918. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stemberger, J. P.
(1985) An interactive activation model of language production. In A. W. Ellis (Ed.), Progress in the psychology of language (Vol. 1). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
(1990) Wordshape errors in language production. Cognition, 35 , 123–158. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, S.
(1969) The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders’ method. Attention and performance II, W. G. Koster (Ed.). Acta Psychologica, 30 , 276–315. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stroop, J. R.
(1935) Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18 (6), 643–662. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vieth, H. E., McMahon, K. L., & de Zubicaray, G. I.
(2014) Feature overlap slows lexical selection: Evidence from the picture–word interference paradigm. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67 (12), 2325–2339. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vigliocco, G., Vinson, D. P., Lewis, W., & Garrett, M. F.
(2004) Representing the meanings of object and action words: The featural and unitary semantic space hypothesis. Cognitive Psychology, 48 , 422–488. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vitkovitch, M.
(1996) Patterns of excitation and inhibition in picture naming. Visual Cognition, 3 (1), 61–80. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vitkovitch, M., & Tyrrell, L.
(1999) The effects of distractor words on naming pictures at the subordinate level. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 52 (4), 905–926. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vitkovitch, M., & Rutter, C.
(2000) The effects of response stimuli interval on error priming in sequential object naming. Visual Cognition, 7 (5), 645–670. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vitkovitch, M., Rutter, C., & Read, A.
(2001) Inhibitory effects during object name retrieval: The effect of interval between prime and target on picture naming responses. British Journal of Psychology, 92 (3), 483–506. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wheeldon, L. R., & Monsell, S.
(1994) Inhibition of spoken word production by priming a semantic competitor. Journal of Memory and Language, 33 (3), 332–356. DOI logoGoogle Scholar