Part of
Bilingualism through the Prism of Psycholinguistics: In honour of Albert Costa
Edited by Mikel Santesteban, Jon Andoni Duñabeitia and Cristina Baus
[Bilingual Processing and Acquisition 17] 2023
► pp. 181204
References (116)
References
Alemán Bañón, J., & Martin, C. (2019). Anticipating information structure: An event-related potentials study of focus assignment via the it-cleft. Neuropsychologia, 134, 107203. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2021). The role of crosslinguistic differences in second language anticipatory processing: An event-related potentials study. Neuropsychologia, 155, 107797. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Altmann, G. T., & Kamide, Y. (1999). Incremental interpretation at verbs: Restricting the domain of subsequent reference. Cognition, 73(3), 247–264. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Caffarra, S., & Martin, C. D. (2019). Not all errors are the same: ERP sensitivity to error typicality in foreign accented speech perception. Cortex, 116, 308–320. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Caffarra, S., Motamed Haeri, A., Michell, E., & Martin, C. D. (2019). When is irony influenced by communicative constraints? ERP evidence supporting interactive models. Eur J Neurosci, 50(10), 3566–3577. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carter, B. T., Foster, B., Muncy, N. M., & Luke, S. G. (2019). Linguistic networks associated with lexical, semantic and syntactic predictability in reading: A fixation-related fMRI study. Neuroimage, 189, 224–240. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chun, E. & Kaan, E. (2019). L2 Prediction during complex sentence processing. Journal of Cultural Cognitive Science, 3, 203–216. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Contemori, C., & Dussias, P. E. (2019). Prediction at the discourse level in Spanish-English bilinguals: An eye-tracking study. Frontiers in Psychology, 10. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Corey, J. D., Hayakawa, S., Aparici, M., Vives, M. L., & Keysar, B. (2019). The role of intentions and outcomes in the foreign language effect on moral judgements. Quaterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72(1), 8–17. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Foucart, A., Arnon, I., Aparici, M., & Apesteguia, J. (2014). “Piensa” twice: On the foreign language effect in decision making. Cognition, 130(2), 236–254. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cowles, H. W., Kluender, R., Kutas, M., & Polinsky, M. (2007). Violations of information structure: An electrophysiological study of answers to wh-questions. Brain and Language, 102(3), 228–242. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
DeLong, K. A., Chan, W. H., & Kutas, M. (2019). Similar time courses for word form and meaning preactivation during sentence comprehension. Psychophysiology, 56(4), e13312. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2021). Testing limits: ERP evidence for word form preactivation during speeded sentence reading. Psychophysiology, 58(2), e13720. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
DeLong, K. A., Urbach, T. P., Groppe, D. M., & Kutas, M. (2011). Overlapping dual ERP responses to low cloze probability sentence continuations. Psychophysiology, 48(9), 1203–1207. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
DeLong, K. A., Urbach, T. P., & Kutas, M. (2005). Probabilistic word pre-activation during language comprehension inferred from electrical brain activity. Nature Neuroscience, 8(8), 1117–1121. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017). Is there a replication crisis? Perhaps. Is this an example? No: A commentary on Ito, Martin, and Nieuwland (2016). Language Cognition and Neuroscience, 32(8), 966–973. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dijkgraaf, A., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Duyck, W. (2017). Predicting upcoming information in native-language and non-native-language auditory word recognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20(5), 917–930. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2019). Prediction and integration of semantics during L2 and L1 listening. Language Cognition and Neuroscience, 34(7), 881–900. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dussias, P. E., Kroff, J. R. V., Tamargo, R. E. G., & Gerfen, C. (2013). When gender and looking go hand in hand. Grammatical gender processing in L2 Spanish. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35(2), 353–387. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Federmeier, K. D. (2007). Thinking ahead: The role and roots of prediction in language comprehension. Psychophysiology, 44(4), 491–505. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Federmeier, K. D., & Kutas, M. (1999). A rose by any other name: Long-term memory structure and sentence processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 41(4), 469–495. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Federmeier, K. D., Kutas, M., & Schul, R. (2010). Age-related and individual differences in the use of prediction during language comprehension. Brain and Language, 115(3), 149–161. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of mind: An essay on faculty psychology. The MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Forster, K. I. (1981). Priming and the effects of sentence and lexical contexts on naming time – Evidence for autonomous lexical processing. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section a-Human Experimental Psychology, 33(Nov), 465–495. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Foucart, A. (2021). Language prediction in L2: Does language similarity matter? In E. Kaan & T. Grüter (Eds.), Prediction in second language processing and learning (pp. 91–114). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Foucart, A., Costa, A., Moris-Fernandez, L., & Hartsuiker, R. J. (2020). Foreignness or processing fluency? On understanding the negative bias toward foreign-accented speakers. Language Learning, 70(4), 974–1016. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Foucart, A., Garcia, X., Ayguasanosa, M., Thierry, G., Martin, C., & Costa, A. (2015). Does the speaker matter? Online processing of semantic and pragmatic information in L2 speech comprehension. Neuropsychologia, 75, 291–303. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Foucart, A., Martin, C. D., Moreno, E. M., & Costa, A. (2014). Can bilinguals see it coming? Word anticipation in L2 sentence reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 40(5), 1461–1469. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Foucart, A., Ruiz-Tada, E., & Costa, A. (2016). Anticipation processes in L2 speech comprehension: Evidence from ERPs and lexical recognition task. Bilingualism-Language and Cognition, 19(1), 213–219. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Foucart, A., Santamaria-Garcia, H., & Hartsuiker, R. J. (2019). Short exposure to a foreign accent impacts subsequent cognitive processes. Neuropsychologia, 129, 1–9. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Garrod, S., & Pickering, M. J. (2004). Why is conversation so easy? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(1), 8–11. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goslin, J., Duffy, H., & Floccia, C. (2012). An ERP investigation of regional and foreign accent processing. Brain and Language, 122(2), 92–102. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grey, S., Schubel, L. C., Mcqueen, J. M., & van Hell, J. G. (2019). Processing foreign-accented speech in a second language: Evidence from ERPs during sentence comprehension in bilinguals. Bilingualism-Language and Cognition, 22(5), 912–929. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grey, S., & van Hell, J. G. (2017). Foreign-accented speaker identity affects neural correlates of language comprehension. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 42, 93–108. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gruter, T. (2021). Predictive processing of implicit causality in a second language: A visual-world eye-tracking study. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 43(1), 133 – 154. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gruter, T., Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A. (2012). Grammatical gender in L2: A production or a real-time processing problem? Second Language Research, 28(2), 191–215. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gruter, T., & Rohde, H. (2021). Limits on expectation-based processing: Use of grammatical aspect for co-reference in L2. Applied Psycholinguistics, 42(1), 51–75. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gruter, T., Rohde, H., & Schafer, A. J. (2017). Coreference and discourse coherence in L2. The roles of grammatical aspect and referential form. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 7(2), 199–229. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grüter, T., Rohde, H., & Schafer, A. J. (2014). The role of discourse-level expectations in non-native speaker´s referential choices. In W. Orman & M. J. Valleau (Eds.), Proceedings of the 38th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 179–191). Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Hahne, A. (2001). What’s different in second-language processing? Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30(3), 251–266. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hanulikova, A., van Alphen, P. M., van Goch, M. M., & Weber, A. (2012). When one person’s mistake is another’s standard usage: The effect of foreign accent on syntactic processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(4), 878–887. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hayakawa, S., Costa, A., Foucart, A., & Keysar, B. (2016). Using a foreign language changes our choices. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(11), 791–793. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hayakawa, S., Tannenbaum, D., Costa, A., Corey, J. D., & Keysar, B. (2017). Thinking more or feeling less? Explaining the foreign-language effect on moral judgment. Psychological Science, 28(10), 1387–1397. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hopp, H. (2013). Grammatical gender in adult L2 acquisition: Relations between lexical and syntactic variability. Second Language Research, 29(1), 33–56. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015). Semantics and morphosyntax in predictive L2 sentence processing. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 53(3), 277–306. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016). Learning (not) to predict: Grammatical gender processing in second language acquisition. Second Language Research, 32(2), 277–307. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hopp, H., & Lemmerth, N. (2018). Lexical and syntactic congruency in L2 predictive gender processing. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 40(1), 171–199. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huettig, F. (2015). Four central questions about prediction in language processing. Brain Research, 1626, 118–135. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huettig, F., & Mani, N. (2016). Is prediction necessary to understand language? Probably not. Language Cognition and Neuroscience, 31(1), 19–31. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ito, A., Corley, M., & Pickering, M. J. (2018). A cognitive load delays predictive eye movements similarly during L1 and L2 comprehension. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21(2), 251–264. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ito, A., Corley, M., Pickering, M. J., Martin, A. E., & Nieuwland, M. S. (2016). Predicting form and meaning: Evidence from brain potentials. Journal of Memory and Language, 86, 157–171. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ito, A., Martin, A. E., & Nieuwland, M. S. (2017a). How robust are prediction effects in language comprehension? Failure to replicate article-elicited N400 effects. Language Cognition and Neuroscience, 32(8), 954–965. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017b). On predicting form and meaning in a second language. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 43(4), 635–652. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017c). Why the A/AN prediction effect may be hard to replicate: A rebuttal to Delong, Urbach, and Kutas (2017). Language Cognition and Neuroscience, 32(8), 974–983. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ito, A., & Pickering, M. J. (2021). Automaticity and prediction in non-native language comprehension. In T. Gruter & E. Kaan (Eds.), Prediction in second-language processing and learning (pp. 25–46). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ito, A., & Sakai, H. (2021). Everyday language exposure shapes prediction of specific words in listening comprehension: A visual world eye-tracking study. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 607474. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ivaz, L., Costa, A., & Dunabeitia, J. A. (2016). The emotional impact of being myself: Emotions and foreign-language processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 42(3), 489–496. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jiang, X. M., Gossack-Keenan, K., & Pell, M. D. (2020). To believe or not to believe? How voice and accent information in speech alter listener impressions of trust. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 73(1), 55–79. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jonczyk, R., Boutonnet, B., Musial, K., Hoemann, K., & Thierry, G. (2016). The bilingual brain turns a blind eye to negative statements in the second language. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 16(3), 527–540. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jonczyk, R., Korolczuk, I., Balatsou, E., & Thierry, G. (2019). Keep calm and carry on: Electrophysiological evaluation of emotional anticipation in the second language. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 14(8), 885–898. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2020). Erratum to: Keep calm and carry on: Electrophysiological evaluation of emotional anticipation in the second language. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Juffs, A., & Harrington, M. (2011). Aspects of working memory in L2 learning. Language Teaching, 44, 137–166. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaan, E. (2014). Predictive sentence processing in L2 and L1. What is different? Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 4(2), 257–282. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaan, E., & Grüter, T. (2021). Prediction in second language processing and learning. John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kheirzadeh, S., & Hajiabed, M. (2016). Differential language functioning of monolinguals and bilinguals on positive-negative emotional expression. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 45(1), 55–69. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kotz, S. A. (2009). A critical review of ERP and fMRI evidence on L2 syntactic processing. Brain and Language, 109(2–3), 68–74. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuperberg, G. R., & Jaeger, T. F. (2016). What do we mean by prediction in language comprehension? Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31(1), 32–59. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuperberg, G. R., Paczynski, M., & Ditman, T. (2011). Establishing causal coherence across sentences: An ERP study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(5), 1230–1246. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2011). Thirty years and counting: Finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 621–647. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1980a). Event-related brain potentials to semantically inappropriate and surprisingly large words. Biological Psychology, 11(2), 99–116. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1980b). Reading between the lines: Event-related brain potentials during natural sentence processing. Brain and Language, 11(2), 354–373. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1980c). Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207(4427), 203–205. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lau, E. F., Holcomb, P. J., & Kuperberg, G. R. (2013). Dissociating N400 effects of prediction from association in single-word contexts. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25(3), 484–502. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lau, E. F., Weber, K., Gramfort, A., Hamalainen, M. S., & Kuperberg, G. R. (2016). Spatiotemporal signatures of lexical-semantic prediction. Cereb Cortex, 26(4), 1377–1387. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leon-Cabrera, P., Flores, A., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., & Moris, J. (2019). Ahead of time: Early sentence slow cortical modulations associated to semantic prediction. Neuroimage, 189, 192–201. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leon-Cabrera, P., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., & Moris, J. (2017). Electrophysiological correlates of semantic anticipation during speech comprehension. Neuropsychologia, 99, 326–334. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A. (2010). Real-time processing of gender-marked articles by native and non-native Spanish speakers. Journal of Memory and Language, 63(4), 447–464. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Libben, M. R., & Titone, D. A. (2009). Bilingual lexical access in context: Evidence from eye movements during reading. Jornal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(2), 381–390. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Luck, Steven J. (2014). An introduction to the event-related potential technique (2nd ed.). The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Luke, S. G., & Christianson, K. (2016). Limits on lexical prediction during reading. Cognitive Psychology, 88, 22–60. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mantegna, F., Hintz, F., Ostarek, M., Alday, P. M., & Huettig, F. (2019). Distinguishing integration and prediction accounts of ERP N400 modulations in language processing through experimental design. Neuropsychologia, 134, 107199. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1987). Functional parallelism in spoken word-recognition. Cognition, 25(1–2), 71–102. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Martin, C. D., Thierry, G., Kuipers, J. R., Boutonnet, B., Foucart, A., & Costa, A. (2013). Bilinguals reading in their second language do not predict upcoming words as native readers do. Journal of Memory and Language, 69(4), 574–588. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McClelland, J. L., & Elman, J. L. (1986). The TRACE model of speech perception. Cognitive Psychology, 18(1), 1–86. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mcclelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: 1. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review, 88(5), 375–407. [URL]. DOI logo
Mitsugi, S., & Macwhinney, B. (2016). The use of case marking for predictive processing in second language Japanese. Bilingualism-Language and Cognition, 19(1), 19–35. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Molinaro, N., Giannelli, F., Caffarra, S., & Martin, C. (2017). Hierarchical levels of representation in language prediction: The influence of first language acquisition in highly proficient bilinguals. Cognition, 164, 61–73. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Moreno, E. M., & Kutas, M. (2005). Processing semantic anomalies in two languages: An electrophysiological exploration in both languages of Spanish-English bilinguals. Cognitive Brain Research, 22(2), 205–220. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Moreno, E. M., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., & Laine, M. (2008). Event-related potentials (ERPs) in the study of bilingual language processing. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 21(6), 477–508. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Neely, J. H. (1977). Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limited-capacity attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 106(3), 226–254. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nicenboim, B., Vasishth, S., & Rosler, F. (2020). Are words pre-activated probabilistically during sentence comprehension? Evidence from new data and a Bayesian random-effects meta-analysis using publicly available data. Neuropsychologia, 142, 107427. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nieuwland, M. S., Arkhipova, Y., & Rodriguez-Gomez, P. (2020). Anticipating words during spoken discourse comprehension: A large-scale, pre-registered replication study using brain potentials. Cortex, 133, 1–36. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nieuwland, M. S., Barr, D. J., Bartolozzi, F., Busch-Moreno, S., Darley, E., Donaldson, D. I., … Von Grebmer Zu Wolfsthurn, S. (2020). Dissociable effects of prediction and integration during language comprehension: evidence from a large-scale study using brain potentials. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences, 375(1791), 20180522. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nieuwland, M. S., & Kazanina, N. (2020). The neural basis of linguistic prediction: Introduction to the special issue. Neuropsychologia, 146, 107532. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nieuwland, M. S., Politzer-Ahles, S., Heyselaar, E., Segaert, K., Darley, E., Kazanina, N., … Huettig, F. (2018). Large-scale replication study reveals a limit on probabilistic prediction in language comprehension. Elife, 7. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pashler, H., & Wagenmakers, E. J. (2012). Editors’ introduction to the special section on replicability in psychological science: A crisis of confidence? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(6), 528–530. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Romero-Rivas, C., Martin, C. D., & Costa, A. (2015). Processing changes when listening to foreign-accented speech. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016). Foreign-accented speech modulates linguistic anticipatory processes. Neuropsychologia, 85, 245–255. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ryskin, R., Levy, R. P., & Fedorenko, E. (2020). Do domain-general executive resources play a role in linguistic prediction? Re-evaluation of the evidence and a path forward. Neuropsychologia, 136, 107258. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schiller, N. O., Boutonnet, B. P., De Heer Kloots, M. L. S., Meelen, M., Ruijgrok, B., & Cheng, L. L. (2020). (Not so) great expectations: Listening to foreign-accented speech reduces the brain’s anticipatory processes. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 2143. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Song, J., & Iverson, P. (2018). Listening effort during speech perception enhances auditory and lexical processing for non-native listeners and accents. Cognition, 179, 163–170. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sulpizio, S., Toti, M., Del Maschio, N., Costa, A., Fedeli, D., Job, R., & Abutalebi, J. (2019). Are you really cursing? Neural processing of taboo words in native and foreign language. Brain and Language, 194, 84–92. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Traxler, M. J., Foss, D. J., Seely, R. E., Kaup, B., & Morris, R. K. (2000). Priming in sentence processing: Intralexical spreading activation, schemas, and situation models. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29(6), 581–595. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Urbach, T. P., DeLong, K. A., Chan, W. H., & Kutas, M. (2020). An exploratory data analysis of word form prediction during word-by-word reading. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 117(34), 20483–20494. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Assche, E., Drieghe, D., Duyck, W., Welvaert, M., & Hartsuiker, R. J. (2011). The influence of semantic constraints on bilingual word recognition during sentence reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 64(1), 88–107. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Bergen, G., & Flecken, M. (2017). Putting things in new places: Linguistic experience modulates the predictive power of placement verb semantics. Journal of Memory and Language, 92, 26–42. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Berkum, J. J., Brown, C. M., Zwitserlood, P., Kooijman, V., & Hagoort, P. (2005). Anticipating upcoming words in discourse: evidence from ERPs and reading times. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(3), 443–467. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Hell, J. G., & de Groot, A. M. B. (2008). Sentence context modulates visual word recognition and translation in bilinguals. Acta Psychologica, 128(3), 431–451. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Petten, C., & Luka, B. J. (2012). Prediction during language comprehension: Benefits, costs, and ERP components. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 83(2), 176–190. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Weber-Fox, C. M., & Neville, H. J. (1996). Maturational constraints on functional specializations for language processing: ERP and behavioral evidence in bilingual speakers. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8(3), 231–256. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Weber, K., Lau, E. F., Stillerman, B., & Kuperberg, G. R. (2016). The yin and the yang of prediction: An fMRI study of semantic predictive processing. PLoS One, 11(3), e0148637. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wicha, N. Y. Y., Bates, E. A., Moreno, E. M., & Kutas, M. (2003). Potato not Pope: Human brain potentials to gender expectation and agreement in Spanish spoken sentences. Neuroscience Letters, 346(3), 165–168. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wicha, N. Y. Y., Moreno, E. M., & Kutas, M. (2003). Expecting gender: An event related brain potential study on the role of grammatical gender in comprehending a line drawing within a written sentence in Spanish. Cortex, 39(3), 483–508. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2004). Anticipating words and their gender: An event-related brain potential study of semantic integration, gender expectancy, and gender agreement in Spanish sentence reading. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(7), 1272–1288. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wlotko, E. W., & Federmeier, K. D. (2015). Time for prediction? The effect of presentation rate on predictive sentence comprehension during word-by-word reading. Cortex, 68, 20–32. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zirnstein, M., van Hell, J. G., & Kroll, J. F. (2018). Cognitive control ability mediates prediction costs in monolinguals and bilinguals. Cognition, 176, 87–106. DOI logoGoogle Scholar