Article published in:
Cognitive Individual Differences in Second Language Processing and Acquisition
Edited by Gisela Granena, Daniel O. Jackson and Yucel Yilmaz
[Bilingual Processing and Acquisition 3] 2016
► pp. 6988
References

References

Abutalebi, J., & Green, D.
(2008) Control mechanisms in bilingual language production: Neural evidence from language switching studies. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23, 557–582. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ackerman, P.L., Beier, M.E., & Boyle, M.O.
(2005) Working memory and intelligence: The same or difference constructs?Psychological Bulletin, 131, 30–60. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M.
(1971) The control processes of short-term memory. Stanford, CA: Institute for Mathematical Studies in the Social Sciences, Stanford University.Google Scholar
Au, J., Sheehan, E., Tsai, N., Duncan, G. J., Buschkuehl, M., & Jaeggi, S. M.
(2015) Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory: a meta-analysis. Psychonomic bulletin & review, 22(2), 366–377. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. D.
(1986) Working memory. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A., Papagno, C., & Vallar, G.
(1988) When long-term learning depends on short-term storage. Journal of memory and language, 27(5), 586–595. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A.D.
(2000) The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 417–423. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. D.
(2003) Working memory and language: An overview. Journal of communication disorders, 36, 189–208. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J.
(1974) Working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 8; pp. 47–89). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Binet, A., & Simon, T.
(1905) New methods for the diagnosis of the intellectual level of subnormals. L'annee Psychologique, 12, 191–244.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A.D. & Wilson, B.A.
(1985) Phonological coding and short-term memory in patients without speech. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 490–502. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2002) Prose recall and amnesia: Implications for the structure of working memory. Neuropsychologia, 40, 1737–1743. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. D., Eldridge, M., & Lewis, V. J.
(1981) The role of subvocalization in reading. Quarterly journal of experimental psychology, 33, 439–454. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A., Gathercole, S., & Papagno, C.
(1998) The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review, 105, 158–173. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A.D., Thomson, N., & Buchanan, M.
(1975) Word length and structure of short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 575–589. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, L. F., Tugade, M. M., & Engle, R. W.
(2004) Individual differences in working memory capacity and dual-process theories of the mind. Psychological bulletin, 130 (4), 553. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Brener, R.
(1940) An experimental investigation of memory span. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 26, 467–482. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Buschkuehl, M., Jaeggi, S. M., & Jonides, J.
(2012) Neuronal effects following working memory training. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2, S167–S179. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cain, K., Oakhill, J., & Bryant, P.
(2004) Children’s reading comprehension ability: Concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal ability, and component skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 31–42. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Caplan, D., & Waters, G. S.
(1999) Verbal working memory and sentence comprehension. Behavioral and brain Sciences, 22(01), 77–94. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Carretti, B., Borella, E., Zavagnin, M., & Beni, R.
(2012) Gains in language comprehension relating to working memory training in healthy older adults. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 28 (5), 539–546. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Carretti, B., Caldarola, N., Tencati, C., & Cornoldi, C.
(2014) Improving reading comprehension in reading and listening settings: The effect of two training programmes focusing on metacognition and working memory. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 84 (2), 194–210. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cheung, H.
(1996) Nonword span as a unique predictor of second-language vocabulary language. Developmental Psychology, 32(5), 867. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chein, J.M., & Morrison, A.B.
(2010) Expanding the mind’s workspace: Training and transfer effects with a complex working memory span task. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12 (2), 193–199. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Churchill Jr, E. F.
(1999) The role of working memory in SLA. Temple University Japan: Working Papers in Applied Linguistics, 14.Google Scholar
Costa, A., & Santesteban, M.
(2004) Lexical access in bilingual speech production: Evidence from language switching in highly proficient bilinguals and L2 learners. Journal of memory and Language, 50(4), 491–511. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cowan, N.
(1988) Evolving conceptions of memory storage, selective attention, and their mutual constraints within the human information processing system. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 163–191. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dahnlin, E., Neely, A., Larsson, A., Backman, L., & Nyberg, L.
(2008) Transfer of learning after updating training mediated by the striatum. Science, 320, 1510–1512. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Daneman, M.
(1991) Individual differences in reading skills. In R. Barr, M.L. Kamil, P. Mosenthal, & P.D. Pearson (Eds.), Handbook of Reading Research (Vol. 2; pp. 512–538). White Plains, NY: Longman.Google Scholar
Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A.
(1980) Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19 (4), 450–466. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Daneman, M., & Green, I.
(1986) Individual differences in comprehending and producing words in context. Journal of Memory and Language, 25, 1–18. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Daneman, M., & Merikle, P.M.
(1996) Working memory and language comprehension: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3, 422–433. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Deng, Y., Booth, J. R., Chou, T. L., Ding, G. S., & Peng, D. L.
(2008) Item-specific and generalization effects on brain activation when learning Chinese characters. Neuropsychologia, 46, 1864–1876 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C., & Beaton, A.
(1993) Psycholinguistic determinants of foreign language vocabulary learning. Language learning, 43(4), 559–617. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Engle, R. W.
(2002) Working memory capacity as executive attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 19–23 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Engle, R.W., Kane, M.J., & Tuholski, S.
(1999) Individual differences in working memory capacity and what they tell us about controlled attenion, general fluid intelligence, and functions of the prefrontal cortex. In A. Miyake & P. Shah (Ed.), Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control (pp. 28–61). New York City, NY: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
García‐Madruga, J. A., Elosúa, M. R., Gil, L., Gómez‐Veiga, I., Vila, J. Ó., Orjales, I., & Duque, G.
(2013) Reading comprehension and working memory’s executive processes: An intervention study in primary school students. Reading Research Quarterly, 48 (2), 155–174. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D.
(1993) Phonological working memory: A critical building block for reading development and vocabulary acquisition?. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 8(3), 259–272. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S.E., & Baddeley, A.D.
(1989) Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 200–213. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1990) Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a casual connection? Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 336–360. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S.E., Hitch, G.J., Service, E., & Martin, A.J.
(1997) Phonological short-term memory and new word learning in children. Developmental Psychology, 33, 966–979. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S.E., Willis, C., Emslie, H., & Baddeley, A.D.
(1992) Phonological memory and vocabulary development during the early school years: A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 28, 887–898. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C. S., Baddeley, A. D., & Emslie, H.
(1994) The children's test of nonword repetition: A test of phonological working memory. Memory, 2(2), 103–127. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Green, D.W.
(1998) Mental control of the bilingual lexicosemantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, 67–81. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hannon, B., & Daneman, M.
(2001) A new tool for measuring and understanding individual differences in the component processes of reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 103–128. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hofmann, W., Gschwendner, T., Friese, M., Wiers, R. W., & Schmitt, M.
(2008) Working memory capacity and self-regulatory behavior: Toward an individual differences perspective on behavior determination by automatic versus controlled processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95 (4), 962. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jacobs, J.
(1887) Experiments on “prehension.” Mind, 12, 75–79. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
James, W.
(1890) The Principles of. Psychology, 2. New York: Henry Holt.Google Scholar
Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Shah, P., & Jonides, J.
(2014) The role of individual differences in cognitive training and transfer. Memory & Cognition, 42 (3), 464–480. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jonides, J.
(2004) How does practice make perfect? Nature Neuroscience, 7, 10–11. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jonides, J. & Smith, E.
(1997) Architecture of Working Memory. In M.D. Rugg (Ed.), Cognitive Neuroscience, (pp. 243–276). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Karbach, J., & Verhaeghen, P.
(2014) Making working memory work a meta-analysis of executive-control and working memory training in older adults. Psychological science, 25(11), 2027–2037. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Karbach, J., Strobach, T., & Schubert, T.
(2014) Adaptive working-memory training benefits reading, but not mathematics in middle childhood. Child Neuropsychology, (ahead-of-print), 1–17.Google Scholar
Kohonen, V.
(1995) Is the relation between phonological memory and foreign language learning accounted for by vocabulary acquisition?. Applied Psycholinguistics, 16(02), 155–172. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Linck, J.A., Osthus, P., Koeth, J.T., Bunting, M.F.
(2014) Working memory and second language comprehension and production: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin Review, 21, 861–883. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Liu, Y., Dunlap, S., Fiez, J., & Perfetti, C.
(2007) Evidence for neural accommodation to a writing system following learning. Human Brain Mapping, 28 (11), 1223–1234. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Logie, R.H.
(1996) The seven ages of working memory. In J.T.E. Richardson, R.W. Engle, L. Hasher, R.H. Logie, E.R. Stoltzfus, & R.T. Zacks (Eds.), Working memory and human cognition (pp. 31–65). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Loosli, S. V., Buschkuehl, M., Perrig, W. J., & Jaeggi, S. M.
(2012) Working memory training improves reading processes in typically developing children. Child Neuropsychology, 18 (1), 62–78. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
MacDonald, M.C.
(1997) Language and cognitive Processes. Special Issue on Lexical Representations and Sentence Processing, 12, 121–399. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Masoura, E. V., & Gathercole, S. E.
(1999) Phonological short‐term memory and foreign language learning. International Journal of Psychology, 34 (5–6), 383–388. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Melby-Lervag, M., & Hulme, C.
(2013) Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Developmental Psychology, 49 (2), 270–291. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Miller, G. A.
(1956) The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 81–97. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Miyake, A., & Friedman, N.P.
(2012) The nature of organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 8–14 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Miyake, A., & Shah, P.
(1999) Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, J.R., Liu, Y., Fiez, J.A., & Perfetti, C.A.
(2009) Assimilation and accommodation patterns in ventral occipitotemporal cortex in learning a second writing system. Human Brain Mapping, 30 (3), 810–820. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nikolaidis, A., Voss, M. W., Lee, H., Vo, L. T., & Kramer, A. F.
(2014) Parietal plasticity after training with a complex video game is associated with individual differences in improvements in an untrained working memory task. Frontiers in human neuroscience, 8, 169. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Norman, D., & Shallice, T.
(1986) Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behavior. In R. Davidson, R.G. Schwartz, & D. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and self-regulation: Advances in research and theory (pp. 1–18). New York, NY: Plenum Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Novick, J. M., Hussey, E., Teubner-Rhodes, S., Harbison, J. I., & Bunting, M. F.
(2014) Clearing the garden-path: Improving sentence processing through cognitive control training. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29 (2), 186–217. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Oberauer, K.
(2002) Access to information in working memory: Exploring the focus of attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28 (3), 411.Google Scholar
Opitz, B., Schneiders, J.A., Krick, C.M., & Mecklinger, A.
(2014) Selective transfer of visual working memory training on Chinese character learning. Neuropsychologia, 53, 1–11. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Page, M., & Norris, D.
(1998) The primacy model: a new model of immediate serial recall. Psychological review, 105(4), 761. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Page, M. P. A., & Norris, D.
(2009) A model linking immediate serial recall, the Hebb repetition effect and the learning of phonological word forms. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 364(1536), 3737–3753. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Papagno, C., & Vallar, G.
(1992) Phonological short-term memory and the learning of novel words: The effect of phonological similarity and item length. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 44A, 47–67. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Papagno, C., Valentine, T., & Baddeley, A.D.
(1991) Phonological short-term memory and foreign-language vocabulary learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 331–347. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pressley, M.
(2000) What should comprehension instruction be the instruction of? In M.L. Kamil, P.B. Mosenthal, P.D. Pearson, & R. Barr (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. 3; pp. 545–561). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Schwaighofer, M., Fischer, F., & Bühner, M.
(2015) Does working memory training transfer? A meta-analysis including training conditions as moderators. Educational Psychologist, 50 (2), 138–166. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Serafini, E.J., & Sanz, C.
(2015) Evidence for the decreasing impact of cognitive ability on second language development as proficiency increases. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, (first view), 1–40. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Service, E.
(1992) Phonology, working memory, and foreign-language learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45A, 21–50. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Skehan, P.
(1998) A cognitive approach to language learning. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Szmalec, A., Verbruggen, F., Vandierendonck, A., & Kemps, E.
(2011) Control of interference during working memory updating. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 137–151.Google Scholar
Tan, L.H., Laird, A.R., Li, K., & Fox, P.T.
(2005) Neuroanatomical correlates of phonological processing of Chinese characters and alphabetic words: A meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 25, 83–91. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Watanabe, Y., & Bergsleithner, J. M.
(2006) A systematic research synthesis of L2 WM measurements. In Z. Madden-Wood & K. Oeki (Eds.), Proceedings 2006: Selected papers from the tenth college-wide conference for students in languages, linguistics, and literature (pp. 47–60). Manoa, HI: University of Hawaii, Manoa, College of Languages, Linguistics, and Literature.Google Scholar
Wass, S.V., Scerif, G., & Johnson, M.H.
(2012) Training attentional control and working memory — Is younger, better? Developmental Review, 32 (4), 360–387. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Waters, G. S., Caplan, D., & Rochon, E.
(1995) Processing capacity and sentence comprehension in patients with Alzheimer's disease. Cognitive neuropsychology, 12(1), 1–30. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Weicker, J., Villringer, A., & Thöne-Otto, A.
(2016) Can impaired working memory functioning be improved by training? A meta-analysis with a special focus on brain injured patients. Neuropsychology, 30, 190–212 Crossref.Google Scholar
Williams, J. N.
(2011) Working memory and SLA. In S. M. Gass & A. Mackey (Eds.), The handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 427–441). New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Xue, G., Chen, C., Jin, Z., & Dong, Q.
(2006) Language experience shapes fusiform activation when processing a logographic artificial language: An fMRI training study. NeuroImage, 31, 1315–1326. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by other publications

LEE, HANSOL, MARK WARSCHAUER & JANG HO LEE
2020. Toward the Establishment of a Data‐Driven Learning Model: Role of Learner Factors in Corpus‐Based Second Language Vocabulary Learning. The Modern Language Journal 104:2  pp. 345 ff. Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 28 november 2020. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.