Part of
Bilingualism: A framework for understanding the mental lexicon
Edited by Maya Libben, Mira Goral and Gary Libben
[Bilingual Processing and Acquisition 6] 2017
► pp. 4972
References (79)
References
Altarriba, J., & Mathis, K. M. (1997). Conceptual and lexical development in second language acquisition. Journal of Memory and Language, 36, 550–568. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barcroft, J. (2002). Semantic and structural elaboration in L2 lexical acquisition. Language Learning, 52(2), 323–363. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barcroft, J., & Sunderman, G. (2008). Learning new words for objects and nonobjects: Theoretical and methodological implications. The Mental Lexicon, 3(3), 325–348. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barnes, J. M., & Underwood, B. J. (1959). "Fate" of first-list associations in transfer theory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58(2), 97–105. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bentin, S., McCarthy, G., & Wood, C. C. (1985). Event-related potentials, lexical decision and semantic priming. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 60(4), 343–355. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bergelson, E., & Swingley, D. (2013). The acquisition of abstract words by young infants. Cognition, 127, 391–397. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brenders, P., Van Hell, J. G., & Dijkstra, T. (2011). Word recognition in child second language learners: Evidence from cognates and false friends. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109, 383–396. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brysbaert, M., & Duyck, W. (2010). Is it time to leave behind the Revised Hierarchical Model of bilingual language processing after fifteen years of service? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(3), 359–371. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Calkins, M. W. (1894). Association. Psychological Review, 1, 476–483. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Comesaña, M., Perea, M., Piñeiro, A., & Fraga, I. (2009). Vocabulary teaching strategies and conceptual representations of words in L2 in children: Evidence with novice learners. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104(1), 22–33. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Comesaña, M., Soares, A. P., Sánchez-Casas, R., & Lima, C. (2012). Lexical and semantic representations in the acquisition of L2 cognate and non-cognate words: Evidence from two learning methods in children. British Journal of Psychology, 103(3), 378–392. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Davis, C., Sánchez-Casas, R., García-Albea, J. E., Guasch, M., Molero, M., & Ferré, P. (2010). Masked translation priming: Varying language experience and word type with Spanish-English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(2), 137–155. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Groot, A. M. B. (2006). Effects of stimulus characteristics and background music on foreign language vocabulary learning and forgetting. Language Learning, 56(3), 463–506. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Groot, A. M. B., & Keijzer, R. (2000). What is hard to learn is easy to forget: The roles of word concreteness, cognate status, and word frequency in foreign-language vocabulary learning and forgetting. Language Learning, 50(1), 1–56. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Groot, A. M. B., & Poot, R. (1997). Word translation at three levels of proficiency in a second language: The ubiquitous involvement of conceptual memory. Language Learning, 47(2), 215–264. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dijkstra, T. (2005). Bilingual visual word recognition and lexical access. In J. F. Kroll & A. M. B. De Groot (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches (pp. 178–201). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dijkstra, T., Haga, F., Bijsterveld, A., & Sprinkhuizen-Kuyper, I. (2012). Lexical competition in localist and distributed connectionist models of L2 acquisition. In J. Altarriba & L. Isurin (Eds.), Memory, language, and bilingualism: Theoretical and applied approaches (pp. 48–73). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dijkstra, T., Miwa, K., Brummelhuis, B., Sappelli, M., & Baayen, H. (2010). How cross-language similarity and task demands affect cognate recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 62(3), 284–301. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dijkstra, T., & Rekké, S. (2012). Towards a localist-connectionist model of word translation. In G. Libben, G. Jarema, & C. F. Westbury (Eds.), Methodological and analytic frontiers in lexical research (pp. 135–154). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dijkstra, T., & Van Heuven, W. J. B. (2002). The architecture of the bilingual word recognition system: From identification to decision. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 5(3), 175–197. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dobel, C., Junghöfer, M., Breitenstein, C., Klauke, B., Knecht, S., Pantev, C., & Zwitserlood, P. (2009). New names for known things: On the association of novel word forms with existing semantic information. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(6), 1251–1261. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Duyck, W., & Brysbaert, M. (2004). Forward and backward number translation requires conceptual mediation in both balanced and unbalanced bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30(5), 889–906. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eilola, T. M., & Havelka, J. (2010). Behavioural and physiological responses to the emotional and taboo Stroop tasks in native and non-native speakers of English. International Journal of Bilingualism, 15, 353–369. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Elgort, I., & Piasecki, A. E. (2014). The effect of a bilingual learning mode on the establishment of lexical semantic representations in the L2. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(3), 572–588. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C., & Beaton, A. (1993). Psycholinguistic determinants of foreign language vocabulary learning. Language Learning, 43(4), 559–617. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Erten, I. H., & Tekin, M. (2008). Effects on vocabulary acquisition of presenting new words in semantic sets versus semantically unrelated sets. System, 36, 407–422. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ferré, P., Sánchez-Casas, R., & Guasch, M. (2006). Can a horse be a donkey? Semantic and form interference effects in translation recognition in early and late proficient and nonproficient Spanish-Catalan bilinguals. Language Learning, 56(4), 571–608. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Finkbeiner, M., & Nicol, J. (2003). Semantic category effects in second language word learning. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24(3), 369–383. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Geukes, S., Gaskell, M. G., & Zwitserlood, P. (2015). Stroop effects from newly learned color words: Effects of memory consolidation and episodic context. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(278), 1–16. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ghazi Saidi, L. (2012). Cross-linguistic transfer (CLT) in bilingual speakers: Neural correlates of language learning. Unpublished PhD dissertation, Université de Montréal.Google Scholar
Grainger, J., & Holcomb, P. J. (2009). Watching the word go by: On the time-course of component processes in visual word recognition. Language and Linguistics Compass, 3(1), 128–156. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grainger, J., Midgley, K., & Holcomb, P. J. (2010). Re-thinking the bilingual interactive-activation model from a developmental perspective. In M. Kail & M. Hickmann (Eds.), Language acquisition across linguistic and cognitive systems (pp. 267–283). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grant, A. M., Fang, S.-Y., & Li, P. (2015). Second language lexical development and cognitive control: A longitudinal fMRI study. Brain and Language, 144, 35–47. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Guo, T., Misra, M., Tam, J. W., & Kroll, J. F. (2012). On the time course of accessing meaning in a second language: An electrophysiological and behavioral investigation of translation recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(5), 1165–1186. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hall, C. J. (2002). The automatic cognate form assumption: Evidence for the parasitic model of vocabulary development. International Review of Applied Linguistics, 12, 185–192. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Holcomb, P. J. (1988). Automatic and attentional processing: An event-related brain potential analysis of semantic priming. Brain and Language, 35(1), 66–85. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Holcomb, P. J., & Grainger, J. (2006). On the time course of visual word recognition: An event-related potential investigation using masked repetition priming. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(10), 1631–1643. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Holcomb, P. J., & Neville, H. J. (1990). Auditory and visual semantic priming in lexical decision: A comparison using event-related brain potentials. Language and Cognitive Processes, 5(4), 281–312. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jeong, H., Sugiura, M., Sassa, Y., Wakusawa, K., Horie, K., Sato, S., & Kawashima, R. (2010). Learning second language vocabulary: Neural dissociation of situation-based learning and text-based learning. NeuroImage, 50, 802–809. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jiang, N. (2000). Lexical representation and development in a second language. Applied Linguistics, 21(1), 47–77. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaushanskaya, M., & Rechtzigel, K. (2012). Concreteness effects in bilingual and monolingual word learning. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 19, 935–941. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaushanskaya, M., Yoo, J., & Van Hecke, S. (2013). Word learning in adults with second-language experience: Effects of phonological and referent familiarity. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56(2), 667–678. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F., & Stewart, E. (1994). Category interference in translation and picture naming: Evidence for asymmetric connection between bilingual memory representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 33(2), 149–174. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F., & Tokowicz, N. (2005). Models of bilingual representation and processing: Looking back and to the future. In J. F. Kroll & A. M. B. De Groot (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches (pp. 531–553). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kroll, J. F., Van Hell, J. G., Tokowicz, N., & Green, D. W. (2010). The Revised Hierarchical Model: A critical review and assessment. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(3), 373–381. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2000). Electrophysiology reveals semantic memory use in language comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(12), 463–470. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1989). An electrophysiological probe of incidental semantic association. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1(1), 38–49. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
La Heij, W., Hooglander, A., Kerling, R., & Van Der Velden, E. (1996). Nonverbal context effects in forward and backward word translation: Evidence for concept mediation. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 648–665. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lau, E. F., Phillips, C., & Poeppel, D. (2008). A cortical network for semantics: [De]constructing the N400. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(12), 920–933. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lotto, L., & De Groot, A. M. B. (1998). Effects of learning method and word type on acquiring vocabulary in an unfamiliar language. Language Learning, 48(1), 31–69. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ma, F., Chen, P., Guo, T., & Kroll, J. F. (2017). When late second language learners access the meaning of L2 words: Using ERPs to investigate the role of the L1 translation equivalent. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 41, 50–69. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Macedonia, M., Müller, K., & Friederici, A. D. (2010). Neural correlates of high performance in foreign language vocabulary learning. Mind, Brain, and Education, 4(3), 125–134. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Magnuson, J. S., Tanenhaus, M. K., Aslin, R. N., & Dahan, D. (2003). The time course of spoken word learning and recognition: Studies with artifical lexicons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132(2), 202–227. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meade, G., Midgley, K. J., Dijkstra, T., & Holcomb, P. J. (in press). Cross-language neighborhood effects in learners indicative of an integrated lexicon. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. DOI logo
Mestres-Missé, A., Münte, T. F., & Rodríguez-Fornells, A. (2009). Functional neuroanatomy of contextual acquisition of concrete and abstract words. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(11), 2154–2171. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Midgley, K. J., Holcomb, P. J., & Grainger, J. (2009). Masked repetition and translation priming in second language learners: A window on the time course of form and meaning activation using ERPs. Psychophysiology, 46(3), 551–565. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nation, P. (2000). Learning vocabulary in lexical sets: Dangers and guidelines. TESOL Journal, 9, 6–10. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Neely, J. H. (1976). Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Evidence for facilitatory and inhibitory processes. Memory & Cognition, 4(5), 648–654. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (1991). Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: A selective review of current findings and theories. In D. Besner & G. Humphreys (Eds.), Basic processes in reading: Visual word recognition (pp. 264–336). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Palmer, S. D., & Havelka, J. (2010). Age of acquisition effects in vocabulary learning. Acta Psychologica, 135, 310–315. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paradis, M. (1981). Neurolinguistic organization of a bilingual’s two languages. In J. Copeland (Ed.), The seventh LACUS forum. Columbia: Hornbeam Press.Google Scholar
Raboyeau, G., Marcotte, K., Adrover-Roig, D., & Ansaldo, A. I. (2010). Brain activation and lexical learning: The impact of learning phase and word type. NeuroImage, 49(3), 2850–2861. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schulpen, B., Dijkstra, T., Schriefers, H., & Hasper, M. (2003). Recognition of interlingual homophones in bilingual auditory word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29(6), 1155–1178. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sholl, A., Sankaranarayanan, A., & Kroll, J. F. (1995). Transfer between picture naming and translation: A test of asymmetries in bilingual memory. Psychological Science, 6(1), 45–49. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sianipar, A., Middelburg, R., & Dijkstra, T. (2015). When feelings arise with meanings: How emotion and meaning of a native language affect second language processing in adult learners. PLoS ONE, 10(12), e0144576. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Slobin, D. (1996). From “thought to language” to “thinking for speaking”. In J. Gumperz & S. Levinson (Eds.), Rethinking linguistic relativity, pp. 70–96. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sunderman, G., & Kroll, J. F. (2006). First language activation during second language lexical processing: An investigation of lexical form, meaning, and grammatical class. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 28, 387–422. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sutton, T. M., Altarriba, J., Gianico, J. L., & Basnight-Brown, D. M. (2007). The automatic access of emotion: Emotional Stroop effects in Spanish-English bilingual speakers. Cognition and Emotion, 21(5), 1077–1090. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Talamas, A., Kroll, J. F., & Dufour, R. (1999). From form to meaning: Stages in the acquisition of second-language vocabulary. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 2(1), 45–58. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thierry, G., & Wu, Y. J. (2007). Brain potentials reveal unconscious translation during foreign-language comprehension. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(30), 12530–12535. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thompson, W. L., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2000). Neural systems activated during visual mental imagery: A review and meta-analyses. In A. W. Toga & J. C. Mazziotta (Eds.), Brain mapping: The systems (pp. 535–560). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tinkham, T. (1993). The effect of semantic clustering on the learning of second language vocabulary. System, 21(3), 371–380. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (1997). The effects of semantic and thematic clustering on the learning of second language vocabulary. Second Language Research, 13(2), 138–163. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tonzar, C., Lotto, L., & Job, R. (2009). L2 vocabulary acquisition in children: Effects of learning method and cognate status. Language Learning, 59(3), 623–646. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Hell, J. G., & De Groot, A. M. B. (1998a). Conceptual representation in bilingual memory: Effects of concreteness and cognate status in word association. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1(3), 193–211. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (1998b). Disentangling context availability and concreteness in lexical decision and word translation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51(1), 41–63. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Verspoor, M., & Schmitt, N. (2013). Language and the lexicon in SLA. In P. Robinson (Ed.), Routledge Encyclopedia of SLA (pp. 353–360). New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Vitevitch, M. S. (2012). What do foreign neighbors say about the mental lexicon? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15(1), 167–172. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Waring, R. (1997). The negative effects of learning words in semantic sets: A replication. System, 25(2), 261–274. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (5)

Cited by five other publications

Piasecki, Anna & Ton Dijkstra
2023. Chapter 6. Cross-language influences in L2 pre-lexical and lexical processing and acquisition. In Cross-language Influences in Bilingual Processing and Second Language Acquisition [Bilingual Processing and Acquisition, 16],  pp. 126 ff. DOI logo
Baxter, Peta, Harold Bekkering, Ton Dijkstra, Mienke Droop, Marianne van den Hurk & Frank Léoné
2021. Grounding Second Language Vocabulary Instruction in Cognitive Science. Mind, Brain, and Education 15:1  pp. 24 ff. DOI logo
Boddaert, G., C. Cornut & S. Casalis
2021. Integration of newly learned L2 words into the mental lexicon is modulated by vocabulary learning method. Acta Psychologica 212  pp. 103220 ff. DOI logo
Tang, Rongchao, Naoko Witzel & Xiaomei Qiao
2021. Masked form priming with newly-learned pseudo-L2 words in novice bilinguals. Journal of Second Language Studies 4:2  pp. 293 ff. DOI logo
Meade, Gabriela, Katherine J. Midgley & Phillip J. Holcomb
2018. An ERP Investigation of L2–L1 Translation Priming in Adult Learners. Frontiers in Psychology 9 DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 27 july 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.