Part of
Bilingualism: A framework for understanding the mental lexicon
Edited by Maya Libben, Mira Goral and Gary Libben
[Bilingual Processing and Acquisition 6] 2017
► pp. 123142
References
Abutalebi, J., Della Rosa, P. A., Ding, G., Weekes, B., Costa, A., & Green, D. W
(2013) Language proficiency modulates the engagement of cognitive control areas in multilinguals. Cortex, 49(3), 905–911. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Abutalebi, J., & Green, D
(2007) Bilingual language production: The neurocognition of language representation and control. Journal of neurolinguistics, 20(3), 242–275. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baus, C., Branzi, F. M., & Costa, A
(2015) On the mechanism and scope of language control on bilingual speech production. In J. W. Schwieter (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of bilingual processing (pp. 508–526). Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Berkes
in preparation). The influence of cultural context on language activation in Korean-English bilinguals. DOI logo
Bialystok, E
(2009) Bilingualism: The good, the bad, and the indifferent. Bilingualism: Language and cognition, 12(1), 3–11. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F., & Luk, G
(2008a) Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, memory, and cognition, 34(4), 859–873. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., & Luk, G
(2008b) Lexical access in bilinguals: Effects of vocabulary size and executive control. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 21(6), 522–538. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bobb, S. C., & Wodniecka, Z
(2013) Language switching in picture naming: What asymmetric switch costs (do not) tell us about inhibition in bilingual speech planning. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 568–585. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bonnin, C. A., Gaonac'h, D., & Bouquet, C. A
(2011) Adjustments of task-set control processes: Effect of task switch frequency on task-mixing and task-switching costs. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 23(8), 985–997. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Branzi, F., Martin, C., Abutalebi, J., & Costa, A
(2014) The after-effects of bilingual language production. Neuropsychologia, 52, 102–116. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Calabria, M., Branzi, F. M., Marne, P., Hernandez, M., & Costa, A
(2015) Age-related effects over bilingual language control and executive control. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(1), 65–78. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Christoffels, I. K., Firk, C., & Schiller, N. O
(2007) Bilingual language control: An event-related brain potential study. Brain research, 1147, 192–208. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clyne, M
(1972) Perspectives on language contact. Melbourne: Hawthorn Press.Google Scholar
Colome, A
(2001) Lexical activation in bilinguals’ speech production: Language-specific or language independent? Journal of Memory and Language, 45, 721–736. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Miozzo, M., & Caramazza, A
(1999) Lexical selection in bilinguals: Do words in the bilingual’s two lexicons compete for selection? Journal of Memory and Language, 41, 365–397. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., & Santesteban, M
(2004) Lexical access in bilingual speech production: Evidence from language switching in highly proficient bilinguals and L2 learners. Journal of Memory and Language, 50(4), 491–511. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Santesteban, M., & Ivanova, I
(2006) How do highly proficient bilinguals control their lexicalization process? Inhibitory and language-specific selection mechanisms are both functional. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32(5), 1057–1074. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Declerck, M., Koch, I., & Philipp, A. M
(2015) The minimum requirements of language control: Evidence from sequential predictability effects in language switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41, 377–394. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Declerck, M., & Philipp, A. M
(2015) A review of control processes and their locus in language switching. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1–16.Google Scholar
Declerck, M., Philipp, A. M., & Koch, I
(2013) Bilingual control: Sequential memory in language switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39(6), 1793. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Declerck, M., Thoma, A. M., Koch, I., & Philipp, A. M
(2015) Highly proficient bilinguals implement inhibition: Evidence from n-2 language repetition costs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41(2), 377–394. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Groot, A., & Christoffels, I. K
(2006) Language control in bilinguals: Monolingual tasks and simultaneous interpreting. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 9(2), 189–201. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dell, G. S
(1988) The retrieval of phonological forms in production: Tests of predictions from a connectionist model. Journal of Memory and Language, 27(2), 124–142. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dijkstra, A., & Van Heuven, W. J. B
(1998) The BIA model and bilingual word recognition. In J. Grainger & A. M. Jacobs (Eds.), Localist connectionist approaches to human cognition (pp. 189–225). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Dijkstra, T., & Van Heuven, W. J
(2002) The architecture of the bilingual word recognition system: From identification to decision. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 5(3), 175–197.Google Scholar
Finkbeiner, M., Almeida, J., Janssen, N., & Caramazza, A
(2006) Lexical selection in bilingual speech production does not involve language suppression. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32(5), 1075. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Finkbeiner, M., Gollan, T. H., & Caramazza, A
(2006) Lexical access in bilingual speakers: What’s the (hard) problem? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 9(2), 153–166. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Garbin, G., Sanjuan, A., Forn, C., Bustamante, J. C., Rodríguez-Pujadas, A., Belloch, V., & Ávila, C
(2010) Bridging language and attention: Brain basis of the impact of bilingualism on cognitive control. NeuroImage, 53(4), 1272–1278. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T., & Ferreira, V
(2009) Should I stay or should I switch? A cost-benefit analysis of voluntary language switching in young and aging bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 640–665. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., & Bonanni, M. P
(2005) Proper names get stuck on bilingual and monolingual speakers' tip of the tongue equally often. Neuropsychology, 19(3), 278–287. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., Cera, C., & Sandoval, T. C
(2008) More use almost always means a smaller frequency effect: Aging, bilingualism, and the weaker links hypothesis. Journal of Memory and Language, 58(3), 787–814. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., Fennema-Notestine, C., & Morris, S. K
(2005) Bilingualism affects picture naming but not picture classification. Memory & Cognition, 33, 1220–1234. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Sandoval, T., & Salmon, D. P
(2011) Cross-language intrusion errors in aging bilinguals reveal the link between executive control and language selection. Psychological Science, 22, 1155–1164. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grainger, J., Midgley, K. J., & Holcomb, P. J
(2010) Re-thinking the bilingual interactive-activation model from a developmental perspective (BIA-d). In M. Kail & M. Hickman (Eds.), Language acquisition across linguistic and cognitive systems (pp. 267–284). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Green, D. W
(1998) Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and cognition, 1(2), 67–81. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Green, D. W., & Abutalebi, J
(2013) Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 515–530. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grosjean, F
(2001) The bilingual’s language modes. In J. Nicol (Ed.), One mind, two languages: Bilingual language processing (pp. 1–22). Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
(2008) Studying bilinguals. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Guo, T., Liu, H., Misra, M., & Kroll, J. F
(2011) Local and global inhibition in bilingual word production: fMRI evidence from Chinese–English bilinguals. NeuroImage, 56(4), 2300–2309. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hanulová, J., Davidson, D. J., & Indefrey, P
(2011) Where does the delay in L2 picture naming come from? Psycholinguistic and neurocognitive evidence on second language word production. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26(7), 902–934. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hasselmo, Nils
1970Code-switching and modes of speaking. In G. Gilbert (ed.), Texas studies in bilingualism (pp. 179–210). Berlin: Walter de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hermans, D., Bongaerts, T., De Bot, K., & Schreuder, R
(1998) Producing words in a foreign language: Can speakers prevent interference from their first language? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, 213–229. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hernandez, A. E
(2009) Language switching in the bilingual brain: What’s next? Brain and Language, 109(2), 133–140. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hernandez, A. E., & Kohnert, K. J
(1999) Aging and language switching in bilinguals. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 6(2), 69–83. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jackson, G. M., Swainson, R., Cunnington, R., & Jackson, S. R
(2001) ERP correlates of executive control during repeated language switching. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 4(2), 169–178. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kong, A. P. H., Abutalebi, J., Lam, K. S. Y., & Weekes, B
(2014) Executive and language control in the multilingual brain. Behavioural Neurology. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F., Bobb, S. C., Misra, M., & Guo, T
(2008) Language selection in bilingual speech: Evidence for inhibitory processes. Acta Psychologica, 128(3), 416–430. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F., Bobb, S. C., & Wodniecka, Z
(2006) Language selectivity is the exception, not the rule: Arguments against a fixed locus of language selection in bilingual speech. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 9(2), 119–135. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F., & Gollan, M
(2014) Speech planning in two languages: What bilinguals tell us about language production. In V. S. Ferreira & M. Goldrick (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of language production (pp. 165–181). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
La Heij, W
(2005) Selection processes in monolingual and bilingual lexical access. In J. F. Kroll & A. M. B. De Groot (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches (pp. 289–307). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Levelt, W. J., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S
(1999) A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(1), 1–38. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Li, Y., Yang, J., Scherf, K. S., & Li, P
(2013) Two faces, two languages: An fMRI study of bilingual picture naming. Brain and Language, 127(3), 452–462. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Linck, J. A., Schwieter, J. W., & Sunderman, G
(2012) Inhibitory control predicts language switching performance in trilingual speech production. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15(3), 651–662. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Martin, C. D., Dering, B., Thomas, E. M., & Thierry, G
(2009) Brain potentials reveal semantic priming in both the ‘active’ and the ‘non-attended’ language of early bilinguals. NeuroImage, 47(1), 326–333. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meiran, N
(2010) Task switching: Mechanisms underlying rigid vs. flexible self control. In R. R. Hassin, K. N. Ochsner, & Y. Trope (Eds.), Self Control in Society, Mind and Brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meuter, R. F., & Allport, A
(1999) Bilingual language switching in naming: Asymmetrical costs of language selection. Journal of Memory and Language, 40(1), 25–40. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Michael E. B., & Gollan, T. H
(2005) Being and becoming bilingual: Individual differences and consequences for language production. In J. F. Kroll & A. M. B. De Groot (Eds.). Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches (pp. 389–407). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Misra, M., Guo, T., Bobb, S. C., & Kroll, J. F
(2012) When bilinguals choose a single word to speak: Electrophysiological evidence for inhibition of the native language. Journal of memory and language, 67(1), 224–237. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Molnar, M., Ibáñez-Molina, A., & Carreiras, M
(2015) Interlocutor identity affects language activation in bilinguals. Journal of Memory and Language, 81, 91–104. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Olson, D. J
(2012) The phonetics of insertional code-switching: Suprasegmental analysis and a case for hyper-articulation. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 2(4), 439–457. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015) The gradient effect of context on language switching and lexical access in bilingual production. Applied Psycholinguistics, 37(3), 725–756. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Philipp, A. M., Gade, M., & Koch, I
(2007) Inhibitory processes in language switching: Evidence from switching language-defined response sets. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19(3), 395–416. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Philipp, A. M., & Koch, I
(2009) Inhibition in language switching: What is inhibited when switching between languages in naming tasks? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(5), 1187. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Poulisse, N., & Bongaerts, T
(1994) First language use in second language production. Applied Linguistics, 15, 36–57. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Prior, A., & Gollan, T. H
(2011) Good language-switchers are good task-switchers: Evidence from Spanish–English and Mandarin–English bilinguals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17(4), 682–691. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rodríguez-Fornells, A., Rotte, M., Heinze, H. J., Nösselt, T., & Münte, T. F
(2002) Brain potential and functional MRI evidence for how to handle two languages with one brain. Nature, 415, 1026–1029. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rodríguez-Fornells, A., Van der Lugt, A., Rotte, M., Britti, B., Heinze, H. J., & Münte, T. F
(2005) Second language interferes with word production in fluent bilinguals: Brain potential and functional imaging evidence. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 422–433. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rodríguez-Fornells, A., De Diego Balaguer, R., & Münte, T. F
(2006) Executive control in bilingual language processing. Language Learning, 56, 133–190. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Runnqvist, E., Strijkers, K., Sadat, J., & Costa, A
(2011) On the temporal and functional origin of L2 disadvantages in speech production: A critical review. Frontiers in Psychology, 2(379), 1–8. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schwieter, J. W., & Sunderman, G
Soares, C., & Grosjean, F
(1984) Bilinguals in a monolingual and a bilingual speech mode: The effect on lexical access. Memory & Cognition, 12(4), 380–386. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Spivey, M. J., & Marian, V
(1999) Cross talk between native and second languages: Partial activation of an irrelevant lexicon. Psychological Science, 10(3), 281–284. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Starreveld, P. A., & La Heij, W
(1996) Time-course analysis of semantic and orthographic context effects in picture naming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 896–918. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Timmer, K., Christoffels I. K., & Costa, A
under review). On the flexibility of bilingual language control: The effect of language context.
Timmer, K., Ganushchak, L. Y., Ceusters, I., & Schiller, N. O
(2014) Second language phonology influences first language word naming. Brain and Language, 133, 14–25. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Assche, E., Duyck, W., & Gollan, T
(2013) Whole-language and item-specific control in bilingual language production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 39(6), 1781–1792. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Heuven, W. J., & Dijkstra, T
(2010) Language comprehension in the bilingual brain: fMRI and ERP support for psycholinguistic models. Brain Research Reviews, 64(1), 104–122. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Heuven, W. J. B., Schriefers, H., Dijkstra, T., & Hagoort, P
(2008) Language conflict in the bilingual brain. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 2706–2716. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Verhoef, K., Roelofs, A., & Chwilla, D. J
(2009) Role of inhibition in language switching: Evidence from event-related brain potentials in overt picture naming. Cognition, 110(1), 84–99. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Verhoef, K. M., Roelofs, A., & Chwilla, D. J
(2010) Electrophysiological evidence for endogenous control of attention in switching between languages in overt picture naming. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(8), 1832–1843. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wang, Y., Kuhl, P. K., Chen, C., & Dong, Q
(2009) Sustained and transient language control in the bilingual brain. NeuroImage, 47(1), 414–422. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Woumans, E., Martin, C., Vanden Bulcke, C., Van Assche, E., Costa, A., Hartsuiker, R., & Duyck, W
in press). Can faces prime a language? Psychological Science.
Wu, Y. J., & Thierry, G
(2010) Chinese–English bilinguals reading English hear Chinese. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30(22), 7646–7651. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2013) Fast modulation of executive function by language context in bilinguals. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33(33), 13533–13537. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zhang, S., Morris, M. W., Cheng, C. Y., & Yap, A. J
(2013) Heritage-culture images disrupt immigrants’ second-language processing through triggering first-language interference. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, direct submission.
Cited by

Cited by 5 other publications

Liu, Cong, Kalinka Timmer, Lu Jiao, Yuan Yuan & Ruiming Wang
2019. The influence of contextual faces on bilingual language control. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 72:9  pp. 2313 ff. DOI logo
Liu, Cong, Kalinka Timmer, Lu Jiao & Ruiming Wang
2020. Symmetries of comprehension-based language switch costs in conflicting versus non-conflicting contexts. International Journal of Bilingualism 24:4  pp. 588 ff. DOI logo
Skoe, Erika, Adrián García-Sierra, Nairán Ramírez-Esparza & Shu Jiang
2022. Automatic sound encoding is sensitive to language familiarity: Evidence from English monolinguals and Spanish-English bilinguals. Neuroscience Letters 777  pp. 136582 ff. DOI logo
Timmer, Kalinka, Marco Calabria & Albert Costa
2019. Non-linguistic effects of language switching training. Cognition 182  pp. 14 ff. DOI logo
TIMMER, KALINKA, INGRID K. CHRISTOFFELS & ALBERT COSTA
2019. On the flexibility of bilingual language control: The effect of language context. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 22:3  pp. 555 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 22 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.